Abstract
The goal of this study was to determine the neural correlates of successful doctor–patient interactions. We performed an experimental neuroimaging study where medical doctors (MDs) performed a treatment task while their brain activation pattern was measured, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). MDs (25–37 years old) first performed a standardized clinical exam of a “professional patient”. Unbeknownst to the doctors, the professional patient was a confederate that rated the doctors’ clinical examination using the Consultation And Relational Empathy (CARE) questionnaire, a standardized protocol assessing a clinician’s social interaction during a consultation. After the clinical exam, MDs were placed inside a brain scanner and the patient was placed on a chair next to the MD. MDs performed a treatment task where an analgesic device was used to alleviate the patient’s pain (experimentally induced), while the MD’s brain activity was measured with fMRI. MDs rated their own empathic concern (equivalent of compassion) and personal distress using the Interpersonal Reactivity Index questionnaire. The patient’s rating of CARE was robustly related to the MD’s own ratings of trait empathic concern and to compassion-related and reward-related activation of medial frontal brain regions during treatment. In contrast, there was no relation with MD’s personal distress, nor with activation in regions associated with the aversive component of experiencing empathy. We conclude that a patient’s positive experience of a medical examination is reflected in doctors’ empathic concern and reward-related brain activations during treatment, suggesting that compassion and pleasure are key factors for successful doctor–patient interactions.
Similar content being viewed by others
Explore related subjects
Discover the latest articles, news and stories from top researchers in related subjects.Introduction
Physicians treating patients are potentially exposed to two opposite psychological processes: one positive feeling related to the experience of helping someone in need, and, on the other hand, the aversive experience related to witnessing someone’s suffering or frustration at the inability to help. The ability to share the feelings of others is often referred to as empathy, and the ability to care for and show concern for others is the core aspect of compassion1,2,3.
The affective state of the professional health worker is of importance for improvement of medical treatments since the clinical dyad between the patient and the clinician has direct consequences for treatment outcomes4,5. While clinicians’ ability to experience empathy in general has been suggested to be an important factor for successful treatment6, it has also been suggested that some aspects of empathy may be maladaptive since it might result in aversive responses, including personal distress in the clinician1,6. In fact, affective burnout is a major cause for sick-leave in doctors7. Researchers have suggested that compassion processes, involving the brain’s reward circuitry, may be a strategy for interacting with patients1. While the distressful component of empathy (often denoted as personal distress8) and compassion may reflect different affective states of the clinician (and thereby shape the interaction with the patient), there is little empirical evidence as to which strategy is more critical and leads to better outcomes for the patient. Empathic concern8 is another term used interchangeably for compassion or sympathy, but it should not be confounded with empathy in the sense of negative affect sharing2,3. While empathic concern, sympathy and compassion represent “other-oriented” responses to someone suffering, the personal distress component of empathy is a “self-oriented” response9.
Distinct neural correlates associated with personal distress and compassion have been demonstrated in various paradigms when viewing individuals in pain1,10,11,12,13,14, including physicians while they treat15. Thus, it is possible to explore if and how these two separate processes are active during patient–physician interactions. In an experimental setting, we studied whether a person’s rating of a medical exam was related to personal distress or compassion (empathic concern) in the treating physician. This novel analysis was based on our previous study where medical doctors underwent brain scanning during a treatment task15. The results presented here are exploratory analyses in addition to the primary results in our previous paper.
Materials and methods
In the present study 18 physicians (aged 25–37 years; ten female) participated after written consent was given. The physicians performed (what they believed to be real transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation [TENS]) treatment of pain to the arm in one of two professional patients—i.e., confederates getting sham TENS treatment (25-year-old female research assistants, here referred to as “patients”)—while undergoing functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). From the same experiment, we have previously reported activations related to expectations, empathy-processing and reward-processing during treatment15. Here, we focused on physician brain activations during treatment in regions observed in “empathy for pain” and compassion studies1,10,11,12,13,14, and, most importantly, related this to the patient’s experience of the physicians’ medical exam (as an indicator of successful treatment).
The physicians performed an initial clinical examination of the patients (~20 min) before the experiment. This allowed us to measure how the patients experienced the patient–physician interaction using the validated consultation and relational empathy (CARE) questionnaire after the clinical exam16. CARE is a 10-item scale developed to measure the patient’s perception of a clinician’s relational empathy and communication skills during a consultation. The questions assess “other-oriented” behavior of the clinician and reflect empathic concern. We also measured physicians’ empathic concern (EC) and personal distress (PD) of the interpersonal reactivity index (IRI)8. While EC assesses other-oriented feelings of sympathy and concern for unfortunate others (closely related to compassion), PD measures self-oriented feelings of personal anxiety and unease in tense personal settings (related to the distressful aspect of empathy)2,3,8. Finally, we used physicians’ satisfaction scores to measure state satisfaction during the treatment task (Fig. 1). CARE was not reported in the original article15 since it was not related to the main hypothesis. For completeness we also describe which other scales were administered in the original study but not reported: The general scale of self-efficacy (GSSE)17, Spielberger state-trait anxiety inventory18, as well as a question regarding “satisfaction being an MD” (0–100%), and a question asking for “confidence in ability to build a relationship with patients” (0–100%).
Correlations were performed using Spearman’s Rho (two-sided), as our behavioral data was not normally distributed. Further methodological details are available elsewhere15. The fMRI-data was analyzed using Statistical Parametric Modeling 12 (SPM12; Wellcome Trust Center for Neuroimaging, London, UK http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). We were specifically interested in regions either belonging to a network processing empathy for pain12 or regions associated with compassion training1. The empathy for pain related regions included the right anterior insula [rAI] and caudal anterior cingulate cortex [cACC]), whereas the compassion related regions included (1) a region encompassing medial Orbitofrontal cortex [mOfc] and subgenual ACC [sgACC] and (2) a region encompassing rostral ACC [rACC] and ventromedial prefrontal cortex [vmPFC] as well as (3) ventral striatum [VS]. These a priori ROIs were derived from coordinates in Klimecki and Singer1 (for compassion training related regions) and Lamm et al.12 (for empathy for pain related regions) using 5 mm sphere radius around peak voxel associated regions. The initial statistical threshold for the ROI-analysis was set to p < 0.01 (uncorrected). A family-wise error (FWE) correction for multiple comparison (p < 0.05) was then performed on voxel and cluster level within the ROIs. The age of the physicians was included as a covariate of no interest in the fMRI analysis.
Ethical approval was received from the Institutional Review Board of the Massachusetts General Hospital and MDs signed an informed consent. After completing the study, MDs were fully debriefed about the confederate “patient”, and the sham device, and offered to withdraw their data. None withdrew their data.
Results
Behavioral results
CARE scores showed a moderate and positive correlation with physicians’ self-reported EC (rs = 0.595, p = 0.009; Fig. 1). While CARE did not significantly correlate with the degree of treatment satisfaction expressed by the physicians (rs = 0.141, p = 0.602), EC was significantly correlated with treatment satisfaction (change in physician satisfaction after treatment versus no-treatment; rs = 0.707, p = 0.002). In contrast, CARE showed no correlation with physician’s PD ratings (rs = −0.058, p = 0.819), and PD did not correlate with treatment satisfaction (rs = −0.080, p = 0.769).
Neuroimaging results
Using a regression analysis of neural activity during the treatment condition, we probed brain regions with neural activity associated with CARE scores, specifically in networks related to empathy for pain and compassion. The regression analysis revealed significant relations between higher CARE scores and activity during treatment in the mOFC/sgACC (x = 9, y = 41, z = −17; t-value = 3.76; FWE-corrected p-value (voxel-level) = 0.013; FWE-corrected p-value (cluster-level) = 0.046) and rACC/vmPFC (x = −15, y = 38, z = 10; t-value = 3.64; FWE-corrected p-value (voxel-level) = 0.015; FWE-corrected p-value (cluster-level) = 0.029)) (Fig. 2).
Discussion
The neural correlates of positive patient-clinician interactions are largely unknown. Here, we report novel findings from a unique neuroimaging study, where physician’s brains were scanned during face-to-face interaction with a patient15. In addition to the primary results from our previous publication15, results suggest that a patient’s subjective experience of a physician (CARE rating) after a medical examination was closely linked both to physician empathic concern (assessed by EC subscale of IRI) and treatment-related activations of brain areas associated with reward and compassion in the medial prefrontal cortex (including mOFC/sgACC and rACC/vmPFC)1. However, CARE was not associated with distressful empathy responses related to the mirroring of negative feelings of others or activations in the empathy for pain network (anterior insula and caudal anterior cortex). Our results therefore suggest that compassion and reward processing is more consistently engaged than distressful empathy in successful patient–physician interactions.
The relationship between a patient and treating clinician is often described as something intangible and out of reach of systematic scientific investigation. Yet, recent advances in cognitive and social neuroscience have provided a research avenue, where the autonomic and neural processes associated with successful social communication and different aspects of empathy can be determined6. Previously, behavioral studies have suggested a shared physiology between patients and physicians, via synchronized autonomic responses during consultations19. Also, such measurements suggest that patients’ ratings of physician empathy correlate to degree of autonomic synchronization20, indicating that objective markers may be helpful in training and improvement of relational empathy in clinical practice. Here, we were able to disentangle physician personal distress and empathic concern and elucidate how they relate to patients’ subjective experience as captured by quantitative ratings, combined with analyses of physician brain activation patterns.
The IRI instrument8 was used to estimate physicians’ self-rated interpersonal sensitivity on two distinct components. The EC dimension reflects tender feelings to others in need, and is considered to be equivalent of compassion2,3, whereas the PD dimension (personal distress) reflects distressful components of empathy and one’s own anxiety and feelings of unease when others are in need of help. The important distinction is whether the interaction evokes mainly emotionally positive or distressful reactions in the physician. Data from the physicians in our study indicate that the positive component is central for successful clinical interactions.
Both insula and caudal ACC are core components in processing of empathy for pain12. Insula has been linked to feeling states based on interoceptive processing21 and often overlapping activation is observed for pain and empathy for pain processes, although the empathy processing is generally more anterior12 and includes some unique activation patterns22. In the first reports from this dataset, we observed activations in empathy for pain related regions such as the anterior insula during a no treatment condition, where physicians were not able to provide analgesia to their patient15. The fact that there was no detectable link between CARE and insular activation during the treatment condition suggests that the negative affect component of empathy reflected in the insula may not be a major factor for successful patient–physician interactions.
Similarly to the insula, caudal ACC may also be activated during evoked pain and processing of empathy for pain. During pain, the caudal ACC has been associated with motor activity, such as avoidance behavior23. We did not observe any activation of the caudal ACC during treatment (treatment vs. no treatment)15. In fact, lower activations in empathy for pain related regions have been observed in physicians when viewing subjects in pain, as compared to non-physicians24. Thus, these studies suggest that physicians suppress aversive empathy-related responses during treatment.
In contrast to the regions processing negative affective components of pain and empathy (discussed above), medial frontal regions (sgACC/mOFC and rACC/vmPFC) were significantly related to CARE scores. These sets of regions have been related to the subjective experience of reward. While rACC/vmPFC has been associated with subjective reward value25 and the added value used in value-based decisions26,27 the mOFC has been linked to hedonic processing28. Thus, we argue that our findings of a positive link between CARE scores and activation in these medial brain regions point to a central role of reward and subjective feelings of pleasantness in patient–physician interactions. In contrast, we did not observe any relation between CARE and ventral striatum, a region involved in processing reward related error responses. Possibly, the present study is inadequately powered to detect such a relation, but alternatively, cortical reward processes may be of more significance in relation to ratings of the patient’s experience of the physician’s social interaction.
Although the present study gives support for compassion related processes being related to successful patient–clinician interactions, the personal distress component of empathy may play a significant role as well. It has been suggested that such distressful processes are important for the motivational drive to help someone in need, and activation of anterior insula when viewing others that suffer may have an important impact on decisions relating to helping others29,30. Thus, such processes may be expected before a decision to treat is taken rather than during the treatment itself.
One limitation of this study is the one-sided neuroimaging of physicians that prevents any analyses of neural synchronization between the patient and physician. Recent advances in neuroimaging techniques have allowed for so-called hyperscanning, where two people can be scanned at the same time in two parallel scanners. Such data will be able to increase our understanding of the dynamics of compassion-related brain activations in patients and clinicians. Yet, our experiment allowed the patient to sit face to face with the physician in the scanner room, creating a physical closeness that will not be feasible when two individuals are placed in two separate scanners. Another limitation is the small sample of physicians used in this study, which means that our results should mainly be seen as the first indication of a novel research line where the neural correlates of successful clinical encounters can be assessed in larger samples. Furthermore, the correlational nature of the study prevents causal conclusions about factors that contribute to successful patient–clinician interactions.
In summary, our results suggest that although the distressful component of empathy may be of importance in the motivational drive to treat, reward processes and compassion are more related to a successful patient–physician interaction. Thus—apart from clinicians’ medical skills and knowledge—compassionate caring for someone may be a determinate of a successful treatment.
References
Singer, T. & Klimecki, O. M. Empathy and compassion. Curr. Biol. 24, R875–R878 (2014).
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D. & Simon-Thomas, E. Compassion: an evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychol. Bull. 136, 351–374 (2010).
Jordan, M. R., Amir, D. & Bloom, P. Are empathy and concern psychologically distinct? Emotion 16, 1107–1116 (2016).
Ardito, R. B. & Rabellino, D. Therapeutic alliance and outcome of psychotherapy: historical excursus, measurements, and prospects for research. Front Psychol. 2, 270 (2011).
Kelley J. M. The influence of the patient-clinician relationship on healthcare outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. PLoS ONE 9, e94207 (2014).
Riess, H. Empathy in medicine—a neurobiological perspective. JAMA 304, 1604–1605 (2010).
Shanafelt, T. D. et al. Burnout and satisfaction with work-life balance among US physicians relative to the general US population. Arch. Intern. Med. 172, 1377–1385 (2012).
Davis, M. H. Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 44, 113–126 (1983).
Singer T. & Lamm C. The social neuroscience of empathy. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1156, 81–96 (2009).
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C. & Singer, T. Functional neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cereb. Cortex 23, 1552–1561 (2013).
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M. & Singer, T. Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 9, 873–879 (2014).
Lamm, C., Decety, J. & Singer, T. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54, 2492–2502 (2011).
Singer, T. et al. Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science 303, 1157–1162 (2004).
Singer, T. et al. Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature 439, 466–469 (2006).
Jensen, K. B. et al. Sharing pain and relief: neural correlates of physicians during treatment of patients. Mol. Psychiatry 19, 392–398 (2014).
Mercer, S. W., Maxwell, M., Heaney, D. & Watt, G. C. The consultation and relational empathy (CARE) measure: development and preliminary validation and reliability of an empathy-based consultation process measure. Fam. Pract. 21, 699–705 (2004).
Schwarzer R. & Jerusalem M. Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. 35–37 (Nfer-Nelson, Windsor) 1995.
Spielberger C. D. Manual For The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y). (Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, CA) 1983.
Glucksman, M. L. Psychological measures and feedback during psychotherapy. Psychother. Psychosom. 36, 185–199 (1981).
Marci, C. D. & Orr, S. P. The effect of emotional distance on psychophysiologic concordance and perceived empathy between patient and interviewer. Appl Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 31, 115–128 (2006).
Craig, A. D. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 59–70 (2009).
Krishnan A. et al. Somatic and vicarious pain are represented by dissociable multivariate brain patterns. Elife 5, e15166 (2016).
Perini, I., Bergstrand, S. & Morrison, I. Where pain meets action in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 33, 15930–15939 (2013).
Decety, J., Yang, C. Y. & Cheng, Y. Physicians down-regulate their pain empathy response: an event-related brain potential study. Neuroimage 50, 1676–1682 (2010).
Petrovic, P. et al. Blocking central opiate function modulates hedonic impact and anterior cingulate response to rewards and losses. J. Neurosci. 28, 10509–10516 (2008).
Kable, J. W. & Glimcher, P. W. The neural correlates of subjective value during intertemporal choice. Nat. Neurosci. 10, 1625–1633 (2007).
Ruff, C. C. & Fehr, E. The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 15, 549–562 (2014).
Berridge, K. C. & Kringelbach, M. L. Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron 86, 646–664 (2015).
Feldmanhall, O., Dalgleish, T. & Mobbs, D. Alexithymia decreases altruism in real social decisions. Cortex 49, 899–904 (2013).
Gospic, K. et al. Altruism costs-the cheap signal from amygdala. Soc. Cogn. Affect Neurosci. 9, 1325–1332 (2014).
Acknowledgements
The work was supported by the Swedish Society for Medical Research (SSMF) and the Swedish Council for Working Life and Social Research to K. Jensen, Swedish Research Council to P. Petrovic, K24 AT004095 (NCCAM) and R01 AT004662 (NCCAM) to T. Kaptchuk, KO1AT003883 (NCCAM), R21AT004497 (NCCAM), R03AT218317 (NIDA), R01AT006364 (NCCAM) to J. Kong, R01AT005280 (NCCAM) to R. Gollub, M01-RR-01066 and UL1 RR025758-01 for Clinical Research Center Biomedical Imaging Core from National Center for Research Resources (NCRR), and P41RR14075 for Center for Functional Neuroimaging Technologies from NCRR. The sponsors had no influence on the study. The authors wish to thank Catherine Kerr for her wisdom and help with the conception of this study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Additional information
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Jensen, K., Gollub, R.L., Kong, J. et al. Reward and empathy in the treating clinician: the neural correlates of successful doctor–patient interactions. Transl Psychiatry 10, 17 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0712-2
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0712-2
- Springer Nature Limited
This article is cited by
-
RETRACTED ARTICLE: Doctors’ and Patients’ Perceptions of Impacts of Doctors’ Communication and Empathy Skills on Doctor–Patient Relationships During COVID-19
Journal of General Internal Medicine (2023)
-
The Effects of Physicians’ Communication and Empathy Ability on Physician–Patient Relationship from Physicians’ and Patients’ Perspectives
Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings (2022)