Introduction

Many psychiatric disorders are highly heritable and are likely to represent the clinical outcome of aberrations in the formation of brain architecture in utero or during early postnatal life.1 Given their strong genetic underpinnings,2 measures of network topology have been implicated as intermediate phenotypes that indicate the genetic risk for neuropsychiatric disorders.3 In recent years, graph-theory-based complex network analysis, which describes important properties of complex systems by quantifying topologies of their respective network representations, has been increasingly used in the study of structural and functional organization of the nervous system.4 By treating the brain as one integrated network, this analysis asks whether the architecture of communication patterns within that network is altered under various conditions and disorders.5

The placement of brain connectivity as an ‘intermediate phenotype’ positioned between genetics and behavior renders it an attractive target for studies that link networks across levels, from molecules to neurons and brain systems.6 Moreover, the emergence of comprehensive data sets on gene expression and brain networks in healthy and affected individuals offers the possibility of addressing new questions regarding the interaction of brain connectivity with genomics and behavior, including its dysregulation, as a possible trigger of brain disorders.7, 8, 9 However, the complexity of genetic makeup, in conjunction with pronounced phenotypic heterogeneity, greatly complicates such systematic attempts.10 Translational studies using inbred or genetically modified mice could help bridge such major gaps.11, 12, 13, 14

From this perspective, the Abelson helper integration site-1 (Ahi1) knockout mouse emerges as a promising model for gaining mechanistic insights into how functional (dis)organization of mature brain networks, accompanied by corresponding behavioral phenotypes, had been established by earlier developmental abnormalities. Ahi1 protein, Jouberin, is highly expressed in mammals throughout the developing brain, with lower expression continuing into adulthood.15 Although conserved in mammals, Jouberin’s accelerated evolution in the human lineage indicates a role in cognitive (dys)function.16 Recent converging data emphasize Ahi1’s extensive developmental role, from essential cellular signaling organelles, such as the primary cilium, to neuronal networks and complex organ systems.17, 18, 19, 20, 21, 22 Previous work by our group discovered significant association of AHI1 with schizophrenia,23, 24 which was later replicated in large European case–control samples.25, 26 In addition, associations of AHI1 with autism and mood disorders have been reported.27, 28 Notably, modest alterations in AHI1 expression have been implicated in both schizophrenia and autism.29, 30

Recently, we performed comprehensive behavioral phenotyping of the heterozygous Ahi1 knockout (Ahi1+/−) mouse, which displayed diminished, rather than absent, Ahi1 expression. Although we had not observed significant between-genotype differences in physical characteristics or gross brain morphology, we presented consistent evidence implicating underexpression of the Ahi1 gene during neurodevelopment in relative resilience to various stressors during adulthood.31 This resilience manifested as an anxiolytic-like phenotype across several paradigms that involved exposure to environmental stress and was accompanied by a blunted response of the autonomic nervous system and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis. Based on seed-voxel correlations derived from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), functional disconnectivity between the amygdala and other brain regions involved in processing of anxiogenic stimuli was suggested as an underlying mechanism for this phenotype.32 Although these data contributed to understanding the genetic basis of emotional regulation and its associated neurocircuitry, we did not correlate gene expression and behavior with the global organization of brain networks. Moreover, we focused mainly on emotional phenotypes and treated Ahi1 expression dichotomously rather than as a continuum.

In this paper, we describe a multi-modal effort to link early variations in expression of the neurodevelopmental gene, Ahi1, to long-term effects on cognitive-emotional phenotypes and functional brain networks. In the present report: (i) We add the cognitive-emotional interface to the stress-resilient behavioral phenotypes previously reported in relation to our Ahi1+/− mouse, showing that although its cognitive performance is diminished overall, it may have an advantage in highly stressogenic scenarios. Then, by analyzing data from a comprehensive neuroinformatic database of inbred mouse strains,33 we corroborate and extend the behavioral data by (ii) demonstrating that lower neocortical Ahi1 expression during neurodevelopment is associated with diminished capacity for associative learning in adulthood and by (iii) suggesting that the latter may arise from polymorphisms in a trans-acting major regulatory element that modulates the expression of several functionally related genes during neurodevelopment. (iv) Finally, we assess the global topology of whole-brain networks using resting-state fMRI, to reveal that networks derived from Ahi1+/− mice display higher assortativity but lower hierarchy compared with the Ahi1+/+ networks.

Materials and Methods

Generation of the Ahi1+/− mouse line

As previously described, chimeric male mice carrying a gene trap vector34 at the Ahi1 locus were backcrossed with WT C57BL/6 females to generate heterozygous Ahi1 knockout mice (Ahi1+/−).31 Ahi1+/− males were then backcrossed to wild-type (Ahi1+/+) females for over eight generations in order to achieve homogenous background. As previously reported, Ahi1+/− mice displayed a ~50% reduction in Jouberin (Ahi1 protein) levels at postnatal day 3,31 corresponding to the neurodevelopmental window when Jouberin is most critical.35 This between-genotype difference was no longer present in adulthood, when Ahi1 expression falls dramatically. For the current experiments, we used a wild-type × heterozygous breeding scheme. By using littermates as controls, biases due to maternal effects are minimized. All animal procedures were approved by the Hebrew University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.

Design of animal experiments

Prior to behavioral testing, mice were housed at the Hebrew University Reproduction Unit under specific pathogen-free conditions with a 12-h light/dark cycle. Food and water were provided ad libitum. All experiments were conducted during morning hours (0900–1200 hours). Two-month-old male mice underwent the following tests: (i) Open field (OF) and swim-to-visible platform task (n=25); (ii) novel object recognition (n=16); and (iii) fear conditioning (n=30). Experiments were conducted in a fixed order and on separate days using counterbalanced groups. In the behavioral experiments, animals were tracked with a video camera positioned overhead and data were extracted with Ethovision (Noldus Information Technologies, Wageningen, The Netherlands). For the graph-based study of functional brain networks, imaging data that had been previously acquired31 was re-analyzed using the relevant mathematical tools (n=25).

Behavioral measures

Open field

The apparatus consisted of a square arena measuring 50 × 50 × 33 cm3 under 15 lux illumination. Mice from both genotypes were allowed to explore the arena for 6 min. Time spent in the central zone of the arena (10 × 10 cm2) was calculated.

Swim-to-visible platform task

Modified from Beiko et al.,36 a platform (15 × 15 cm2) was placed inside the center of a round (120 cm diameter) pool, protruding 2 cm above the surface of the water and marked by a 10-cm tall flag. Each mouse was placed in the pool once and allowed a maximum of 120 s to locate the platform. Escape latency, distance travelled and percentage of time spent in the peripheral 20 cm ring were calculated.

Novel object recognition test

In the one-trial non-matching-to-sample object recognition task,37 mice were allowed to explore two ‘identical’ to-be-familiarized (sample) objects for 10 min. After 1 h, mice were returned to the apparatus, which now contained one familiar object and a novel object. Discrimination ratio was defined as the novel object interaction duration divided by the total duration of interaction with both objects during a 5-min test session. As stress and novelty can adversely affect object interaction and hence object recognition, mice were handled daily for 3 days and habituated to the test arena before the experiment.

Fear conditioning

Training was conducted in a conditioning chamber (19 × 25 × 19 cm3) equipped with black methacrylate walls, transparent front door, a speaker and grid floor, placed inside an outer sound-attenuating chamber (StartFear System, Panlab, Holliston, MA, USA). A computer program (PACKWIN 2.0, Panlab) controlled the audio generator to deliver the tone and the shock generator, wired to the grid floor, which generated a scrambled foot-shock. Following a similar design to that described by Licht et al.,38 fear conditioning to both context and cue was evaluated 24 h after the conditioning procedure (see Supplementary Methods for further details).

Measuring the relationship between Ahi1 expression and fear learning across the BXD recombinant inbred strains

We employed GeneNetwork (http://www.genenetwork.org), a website that integrates data sets of complex traits with software for quantifying correlations among traits.33, 39 The data sets include both biological traits—neuroanatomical, pharmacological, and behavioral—and microarray-based gene expression data from numerous brain regions across recombinant inbred (RI) lines, which were generated by crosses of C57BL/6J and DBA/2 (BXD) inbred strains. Each of the BXD strains is a unique ‘mosaic’ of chromosomal segments inherited from either the B or D progenitor strain.40 A search function finds correlations between mRNA expression and biological traits.33 Further details depicting the methodology used for curating neocortical Ahi1 expression profiles and fear conditioning traits and then inter-correlating them are provided in the Supplementary Methods and in Supplementary Figure S1.

Mapping QTLs that modulate Ahi1 expression and fear learning across the BXD RI strains

A quantitative trait loci (QTL) is a chromosome region that contains one or more sequence variants that modulate the distribution of a variable trait measured in a sample of genetically diverse individuals from an interbreeding population. Differences in the expression of mRNA are often treated as standard phenotypes, much like behavioral phenotypes. However, QTLs of mRNAs (termed expression QTLs (eQTLs)) have the unique property of (usually) having a single parent gene and genetic location. An eQTL that maps to the location of the parent gene that produces the mRNA is referred to as a cis-eQTL, while an eQTL that maps far away from its parent gene is referred to as trans-eQTL.41 One of the most powerful features of GeneNetwork is its ability to detect and evaluate, both qualitatively and quantitatively, common and unique (e)QTLs for a set of traits that include both expression and behavior phenotypes.33, 42 In the current analysis, the following issues were sequentially addressed: (i) eQTL mapping of neocortical Ahi1 expression during the neonatal period; (ii) determining whether the major eQTL regulating neonatal Ahi1 expression also serves as a QTL for associative fear learning in adulthood; (iii) identifying genes within the developing neocortex that share their major eQTL with Ahi1; (iv) enrichment analysis based on these genes; and finally (v) Generation of QTL heat maps loaded with traits that represent both the gene transcripts that comprise the highly enriched Gene Ontology categories identified and the relevant fear conditioning traits. Further details of the methodology used are provided in Supplementary Methods.

Resting-state functional networks

Acquisition

As previously described,31 and in line with a recent report using a similar methodology in rats,43 MRI measurements were performed with a 4.7-T Bruker BioSpec scanner (Bruker Biospin, Ettlington, Germany) using a Dotty quadrature mouse head coil (see Supplementary Methods for further details).

Preprocessing

As previously described,31, 43 functional data were first processed in the SPM8 software (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8) using standard spatial preprocessing steps. Images were slice-time corrected and realigned. Data with motion that was >0.2 mm (one voxel) or that needed a rotation >±0.002° for their registrations were removed from the analysis. At a second step, analysis was performed using the custom-made IDL and MATLAB software. It included regression-out of the six functions that are related to motion (from the SPM analysis), 2D non-linear alignment of the MRI images to the mouse brain atlas,44 data smoothing by a three-point-Gaussian kernel and band-pass filtering (<0.01 to >0.1 Hz).

Generation of functional brain networks

Based on our previous experience43 and current hypothesis, regions of interest (ROIs) scattered throughout the brain were preselected. Representative ROIs within the cortices, basal ganglia, limbic system and midbrain were included. Inter-regional correlations in blood oxygenation-level dependent (BOLD) signal between all ROIs were averaged across time using the above-mentioned software. For improving normality, all correlation values underwent a Fisher’s r-to-z transformation.

Graph-based analysis

For graph-theoretical analysis of neural networks through fMRI, anatomical brain regions are considered as nodes, linked by edges, which represent the connectivity measured by the temporal correlation of BOLD signal fluctuations between the nodes.5 The topological properties of the networks derived by thresholding the matrix of inter-regional BOLD signal correlations depend on the choice of threshold values.4 Our first effort was therefore to define a range of thresholds that yielded fully connected networks with a small-world topology.45 Next, we determined the assortativity46 and hierarchy47 coefficients for each individual network and compared means between groups48 (see Supplementary Methods for further details).

Statistical analysis

In the behavioral tests, mean scores among Ahi1+/+ and Ahi1+/− mice were compared using the non-paired samples t-test. In the fear conditioning experiments that involved both genotype and conditioning-state as the independent between- and within- group variables, respectively, mixed analysis of variances were conducted. These were then followed by simple effects analysis, depending on the statistical significance of the interaction and the main effects.49 Both Pearson’s product–moment and Spearman rank-order correlations were used for estimating relationships between gene expression profiles and behavioral traits obtained from the BXD RI strain database. In the graph-based network analysis, quantification of local and global network metrics was performed using the MATLAB-based graph-theoretical analysis toolbox for analyzing between-group differences in large-scale structural and functional brain networks.48 Between-genotype group comparisons were performed using a bootstrap test for non-paired samples (1000 permutations). Further details of the statistical approaches used are provided in Supplementary Methods.

Results

Behavioral experiments measuring the cognitive-emotional interface

Besides replicating the previously reported anxiolytic-like phenotype,31 these experiments were conducted in order to explore cognitive aspects of the Ahi1 knockout mouse, with an emphasis on the cognitive-emotional interface.

Anxiety and performance during exposure to a novel stressor

When introduced into novel OF and Morris water maze (MWM) arenas, naive Ahi1+/− mice displayed significantly lower thigmotactic behavior compared with naive Ahi1+/+ mice (OF; t23=2.7, P=0.013 and MWM; t23=3.1, P=0.005, respectively, Figures 1a–d). During first-time MWM exposure, Ahi1+/− mice displayed superior performance compared with Ahi1+/+ mice in the swim-to-visible platform task, manifested as a shorter latency and a trend for shorter distance to reach platform (t23=2.2, P=0.042 and t23=1.9, P=0.075, respectively, Figures 1e and f). Notably, travelling and swimming velocities were similar across genotypes (Figures 1g and h), indicating that differences in motor abilities did not account for the superior performance of the Ahi1+/− mice in this task.

Figure 1
figure 1

Anxiety vs performance during exposure of the Ahi1 knockout mouse to a novel stressor. (a, b) Thigmotaxis in the open field arena. (a, upper panel) Track visualizations of typical Ahi1+/+ (left, blue tracing) and Ahi1+/− (right, yellow tracing) mice inside the open field arenas. (a, lower panel) Heat map visualizations of Ahi1+/+ (left) and Ahi1+/− (right) mice inside the open field arena. For each genotype, all relevant tracks were merged into one heat map. The color of each pixel represents the mean time subjects located at that specific location. (b) Bar graphs depicting the proportion (percent) of time that mice from each genotype spent in the center of the open field arena. Compared with Ahi1+/+ mice, Ahi1+/− mice displayed significantly more time in the center when placed in the novel, highly stressogenic open field arena. (c, d) Thigmotaxis in the first-time swim-to-visible platform task inside a Morris water maze (MWM) arena. (c) Track visualizations of typical Ahi1+/+ and Ahi1+/− mice (upper panel) and heat map visualizations of group means (lower panel) in this task are presented as detailed above. (d) Bar graphs depicting the proportion (percentage) of time that mice from each genotype spent in the center of the MWM arena. (e, f) First-time performance in the swim-to-visible platform task. Ahi1+/− mice outperformed their Ahi1+/+ controls in the MWM visible platform escape task, manifested by the latter’s longer escape latency (e) and marginally longer distance swim (f). (g, h) Velocities during exposure to the novel stressors. Travelling (g) and swimming (h) velocities were similar across genotypes. *P<0.05. **P<0.01. n=12 (Ahi1+/+), n=13 (Ahi1+/−). Data are expressed as mean±s.e.m.

PowerPoint slide

Learning in a low-stress environment

By choosing a small apparatus with which the animals have been familiarized during pretraining, the OF situation which evokes anxiety- and stress-related behaviors such as thigmotaxis can be minimized.50 Under such conditions, the preference of Ahi1+/− mice for the novel object during the novel object recognition test (NORT) was significantly reduced compared with their Ahi1+/+ littermates, as expressed by the corresponding difference score (−1.89 vs 4.00, t8.8=2.9, P=0.017) and discrimination ratio (0.44 vs 0.62, t8.2=3.0, P=0.016, Figure 2a).

Figure 2
figure 2

Memory and fear learning in the Ahi knockout mouse. (a) Novel object recognition. In this low-stress task, Ahi1+/+ mice displayed better performance than their Ahi1+/− littermates, as manifested by their superior discrimination ability (ratio of time spent with the novel object out of total time spent with both objects). *P<0.05. n=7 (Ahi1+/+), n=9 (Ahi1+/−). (b, c) Pavlovian fear conditioning. In these fear-learning paradigms, Ahi1+/− exhibited a diminished ability to memorize the association between the shock and the context (b) or cue (c). Although baseline freezing (percentage of time freezing during 30 s) was similar across genotypes (habituation and pretone periods), as was the magnitude of freezing increment in the immediate postshock period (b), during the test, exposure to the preconditioned context (b) or cue (tone, c) resulted in freezing responses that were significantly lower in the Ahi1+/− mice compared with Ahi1+/+ littermates, leading to a significant conditioning state vs genotype interaction. ***P<0.001 compared with the same genotype’s freezing during the prior conditioning state. ++P<0.01 compared with Ahi1+/+ mice exposed to the conditioned stimulus. n=15 mice/group. Data are expressed as mean±s.e.m.

PowerPoint slide

Associative fear learning

Although passive defensive responses such as freezing are reflexive, fear conditioning paradigms measure the freezing response that takes place following pairing of an unconditioned stimulus, such as foot shock, with a conditioned stimulus.51 As such, fear conditioning to either context or cue represents a form of associative learning that has been extensively studied in many species.52 Using a mixed analysis of variance design, with conditioning-state (habituation vs training vs context test) and genotype as the within- and between-subject variables, respectively, and freezing to context (percentage) as the dependent variable, a significant interaction was noted (F2,56=8.4, P=0.0006, Figure 2b). Post-hoc analysis revealed significant increases in freezing behavior during the immediate postshock period (Bonferroni’s pairwise comparisons, mean differences relative to habituation are 20.47 and 19.97 for Ahi1+/+ and Ahi1+/− mice, respectively, P<0.0005 for both genotypes) and then again during the context test (mean differences relative to postshock period are 53.59 and 33.43 for wild-type and Ahi1+/− mice, respectively, P<0.0005 for both genotypes). However, analysis of the simple main effect of genotype at each conditioning-state level revealed that, while freezing behavior was similar during habituation and postshock periods, during the context test Ahi1+/− mice displayed significantly lower freezing compared with wild-type littermates (independent samples test, t28=3.1, P=0.005). These results suggest that underlying differences in the innate, footshock-elicited, defensive freezing reaction were unlikely to account for the strong between-genotype difference upon context re-exposure. Using a similar design, with exposure to cue (pretone vs tone) and genotype as the within- and between-subject variables, respectively, a significant interaction was also noted (F1,28=8.274, P=0.008, Figure 2c). Similarly, post-hoc analysis revealed that, while exposure to the cue elicited large increases in freezing in both Ahi1+/+ and Ahi1+/− mice, a between-genotype difference was only evident during the tone (independent samples test, t19.9=3.5, P=0.002).

Relationship between Ahi1 expression and fear learning across the BXD RI strains

Given the distinct phenotypes observed in Ahi1+/− mice with respect to fear learning, we attempted to correlate these traits with physiological variation in neocortical Ahi1 expression, measured in neonates (P3), in juveniles (P14) and in adults, across the BXD RI strains. In both developmental neocortical expression data sets (BIDMC/UTHSC Dev Neocortex P3 and BIDMC/UTHSC Dev Neocortex P14) and in the corresponding adult data set (HQF BXD Neocortex P60), we identified two probes for Ahi1, one located in exon 5 and the other in the gene’s 3′UTR. The latter probe was chosen for further analysis owing to its consistently higher expression (Supplementary Table S1). Expression profiles measured at each of the three developmental stages were normally distributed, yet highly variable, across strains (Figures 3a and b, Supplementary Figure S1a and Supplementary Table S2). Moreover, while expression profiles at P3 and P14 were modestly correlated across strains (rpearson=0.446, P=0.002, Figure 3c), they were weakly correlated with the P60 expression profile (Supplementary Figures 1b and c), suggesting that (i) significant changes in Ahi1 expression occur upon maturation of the brain and (ii) these changes themselves are variable across strains.

Figure 3
figure 3

Relationship between Ahi1 expression and fear learning across the BXD recombinant inbred strains. (a, b) Bar graphs depicting the expression of the Ahi1 3′UTR (untranslated region) probe (y axis, arbitrary units (log2)) across the BXD RI and parent strains assessed in the neocortex at postnatal days (a) P3 and (b) P14. Note variability in probe expression across strains (2–3-fold range). (c) Pearson’s product–moment correlation between expression of the Ahi1 3′UTR probe measured at P14 (y axis) vs P3 (x axis). Note that, although significant, it is only modest in strength. (d) Bar graph depicting ‘PC_Owen’, the major principal component (PC) derived from three traits related to contextual fear conditioning reported by Owen et al.53 across adult mice from 23 RI strains. Note that PC trait is highly variable across strains. (eg) Pearson’s product–moment correlations between ‘PC_Owen’ (y axis) and expression of the Ahi1 3′UTR probes (x axis) measured at (e) P3, (f) P14 and (g) P60. Only strains for which both expression and behavioral data were available were entered into the correlation. Note the strong, highly significant correlation between contextual fear learning in adulthood and Ahi1 expression in neonates. Fear learning was not correlated with Ahi1 expression in adulthood and only marginally correlated with Ahi1 expression in juveniles. (h) Bar graph depicting ‘PC_Philip’, the major PC derived from six traits related to contextual and cued fear conditioning reported by Philip et al.54 across adult mice from 60 RI strains. (ik) Pearson’s product–moment correlations between ‘PC_Philip’ (y axis) and expression of the Ahi1 3′UTR probes (x axis) measured at (i) P3, (j) P14 and (k) P60. Note that cued fear learning in adult mice was moderately correlated with the neonatal expression profile of Ahi1 and weakly correlated with the adult profile. Notice that although the correlations between Ahi1 expression and PC_Philip were negative, the traits reported by Philip et al.54 reflect activity upon presentation of the conditioned stimulus, while those reported by Owen et al.53 reflect freezing. Hence, correlations between Ahi1 expression and fear conditioning in these two reports point to the same biological direction, so that strains exhibiting lower neocortical Ahi1 expression tend to perform worse on these associative learning paradigms. The data in the bar graphs depict mean±s.e.m. calculated over samples from each individual RI strain. For sample sizes, please refer to individual reports.53, 54

PowerPoint slide

Using a predefined keyword search across the BXD Published Phenotypes Database, we identified four independent scientific reports measuring contextual and cued fear conditioning53, 54, 55, 56 (Supplementary Tables S3 and S4). Principal component (PC) analysis revealed single PCs accounting for >65% of the variability in the traits reported by Owen et al.53 (PC_Owen derived from three traits related to contextual conditioning, Figure 3d and Supplementary Table S5), by Philip et al.54 (PC_Philip derived from six traits related to contextual and cued conditioning, Figure 3h and Supplementary Table S6) and by Brigman et al.55 (PC_ Brigman derived from three traits related to contextual and cued conditioning, Supplementary Table S7). There was only one relevant trait reported by Yang et al.56 (10901_Yang). PC_Philip and PC_Brigman were the only significantly inter-correlated PCs (Supplementary Table S8), suggesting considerable intra-strain differences across the four reports.

Three of the abovementioned reports recorded data on at least 10 strains that were also represented in the relevant neocortical expression data sets.53, 54, 55, 56 Although a strong, highly significant correlation across strains was noted between PC_Owen and the P3 Ahi1 expression profile (rpearson=0.735, Praw=0.001, Padj=0.009), PC_Owen’s correlation with the P14 profile, which was only moderate, did not withstand correction for multiple testing (rpearson=0.516, Praw=0.041, Padj=0.092, Figures 3e and f, respectively). PC_Owen and the P60 Ahi1 expression profile were not correlated at all (rpearson=0.070, Praw=0.790, Figure 3g). With respect to PC_Philip, a significant correlation across strains with the P3 Ahi1 expression profile was noted (rpearson=−0.418, Praw=0.011, Padj=0.0495, Figure 3i). PC_Philip’s correlations with the P14 and P60 profiles were non-significant (rpearson=−0.305, Praw=0.039, Padj=0.092 and rpearson=−0.079, Praw=0.645, Figures 3j and k, respectively). A detailed summary of Pearson’s product–moment and Spearman rank-order correlations between the Ahi1 expression profiles and fear conditioning traits is provided in Supplementary Tables S9 and S10. Notably, although the correlations between Ahi1 expression and PC_Philip were negative, the traits reported by Philip et al.54 depict activity upon presentation of the conditioned stimulus, while those reported by Owen et al.53 relate to freezing. Hence, correlations between Ahi1 expression and fear conditioning in these two reports point essentially to the same biological direction, so that strains exhibiting lower neocortical Ahi1 expression tend to perform worse on these associative learning paradigms. Finally, hierarchical regression analysis revealed that while Ahi1 expression at P3 predicted 48% and 23% of the total variance in PC_Owen and PC_Philip, respectively, addition of the P14 and P60 time points to the earlier one did not improve model strength (Supplementary Tables S11 and S12). These data suggest that Ahi1 expression level during early neurodevelopment may be a critical determinant of fear-learning performance in adulthood.

QTLs modulating Ahi1 expression and fear learning across the BXD RI strains

Given the implied role of Ahi1 expression levels during early neurodevelopment in determining associative learning in adulthood, we next performed an eQTL analysis of Ahi1, as measured within the P3 neocortical expression data set, by computing linkage maps across the entire genome. A trans-eQTL with a maximum logarithm of odds score of 4.59, well above the whole-genome significance cutoff of 3.80, was mapped to Chr1: 69.048453, (Figures 4a–c and Supplementary Figure S3). This locus harbors an intronic single-nucleotide polymorphism (rs6312657) within the ErbB4 gene that varies across the BXD progeny. The DD genotype was associated with mean Ahi1 levels that were roughly ~35% higher compared with the BB genotype (t41=5.1, P<0.0005, Figure 4d). The PC_Owen, PC_Philip and PC_Brigman traits included strain-specific data that partially overlapped the rs6312657-genotyped data set, so that at least eight phenotyped mice could be grouped into each homozygous genotype category (Supplementary Table S13). Notably, out of these PC traits, rs6312657 polymorphisms were associated with significant differences in PC_Brigman, so that strains carrying the DD genotype displayed enhanced fear learning compared with BB carriers (t18=2.687, Praw=0.015, Padj=0.045, Figure 4e and Supplementary Table S14).

Figure 4
figure 4

Mapping quantitative trait loci (QTLs) that modulate Ahi1 expression and fear conditioning. (ac) The major QTL controlling the expression of Ahi1 in the neocortex during early neonatal life, identified by interval mapping the Ahi1 probe (ILMN_2813712) from the BIDMC/UTHSC Dev Neocortex P3 database against dense genotype data for 43 BXD RI strains. Plots show interval maps for the whole genome (a), for Chr 1 (b) and for the maximal QTL region on chromosome 1 at 66–72 Mb (c). The x axis represents the physical map of the chromosome(s); the y axis and thick blue line provide the logarithm of odds (LOD) score of the association between Ahi1 expression and the genotypes of markers. The two horizontal lines are the suggestive (gray) and significance (pink) thresholds computed using 5000 permutations. The secondary lines indicate whether C57BL/6J (red) or DBA/2J (green) alleles increase Ahi1 expression values. The additive effect of these alleles is quantified using the green scale on the far right of the plot, which represents normalized log2 expression values. Yellow bars (in panels b and c) indicate results of a bootstrap analysis evaluating approximate confidence limits of QTL peaks. Orange lines show single-nucleotide polymorphism density. Color bars above the thick blue line denote actual gene locations. Note position of the ErbB4 and Mtap2 genes. (d, e) Effect plots of marker rs6312657 (Chr 1 at 69.048453 Mb) on the expression of Ahi1 in the neocortex during early neonatal life (d) and on fear conditioning during adulthood (PC_Brigman, e). Notice higher phenotypic means for the D allele when compared with the B allele. This difference is significant and therefore results in the presence of a single QTL modulating both traits. *P<0.05, ****P<0.0005. (f) Enrichment analysis based on GO terms among 41 transcripts that have a single best trans-eQTL that can be mapped to the Chr1: 69.048453 locus with LOD scores >3.80. Only biological processes are depicted. Bubbles are color-coded according to the false-discovery rate-adjusted log10 P-value of category-enriched genes relative to the whole mouse genome. Significance ranges from ~2 × 10−8 (blue) to ~2 × 10−5 (red). The x and y coordinates depict semantic similarities among GO terms. (g) QTL ‘heat map’ of LOD scores, with red indicating modulation by DBA/2J alleles and blue indicating modulation by C57BL/6J alleles. More intense colors mark chromosomal regions with comparatively high linkage statistics. Only loci with LOD scores >(0.5 × suggestive threshold) are color-coded. The individual traits are run side by side to enable easy detection of common and unique QTLs. Seven transcripts involved in neuronal development, differentiation and projections and four fear-conditioning traits (Supplementary Tables S16 and S8, respectively) are displayed. The genome location is shown along the long axis of the map, marker by marker. As it is intended for qualitative assessment, only two representative chromosomes (1 and 2) are depicted. Notice that sharing of QTLs among the subset of seven transcripts and between them and the fear conditioning traits is evident not only at the significant Chr1: 69.048453 QTL (pink arrow) but also at several other suggestive (or near-suggestive) QTLs (gray arrows).

PowerPoint slide

Next, we sought to determine whether expression of other relevant genes during neocortical development is also modulated by the same locus that regulates Ahi1 expression. eQTL analysis of the entire BIDMC/UTHSC Dev Neocortex P3 data set revealed 41 transcripts that have a single best eQTL (trans-eQTL in all cases) that can be mapped to the Chr1: 69.048453 locus with logarithm of odds scores >3.80 (Supplementary Table S15). Among those 41 transcripts, enrichment analysis revealed significant over-representation of a subset of seven genes involved in key biological processes such as neuron development (GO:0048666, Praw=5.38 × 10−11, Padj=1.38 × 10−8), neuron differentiation (GO:0030182, Praw=2.95 × 10−10, Padj=2.53 × 10−8) and cell projection organization (GO:0030030, Praw=2.22 × 10−10, Padj=2.53 × 10−8 (Figure 4f and Supplementary Table S16). Notably, besides Ahi1, coded by these seven genes are proteins such as Hap1, which forms, together with Ahi1, a stable protein complex that is critical during early brain development, and may be involved in emotional regulation.57, 58, 59 Finally, using a heat map visualization that facilitates detection of common QTLs for sets of traits, sharing of suggestive and significant QTLs, other than at Chr1: 69.048453, was observed (i) among the subset of seven genes; and (ii) between these genes and the fear conditioning traits (Figure 4g).

Studies of functional brain networks

Given the recently proposed placement of brain connectivity as an ‘intermediate phenotype’ positioned between genetics and behavior,6 together with the long-lasting effects that naturally occurring or genetically manipulated variations in Ahi1 expression exert in terms of cognitive-emotional outcomes, we next attempted to determine the effects of reduced Ahi1 expression, evident in the Ahi1 knockout mouse, on the topology of whole-brain functional networks. The nodes depicted in these networks were based on 27 preselected ROIs (Supplementary Table S17).

Small-world organization of resting-state brain networks

The small-world index of a network is defined as the ratio of the global clustering coefficient to the mean shortest path length, after both metrics have been standardized by comparing their values to those in equivalent random networks.4 Networks were evaluated across connection densities greater than the minimal density of 0.34, at which all nodes were connected for the first time. Whole-brain networks of both Ahi1+/+ and Ahi1+/− mice had clustering coefficients that were higher than those expected in random networks, while having shortest path lengths similar to random networks. This pattern resulted in small-world indices greater than one across densities (Supplementary Figures S4a–c). However, between-genotype differences in these global network metrics were non-significant (Supplementary Figures S4d–f). As expected, when the connection density was increased, the small-world index σ monotonically declined toward an asymptotic value of 1. For densities >0.50, we found that σ⩽1.2, indicating that the brain networks were becoming topologically indistinguishable from random graphs. Thus we identified the range of densities 0.34⩽d≤0.50 as the regime associated with small-world properties of fully connected functional networks.

Assortative organization of resting-state brain networks

The assortativity coefficient (α) is the Pearson correlation coefficient of degree between pairs of linked nodes46 (Supplementary Figures S5a and b). Quantified for each individual functional network, α was distributed normally across mice (Supplementary Table S18). At minimum network density (d=0.34), α ranged between 0.01 and 0.56. Notably, although the total number of edges (that is, connections) is identical when networks are held at same densities, individual differences in assortativity produced strikingly different topologies. Although these differences are most prominent graphically when nodes were positioned according to their degree metrics and edges were binarized (Figures 5a and b), anatomical and heat map visualizations of these same networks, in which edges depict the weighted strength of the functional connections, convey additional information. In the individual networks presented, the non-assortative network displays strong intra-cortical connections, with moderate long-range functional connections between rostral and caudal structures (Figures 5c and e). On the other hand, while the assortative network displays dense mesolimbic connections, long-range rostrocaudal connections are sparse (Figures 5d and f). Beyond qualitative examination of individual networks with different assortativity coefficients, graph-based analysis can provide valuable quantitative information that enables direct between-group comparisons. On a group level, Ahi1+/− mice displayed an assortative mixing pattern whereas the topology of the Ahi1+/+ mice was virtually non-assortative (Figure 5g). This difference remained significant across densities (bootstrap for independent samples test, P=0.016–0.058, PAUC=0.026, Figure 5h).

Figure 5
figure 5

Assortative organization of whole-brain functional networks. (af) Examples of functional networks derived from individual mice held at minimum density (d=0.34). (a, b) Schematic visualizations of non-assortative (a, α=0.01) and highly assortative (b, α=0.56) networks. Node position along the y axis corresponds to nodal degree. Edges are binarized. Note that, while in the assortative network nodes tended to preferentially connect to other nodes with a similar degree, connections in the non-assortative network seem random in that sense. Moreover, although the total number of edges is identical in both networks, the difference in assortativity gives rise to strikingly different topologies. (c, d) Anatomical visualizations of these same networks, viewed from the horizontal and sagittal planes (top and bottom images, respectively). These images were produced with weighted edges, which depict the strength of the functional connections. Edge colors correspond to the Fisher-transformed Z-score of the correlation coefficient between each pair of connected nodes (see color bar to the right). Only the top 34% of connections, in terms of their absolute Z-score, are depicted. Nodes are color-coded according to the different brain regions that they represent: Dark green, cortical regions; light blue, striatum; marine blue, putamen; light green, amygdala; olive, hippocampus; pink, midbrain structures; red, mammillary bodies. (e, f) Heat maps of the adjacency matrices derived from these same networks, using the same Z-score color bar as above. Regions of interest are presented according to their anatomical position along the rostrocaudal axis, so that the most rostral are assigned numbers 1 and 2 and the most caudal 26 and 27. (g) Changes in network assortativity as a function of network density. Mean network assortativity for Ahi1+/+ and Ahi1+/− mice (blue and yellow, respectively) are depicted across densities within the small-world regime. Note that Ahi1+/− mice displayed an assortative pattern across densities, compared with a non-assortative topology displayed by Ahi1+/+ mice. (f) Between-genotype differences in network assortativity as a function of network density. The 95% confidence intervals and between-group differences in assortativity are depicted. The red * marker shows the difference between Ahi1+/− and Ahi1+/+ networks; thus * signs falling above the confidence intervals indicate densities in which Ahi1+/− mean network assortativity is significantly higher than Ahi1+/+. Relative to the Ahi1+/+ networks, the assortativity coefficients of the Ahi1+/− networks were significantly higher across almost all densities. n=13 (Ahi1+/+), n=12 (Ahi1+/−).

PowerPoint slide

Hierarchical organization of resting-state brain networks

The hierarchy coefficient (β) depicts the power–law relationship between each node’s local clustering and degree metrics across the entire network47 (Supplementary Figure S5c). Quantified per each individual functional network, hierarchy was inversely correlated with assortativity across mice (r=−0.695, P<0.001, Supplementary Figure S5d). At minimum network density (d=0.34), β ranged between −0.45 and 0.42. Again, while these differences are most conspicuous when presented in graph format (Figures 6a and b), anatomical and heat map visualizations of these same networks revealed that compared with the dis-hierarchical one, the hierarchical network displays strong local intra-cortical connections with prominent corticolimbic and mesocortical anticorrelations (Figures 6c–f). Beyond quantification of individual network hierarchies (Figures 6g and h), graph-based group estimates revealed that while Ahi1+/+ mice displayed a hierarchical network topology (mean β=0.16, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.07–0.25), their Ahi1+/− littermates displayed a non-hierarchical pattern (mean β=−0.06, 95% CI −0.19 to 0.09). The resulting between-group difference was significant when hierarchy was measured in networks held at minimum density (bootstrap for independent samples test, P=0.022) as well as in networks derived from parameter area under the curve-averaged across densities (same test, P=0.019).

Figure 6
figure 6

Hierarchical organization of whole-brain functional networks. (af) Examples of functional networks derived from individual mice held at minimum density (d=0.34). (a, b) Schematic visualizations of dis-hierarchical (a) and hierarchical (b) networks. Node position along the y axis corresponds to nodal degree, while node size corresponds to nodal clustering coefficient. Edges are binarized. Note that, while in the dis-hierarchical network high-degree nodes are also characterized by high clustering coefficients, a reciprocal relationship is evident in the hierarchical network. (c, d) Anatomical visualizations of these same networks, viewed from the horizontal and sagittal planes (top and bottom images, respectively). These images were produced with weighted edges, which depict the strength of the functional connections. Edge colors correspond to the Fisher-transformed Z-score of the correlation coefficient between each pair of connected nodes (see color bar to the right). Only the top 34% of connections, in terms of their absolute Z-score, are depicted. Nodes are color-coded as in the previous figure. (e, f) Heat maps of the adjacency matrices derived from these same networks, based on Z-scores (see color bar). Regions of interest are presented according to their anatomical position along the rostrocaudal axis. (g, h) The network’s hierarchy coefficient β is quantified by estimating the inverse of the regression line’s slope. n=13 (Ahi1+/+), n=12 (Ahi1+/−).

PowerPoint slide

Regional functional connectivity of the SC

Important brain regions (hubs) often interact with many other regions, facilitate functional integration and have a key role in network resilience to insult. Measures of node centrality variously assess importance of individual nodes on the above criteria. Nodal degree is one of the most common measures of centrality and has a straightforward neurobiological interpretation: nodes with a high degree are interacting, structurally or functionally, with many other nodes in the network.5 As the superior colliculi (SC) serve as relative hubs both in the non-assortative as well as in the assortative network (pink nodes in Figure 5), and given their unique roles in the processing of sensory information,60 we next attempted to compare differences in the SC’s nodal degree between wild-type mice and Ahi1+/− littermates. Using a one-way multivariate analysis of variance with the right and left SC as the dependent variables, we detected a statistically significant difference between genotypes on the combined end point (F[2,24]=3.514, P=0.046; Wilks’ Λ=0.774; partial η2=0.226), suggesting that the SC could serve as an example for the association between Ahi1 underexpression and distinct spatial patterns of functional connectivity.

Discussion

We have reported a composite effort to link early variations in the expression of the neurodevelopmental gene Ahi1 to long-term effects on functional brain networks and cognitive-emotional phenotypes. Our behavioral findings suggest that under test conditions which bring forth their stress-resilient phenotype (swim-to-visible-platform) but do not present a significant cognitive load, Ahi1+/− mice outperform their Ahi1+/+ comparators. However, under test conditions that obviate between-genotype stress-related differences, as is the case with the NORT (minimal stress) and fear conditioning paradigms (bypass stress resilience), Ahi1+/− mice display consistent cognitive deficits. These findings were corroborated by quantifying neonatal expression × adult behavior correlations and neonatal eQTL × adult QTL relationships, using a comprehensive neuroinformatic database of RI mouse strains, in which Ahi1 expression is measured as a continuum at several developmental points. Finally, using resting-state fMRI we demonstrated that whole-brain functional networks in Ahi1+/− mice display strikingly different properties in terms of their assortative and hierarchical organization compared with Ahi1+/+ littermates.

Introducing rodents into a novel environment causes a significant stress response.61 In such situations, rodents are often inclined to mainly explore the peripheral zones. The tendency to remain near the walls, thigmotaxis, serves as a reliable ethological index of anxiety.62 In a new cohort of young adult mice, we have replicated our previous finding regarding the anxiolytic-like phenotype manifested by Ahi1+/− mice under neophobic conditions.31 Imposing forced activity, such as forced swimming, likewise induces a profound stress response in rodents.63 Not surprisingly, when faced for the first time with the combination of forced swimming and a novel environment, such as in the MWM, mice display strongly thigmotactic behavior. Thigmotaxis and anxiety have been negatively correlated with performance in this scenario. For instance, relative to males, naive female rats displayed higher anxiety and inferior performance in swimming to a visible platform of a MWM owing to strongly thigmotactic swimming.36 Hence, it is plausible that the Ahi1+/− mice outperformed their Ahi1+/+ controls in this straightforward, learning-independent task by virtue of their relative stress resilience.

Rodents have a tendency to interact more with a novel object than with a familiar object. This tendency has been used to study learning and memory.64 In fact, the NORT is not only very sensitive and capable of detecting subtle differences in memory (discrimination) but also in exploratory performance.65 As a consequence, it is susceptible to potential biases owing to neophobic stress. By choosing a small apparatus with which the animals have been familiarized with during pretraining, the OF situation that evokes anxiety and stress-related behaviors such as thigmotaxis can be minimized.50 Thus the minimal amount of exploration required for stable discrimination performance is achieved.65 Under such circumstances, it is plausible that a genetically determined ‘advantage’ of a knockout mouse, in terms of stress resilience and exploratory performance, could be minimized. In this task, Ahi1+/+ mice outperformed their Ahi1+/− littermates. The superior performance of Ahi1+/+ mice in the NORT, as opposed to their inferior performance in the MWM, may thus be due to fundamental differences in these two tasks in terms of the skills required (learning vs exploration and agility) and the stress imposed (low vs high).

As opposed to the above-mentioned paradigms, in which performance can be interpreted in a relatively straight-forward manner, deciphering what underlies the marked reduction of fear memory retrieval in Ahi1+/ mice in the context and cued fear conditioning paradigms is more challenging. The immediate foot-shock-induced freezing response serves as an innate defense mechanism when animals are presented with a sudden nociceptive stimulus and has been used as a model for assessing anxiolytic drugs.66 Thus our results showing similar between-genotype freezing in the immediate postshock period of the training phase suggest that an ability to experience anxiety is preserved in Ahi1+/ mice. Notably, it was recently suggested that this immediate response is primarily elicited through the spino-parabrachio-amygdaloid pathway.67 In this way, pain could elicit direct amygdalar activation, obviating the need for intact corticolimbic circuitry. To this end, we have previously demonstrated that corticolimbic disconnectivity could underlie the stress-resilient phenotype evident when Ahi1+/− mice are exposed to environmental threats.31 We have also shown that pentylenetetrazole and caffeine, which are presumed to induce direct activation of the amygdala, brought about similar increases in serum corticosterone in both genotypes. Thus it seems reasonable to assume that when cortical deficits in threat detection are bypassed, through either nociception or pharmacological manipulation, Ahi1+/ mice retain an inherent ability to ‘feel’ anxious.

In contrast to immediate foot-shock-induced freezing, fear conditioning involves the pairing of a neutral stimulus with an aversive, unconditioned stimulus (US).52 The neutral stimulus initially elicits no emotional reaction, but after repeated pairings with the US, the neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus signaling imminent US onset and inducing anxiety associated with the anticipation of the aversive US. Fear conditioning is generally an adaptive and self-preserving form of associative learning. As such, it is recognized as a robust model system to investigate the neurobiological mechanisms of learning and memory in the mammalian brain. Although responding to the US largely depends on ascending spinal pathways, learning to associate it with the conditioned stimulus requires the participation of additional circuits, including the hippocampus and entorhinal, perirhinal and cingulate cortices. In fact, based on c-Fos immunoreactivity, the degree of freezing to conditioned context and tone has recently been correlated with activation of the entorhinal cortex, hippocampus (granule cell layer) and lateral amygdala.68

Given the previously demonstrated functional disconnectivity between the amygdala, ventral hippocampus and entorhinal cortex that characterizes the Ahi1+/ mice, our fear conditioning results could be viewed as a cognitive deficit attributed to such disconnectivity. This view is supported by the underperformance of Ahi1+/ mice in the NORT, as the most essential brain structures implicated in this cognitive task are also the hippocampus and rhinal cortices.69 Notably, in a recent study of mice with deletion of Rapgef6, another schizophrenia-related gene, the authors also attributed an anxiolytic-like phenotype, along with deficits in fear conditioning, to impaired hippocampal and amygdalar function.70 Although in our case this interpretation is well justified, others are plausible. Fear conditioning has long been implicated in the pathogenesis of stress-related psychopathology.71 In fact, higher conditionability has been associated with development of pathological anxiety.72 In this sense, the decreased conditioning response of the Ahi1+/− mice could (also) be viewed as a form of resilience.

Across mammals, Ahi1 expression is critical for normal cortical development. In humans, homozygous AHI1 mutations have been shown to impair neuronal migration, giving rise to aberrant cortical architecture.73 In mice, loss of Ahi1 affected early development by impairing neuronal differentiation22 and cortical axonal decussation.74 As major cortical inputs are implicated in associative learning, it is plausible to assume that physiological variation in Ahi1 expression during neurodevelopment exerts long-lasting effects on the efficiency of this cognitive domain. To this end, the findings obtained from the neuroinformatic analysis, suggesting significant correlations across the BXD RI strains between neocortical Ahi1 expression and fear conditioning to both context and cue, provide independent support for the data obtained from our Ahi1 knockout model.75

This neuroinformatic analysis also supports further insights. Our knockout mouse, much like other genetic models, provides a dichotomous system for exploring the effects of a gene’s altered expression compared with its ‘natural’ expression. Although utilization of such models may improve chances to reveal a given gene’s role, genetic variations that cause more subtle alterations of gene expression have also been shown to contribute to a wide range of neuropsychiatric disorders,76 including mood and anxiety dysregulation.77 Based on such findings, and in line with the conceptual approach underlying the recently proposed NIH Research Domain Criteria78 for psychiatric disorders, the correlations between naturally divergent Ahi1 expression and relevant behavioral traits across the BXD RI strains provide a dimensional rather than categorical approach for linking gene expression and behavior.

Although, as we have previously shown, the effect of heterozygous knockout of the Ahi1 gene on protein levels may be limited to the neonatal period, it could still exert long-term consequences, manifested by altered behavior and neural connectivity in adulthood.31 This observation is in line with previous data, revealing that in Ahi1-null mice morphological brain anomalies persist into adulthood despite near-normalization of Ahi1-related signal transduction pathways upon maturation of the brain.20 The current neuroinformatic analysis consolidates our previous results by highlighting that, although neocortical Ahi1 expression across strains could significantly predict the efficacy of fear learning in adulthood, the major contribution to this prediction came from the early postnatal expression profile, while the addition of the juvenile and adult profiles did not increase prediction strength. This finding is consistent with current neurodevelopmental theories, suggesting that aberrant expression during the early postnatal period is sufficient to set in motion a cascade of events, which leads to long-lasting changes in brain connectivity and altered anxiety-like behavior throughout life.79, 80

The QTL analysis utilizing the BXD RI strains, which revealed that a single QTL at the Chr1: 69.048453 locus modulated both early neocortical expression of Ahi1 and fear conditioning in adulthood, bolsters the significant expression–phenotype correlations noted above.75 Furthermore, through the identification of additional transcripts whose early neocortical expression is also tightly regulated by the same locus, further insights may be proposed. It has been previously shown that trans-eQTLs associated with expression of diverse genes can often be localized to common loci.81 Genes at these loci appear to control the expression of large numbers of downstream genes, suggesting that they act as ‘master modulatory loci’. In fact, a key master locus that modulates hundreds of brain transcripts has been identified within the Mtap2 locus, just 1.1 Mb upstream of Ahi1’s major trans-eQTL.82 Mtap2 contains at least seven missense polymorphisms between C57BL/6J and DBA/2J strains. As Mtap2 is a modestly cis-regulated transcript, these polymorphisms potentially have a causal role in regulating the transcripts mapping to the Chr1: 69.048453 locus during neurodevelopment. Another possibility is that the Chr1: 69.048453 polymorphism, located to ErbB4’s first intron, alters the latter’s splicing, thereby modulating the expression of relevant downstream genes. Such an idea seems exciting, given the mounting evidence linking dysregulated ErbB4 splicing with abnormal cortical development.83

Notably, irrespective of the exact genetic mechanism responsible for the Chr1: 69.048453 trans-regulation of key transcripts, the Gene Ontology categories over-represented among these transcripts include, besides neuron development and differentiation, also the organization of cell projections. Given Ahi1’s postulated role in organizing neural projections such as the primary cilium,15, 18, 19, 21, 84 it is plausible that a trans-acting element at, or adjacent to, the Chr1: 69.048453 locus modulates the expression of a set of genes involved in ciliogenesis. Finally, as this set of genes share suggestive and significant QTLs, other than at Chr1: 69.048453, both among themselves as well as with several fear conditioning traits, preliminary data supporting an association between early neocortical Ahi1 expression, ciliogenesis and fear learning during adulthood are provided.

The placement of brain connectivity as a bridge linking genetics and behavior has been increasingly encouraged.6, 78 Regarding Ahi1, on the one hand, Ahi1-dependent modulation of Wnt signaling19 is presumed to regulate neuronal migration, axonal development and synaptogenesis, ultimately shaping wiring of the brain.85 On the other hand, differential Ahi1 expression has been implicated in schizophrenia29 and autism30 in humans and in distinct cognitive-emotional deficits in mice,31, 59, 86 with convergent findings suggesting that these disorders are associated with specific disturbances of network connectivity.6 To this end, while using a seed-voxel analysis of resting-state fMRI data we previously demonstrated that Ahi1+/− mice display functional corticolimbic disconnectivity,31 a more comprehensive approach to study brain wiring would be to analyze the data in a form of network models.6 Thus brain connectivity can be abstracted to a graph of nodes, representing brain regions, linked by edges, representing functional interaction between nodes. Such a representation brings connectomic data into the realm of graph theory, affording a rich repertoire of mathematical tools and concepts that can be used to characterize diverse properties of brain networks.8 Specifically, quantifying network topology enables one to generate predictive models regarding the functional consequences attributed to the gene or disorder in question.9 Although utilization of graph theory to the study of resting-state functional networks in inbred mice has recently been reported using fMRI,87 to the best of our knowledge our current analysis provides the first reported implementation of this approach in the context of genetically modified mouse models.

Originally described in social networks, the ‘small-world’ property, a prominent measure of global network topology, combines high levels of local clustering with short path lengths that globally link all nodes of the network. Recent brain imaging studies have demonstrated that the human brain’s structural and functional networks have small-world properties,4 supporting the rapid real-time integration of information across segregated sensory brain regions. These features confer resilience against pathological attacks and maximize efficiency at a minimal cost for effective information processing between different brain regions.88 Although reports of resting-state functional networks in rodents are sparse, recent evidence from rats suggests that obtaining small-world coefficients may guarantee that the networks are indeed biologically plausible.89 In line with other recent reports,90, 91, 92 the current fMRI-based graph analysis provides evidence regarding the feasibility of generating resting-state functional networks with small-world properties within the mouse brain. Notably, both Ahi1+/+ and Ahi1+/− mice demonstrated small-world properties, most prominent at lower network densities. Nonetheless, small-worldness of brain networks has been a consistent observation, irrespective of illness status,93 suggesting that key aspects of brain organization are highly conserved even in the presence of neurodevelopmental disorders with profound effects on complex brain functions.94

Our observation that in Ahi1+/+ mice whole-brain resting-state functional networks tend to be hierarchical corresponds well with previous studies in humans.95 The hubs of hierarchical networks are highly connected but have low clustering; they are mainly connected to nodes that are not otherwise connected to each other. This architecture supports highly efficient information processing and executive-report relationships between nodes while conserving wiring costs.47 Moreover, the weighted connectivity graphs and adjacency matrices imply that reciprocal relationships between cortical and non-cortical regions, manifested as negative (anti) correlations, may be more prominent in the hierarchical configuration. In line with recently published data,96, 97 these findings suggest that hierarchical topology may be associated with a ‘division of labor’ between several networks. Thus the differential network organization of Ahi1+/− mice, which varied predominantly between non- and dis-hierarchical topologies and lacked significant negative correlations, could underlie their underperformance in tasks relying on efficient information processing. Remarkably, similar findings suggesting lower hierarchy of structural cortical networks have recently been attributed to patients with schizophrenia,94 a putatively neurodevelopmental disorder marked by impairments in cognition and information processing, and also associated with AHI1 polymorphisms.24, 25

The hubs of an assortative network are highly connected to each other. Compared with non-assortative topology, this organization offers robustness against random or targeted removal of hubs.46 Our results, showing that on average individual assortativity coefficients in Ahi1+/− mice were significantly higher than in Ahi1+/+ mice, parallel findings in schizophrenia patients contrasted with healthy volunteers.94 These results could imply that functional brain networks in Ahi1+/− mice are more resilient when facing pathological attacks. Although in graph theory the term ‘attacks’ refers to the removal of vertices, acute environmental stress is often seen as an ethologically valid form of a ‘behavioral attack’.98 Should this conceptual gap between ‘attack’ nuances in the graph theory vs behavioral fields prove negotiable, it could be argued that the Ahi1+/− mouse displays a multi-faceted resilient phenotype, manifested in terms of brain network topology as well as behavior. Notably, individual differences in personality traits have recently been associated with topological metrics of whole-brain functional networks. Specifically, the cortical networks of extraverts suggested a configuration that favors a higher arousal threshold and higher levels of arousal tolerance.99 Finally, the weighted graphs imply that in the assortative configuration which characterizes the Ahi1+/− mouse, mesolimbic connections could possibly emerge at the expense of long-distance functional connections. Notably, such a trade-off has been implicated in the pathogenesis of brain disorders that are associated with metabolic distress.88

Although global network measures capture important characteristics related to overall efficiency and robustness, it was recently argued that they should be supplemented by the more fine-grained local measures of network topology.100 To this end, the data suggesting that functional connectivity of the SC, as assessed by the node-degree parameter, is significantly altered in mice underexpressing the Ahi1 gene, complement the differences observed in global network topology. These midbrain structures have a major role in processing information necessary for spatial locomotion and sensory-guided navigation,101 and are also involved in the modulation of avoidance and escape behavior,102 all of which are highly relevant components during the swim-to-visible platform task. In this regard, the striking differences between our functional model and wild-type controls in terms of escape latency and thigmotaxis (avoidance of open places), when presented with an immediate sense of danger, could theoretically be explained by a differential layout of visual maps that is caused by subtle differences in Ahi1 expression. Although this speculation obviously needs further validation using direct in vivo recordings, it seems highly attractive, given the recent compelling data implicating a SC-mediated orientation bias in computational processes related to avoidance and escape maneuvers. Specifically, following the hypothesis generated by Ahmadlou and Heimel,60 it could be argued that the superior performance displayed by the Ahi1+/− mouse in locating the escape platform could be the result of altered gaze towards, and/or detection of, critical visual stimuli located in the arena center, while the mouse engages in thigmotactic radial motion. To this end, and although direct evidence linking Ahi1 expression with layering of the SC has not been examined, preliminary support for such a role could be drawn from data based on a different Ahi1-related functional model, which suggests that in utero induction of Ahi1 overexpression leads to abnormal cortical layering (unpublished data).

The current work uses different methodological approaches to unravel the neurodevelopmental mechanisms that are under study. Thus behavioral experiments in rodents are complemented by functional imaging and graph-theory-based analysis. Moreover, we took advantage of comprehensive online data sets to seek support for some of our key findings.103 Thereby, we were able to corroborate and extend our original functional data. Moreover, the approach employed in the construction of large expression data sets, such as those provided by GeneNetwork,39 treats gene expression as a continuous variable across RI strains, rather than as a categorical one (knockout model). Hence, we believe that using these complementary, yet conceptually distinct, approaches enhanced our ability to propose mechanistic insights.

A limitation of the current study relates to the non-trivial relationship between structural and functional brain connectivity.4 Structural networks represent anatomical wiring diagrams. In contrast, functional networks represent patterns of activity correlations that do not necessarily coincide with direct neuronal communication. Although assuming a role for Ahi1 in emerging structural connections seems relatively straightforward, given its postulated role in neuronal migration and primary cilia development, the current graph-based analysis is based on measures of functional connectivity. Nonetheless, convergent findings indicate that the brain’s structural and functional networks are intimately related and share common topological features.4, 96 Moreover, Ahi1 could also directly modulate the strengths of functional connections through its putative roles in ciliogenesis84 and Wnt-mediated synaptogenesis.104 Supporting this hypothesis, recent evidence suggests that loss of Ahi1 impairs neurotransmitter release.59 Future studies conducting topological comparisons between anatomical and functional connections will help delineate the mechanisms through which Ahi1 affects wiring of the brain. Another possible shortcoming related to the generation of functional networks is the relatively small number of nodes used. However, while networks that are comprised of hundreds, or even thousands, of nodes may be ideal for quantifying global metrics and for the detection of subtle between-group differences, basic configurations can also be readily visualized in networks comprised of only several dozens of nodes.47, 94 Nevertheless, replication of our findings using larger networks is warranted and should be made feasible in the near future given the tremendous advances in imaging technology. Finally, the use of isoflurane anesthesia during acquisition of functional data has recently been debated. However, a recent report suggested that, even though local functional properties could become reorganized under isoflurane anesthesia in rats, global topological features were preserved.105 Moreover, another recent report indicating that the use of isoflurane does not mask network information43 bolsters the validity of our methodology.

A major limitation of the current study is the lack of direct functional data assessing the long-term effects of reduced Ahi1 expression in specific brain regions and during specific developmental stages. Investigating the long-term effects exerted by temporal expression profiles of specific genes has been the focus of several studies employing heterozygous knockout models for neurodevelopmental genes. For instance, mice with heterozygous knockout of GAP-43, a gene involved in some forms of neural plasticity,106 display a significant decrease in the corresponding protein levels in newborns, with a gradual convergence to near-normal protein levels in later life.107 In line with this observation, we had previously shown that in our current functional model, Ahi1 protein levels in the adult mouse brain did not differ between wild types and Ahi1 heterozygous knockouts.31 However, in neonates, when Ahi1 expression is presumably at its peak,15, 74 protein levels were significantly lower in the Ahi1+/− mice. Thus we argue that our model does recapitulate, albeit to a limited extent, the ‘essence’ of temporal conditionality, as the striking differences in both network topology and behavior during adulthood could be demonstrated (long) after the initial differences in Ahi1 expression had already subsided. Moreover, while manipulating Ahi1 expression across several regions would be a most informative approach to delineating the spatial roles of Ahi1, we argue that the data suggesting that functional connectivity of the SC is significantly altered in mice underexpressing the Ahi1 gene provide additional evidence linking alterations in Ahi1 expression with spatial phenotypes and possibly also with visually based information processing and behavior. Nonetheless, future generation of spatiotemporal conditional knockout models is highly warranted, as it will likely stimulate new insights into Ahi1-related cognitive and emotional developmental trajectories.

Finally, although we have attempted to depict a cascade linking gene expression, brain wiring and behavior through multiple lines of evidence, a phenotype at the neuronal level could provide another important link. However, while full Ahi1 knockout results in striking histological phenotypes,20 in our mouse model, which is based on moderate reduction of Ahi1 expression, we expect neuronal phenotypes to be subtle. To this end, and given Ahi1’s postulated role in the assembly of subcellular components such as the primary cilium,19 together with the data from the eQTL-based enrichment analysis, electron microscopic studies in neuronal cultures could shed valuable light on further links in this developmental cascade.

A central idea emanating from the current work is that physiological variation in the expression of genes that are co-regulated and function together during neurodevelopment may eventually be translated into distinct intermediate phenotypes along a continuum of metrics depicting network topology. Within this framework, it seems that the brain’s connectome may not be optimized to maximize advantageous topological properties,88 such as hierarchy or assortativity. Instead, we propose that brain network organization could be the result of an adaptive trade-off between efficiency of the brain network and its resilience. Ultimately, and possibly a consequence of the former, a compound behavioral phenotype along a cognitive-emotional continuum may crystallize, expressing varying levels of cognitive performance and stress resilience. Extrapolating from this hypothesis, and in line with recent theoretical models,88 it could be proposed that impairments in cognitive function and/or emotional regulation that characterize neuropsychiatric disorders may reflect genetically mediated effects on those global properties of brain networks that are crucial for integrative processing and adaptive behavior.