Skip to main content
Log in

Fostering Intrinsic Motivation in Early Childhood Classrooms

  • Published:
Early Childhood Education Journal Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Young children are born with an innate curiosity to learn about their world. This intrinsically instigated learning is often called mastery motivation. Patterns of motivation are established at an early age. The early childhood years are crucial for establishing robust intrinsic motivational orientations which will last a lifetime. By the time many children reach school, much of their motivation has been lost or replaced with extrinsically motivated learning strategies. Preschools and elementary schools have been criticized for contributing to such negative motivational patterns in children. This can be changed. Early child care situations and preschools can instead be instrumental in the strengthening of children's motivation. The goal of this paper is to show that through an understanding of the beginnings of motivation, we can begin to find ways to build strong motivational patterns in children that can carry on to later years of learning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, K. C., & Morgan, G. A. (1995). Continuities and discontinuities in mastery motivation during infancy and toddlerhood. A conceptualization and review. In R. H. MacTurk & G. A. Morgan (Eds.) Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualizations and applications (pp. 57–94). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belsky, J., Goode, M. K., & Most, R. K. (1980). Maternal stimulation and infant exploratory competence: Cross-sectional, correlational, and experimental analysis. Child Development, 51, 1163–1178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berk, L. E., & Winsler, A. (1995). Scaffolding children's learning: Vygotsky and early childhood education. Washington, DC: National Association for the Education of Young Children.

    Google Scholar 

  • Busch-Rossnagel, N. A., Knauf-Jensen, D. E., & DesRosiers, F. S. (1995). Mothers and others: The role of the socializing environment in the development of mastery motivation. In R. H. MacTurk & G. A. Morgan (Eds.), Mastery motivation: Origins, conceptualizations and applications (pp. 117–145). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cameron, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1994). Reinforcement, reward, and intrinsic motivation: A meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 39, 363–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeCasper, A. J., & Carstens, A. A. (1981). Contingencies of stimulation: Effects on learning and emotion in neonates. Infant Behavior and Development, 4, 19–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1024–1037.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deci, E. L., Vallerand, R. J., Pelletier, L. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). Motivation and education: The self-determination perspective. Educational Psychologist, 26, 325–346.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. E., & Thompson, R. A. (1985). Perceptions of personal agency and infant attachment: Toward a life-span perspective on competence development. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 8, 377–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frodi, A., Bridges, L., & Grolnick, W. (1985). Correlates of mastery-related behavior: A short-term longitudinal study of infants in their second year. Child Development, 56, 1291–1298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottfried, A. E. (1985). Academic intrinsic motivation in elementary and junior high school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 631–645.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar, M. R., & Stone, C. (1984). The effects of positive maternal affect on infant responses to pleasant, ambiguous, and fear-provoking toys. Child Development, 55, 1231–1236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered: Toward a developmental model. Human Development, 21, 34–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrickson, N. J., & Hansen, S. L. (1977). Toddlers: Competence and behavior patterns. Child Study Journal, 7, 79–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jennings, K. D., Harmon, R. J., Morgan, G. A., Gaiter, J. L., & Yarrow, L. J. (1979). Exploratory play as an index of mastery motivation: Relationships to persistence, cognitive functioning, and environmental measures. Developmental Psychology, 15, 386–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R. (1983). Extrinsic reward and intrinsic motivation: Implications for the classroom. In J. M. Levine & M. C. Wang (Eds.) Teacher and student perceptions: Implications for learning (pp. 281–317). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermining children's intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test of the over-justification hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1990). Violation of expectancy, loss of control, and anger expressions in young infants. Developmental Psychology, 26, 745–751.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., Alessandri, S. M., & Sullivan, M. W. (1992). Differences in shame and pride as a function of children's gender and task difficulty. Child Development, 63, 630–638.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Goldberg, S. (1969). Perceptual-cognitive development in infancy: A generalized expectancy model as a function of the mother-infant interaction. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 15, 81–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R., & Schunk, D. H., (1996). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pratt, M. W., Kerig, P., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (1988). Mothers and fathers teaching 3-year-olds: Authoritative parenting and adult scaffolding of young children's learning. Developmental Psychology, 24, 832–839.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rovee-Collier, C. K. (1987). Learning and memory. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (2nd ed., pp. 98–148). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, H. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (1996). Attention in early development: Themes and variations. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, R. M., & Powelson, C. L. (1991). Autonomy and relatedness as fundamental to motivation and education. Journal of Experimental Education, 60, 49–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shields, P. J., & Rovee-Collier, C. K. (1992). Long-term memory for context-specific category information at six months. Child Development, 63, 245–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, M. A. (1996). Impact of motivational climate on students' behaviors and perceptions in a physical education setting. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 731–738.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. J. (1988). Motivation to learn: From theory to practice. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Farrar, M. J. (1986). Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 1454–1463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wachs, T. D. (1987). Specificity of environmental action as manifest in environmental correlates of infants' mastery motivation. Developmental Psychology, 23, 782–790.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J. S., & Ramey, C. T. (1972). Reactions to response-contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 219–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winsler, A., & Diaz, R. M. (1995). Private speech in the classroom: The effects of activity type, presence of others, classroom context, and mixed-age grouping. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 18, 463–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winsler, A., Diaz, R. M., & Montero, I. (in press). The role of private speech in the transition from collaboration to independent task performance in young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly.

  • Yarrow, L. J., MacTurk, R. H., Vietze, P. M., McCarthy, M. E., Klein, R. P., & McQuiston, S. (1984). Developmental course of parental stimulation and its relationship to mastery motivation during infancy. Developmental Psychology, 20, 492–503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yarrow, L. J., Rubenstein, J. L., Pedersen, F. A., & Jankowski, J. J. (1972). Dimensions of early stimulation and their differential effects on infant behavior. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 205–218.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Carlton, M.P., Winsler, A. Fostering Intrinsic Motivation in Early Childhood Classrooms. Early Childhood Education Journal 25, 159–166 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025601110383

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025601110383

Navigation