Skip to main content
Log in

Comorbidity Between Autistic Syndrome and Biological Pathologies: Which Implications for the Understanding of the Etiology?

  • Published:
Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

It is estimated that at least 24% of cases of autism are potentially associated with other syndromes, to infective, metabolic or genetic pathologies, and to anatomical or functional alterations. Problems connected with interpretation of the association between biological damage and autistic behavioral phenotype in causal terms are briefly exposed. Three critical periods for the beginning of an anomalous behavioral development in the child with autism are identified: prenatal, prior to 9 months from birth, after 1 year of age. Psychopathological consequences potentially deriving from biological damages on every critical period are described, and their compatibility with the characteristic behavioral features observed in individuals affected by autism is discussed. On this basis, a theoretical model that represents infantile autism as a specie-specific human psychopathology of early emotional communication during the primary intersubjectivity stage is proposed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adrien, J. L., Lenoir, P., Martineau, J., and Perrot, A. (1993). Blind ratings of early symptoms of autism based upon family home movies. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 32: 617-626.

    Google Scholar 

  • APA (1994). DSM IV. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn., American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Attwood, A., Frith, U., and Hermelin, B. (1988). The understanding and use of interpersonal gesture by autistic and Down's syndrome children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 18(2): 241-257.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1989). Joint attention deficits in autism: Towards a cognitive analysis. Dev. Psychopathol. 1: 185-189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). The theory of mind deficit in autism: How specific is it? Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 9: 301-314.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartak, L., Rutter, M., and Cox, A. (1975). A comparative study of infantile autism and specific developmental receptive language disorders: I—the children. Br. J. Psychiatry 126: 127-145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barton, M., and Volkmar, F. (1998). How commonly are known medical conditions associated with autism? J. Autism Dev. Disord. 28(4): 273-278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1976). Language and Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics Academic Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bergman, P., and Escalona, S. K. (1949). Psychoanal. Study Child 3-4:333-343. Quoted from Ornitz and Naruse (1992).

  • Bristol-Power, M. M., and Spinella, G. (1999). Research on screening and diagnosis in autism: A work in progress. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29(6): 435-438.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech acts. J. Child Lang. 2: 1-19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryson, S. E., Clark, B. S., and Smith, I. M. (1988). First report of a Canadian epidemiological study of autistic Syndromes. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 29: 433-445.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryson, S., Smith, I., and Eastwood, D. (1989). Obstetrical optimality in autistic children. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 27: 418-422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Kasari, C., Yirmiya, N., and Sigman, M. (1993). Parental perception of emotional expressiveness in children with autism. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 61(3): 475-484.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capps, L., Yirmiya, N., and Sigman, M. (1992). Understanding of simple and complex emotions in non-retarded children with autism. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 19: 351-362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carter, C. S., Ledehendler, I. I., and Kirkpatrick, B. (1997). The integrative neurobiology of affiliation. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 807: xiii-xviii.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castello, P., and Dawson, G. (1993). Subclassification of children with autism and pervasive developmental disorders: A questionnaire based on Wing's subgrouping scheme. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 23: 225-237.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G. (1996). L'autismo tra eziologia multipla e multifattorialità: una panoramica critica sui modelli esplicativi. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia XXIII(4): 617-643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G. (in press). Human beings, animals and inanimate objects: What do people with autism like? Autism 6(1).

  • Celani, G., and Arcidiacono, L. (2000). Autistic People Propensity Towards Interpersonal Relationship, International Congress of Autism-Europe, Glasgow, pp. 19-21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G., Arcidiacono, L., Cavina, C., Codispoti, M., and Battacchi, M. W. (1998). La rappresentazione verbale delle reazioni emotive nei soggetti con comportamento autistico. In Scopesi, A., and Zanobini, M. (Eds.), Processi comunicativi e linguistici nei bambini e negli adulti, Franco Angeli, Milano.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., and Arcidiacono, L. (1999a). The understanding of the emotional meaning of facial expressions in people with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 29(1): 57-66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., Arcidiacono, L., and Di Domenico, M. (1999b). Valutazione di volti con espressione felice o triste nelle persone con autismo. Psicologia Clinica dello Sviluppo III(2): 213-240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G., Codispoti, O., and Giuberti, V. (2000). Capacità imitative ed espressività emotiva facciale nelle persone con autismo. Età Evolutiva 65: 46-58.

    Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G., Scalembra, S., Colace, C., and Battacchi, M. W. (1998). L'attenzione condivisa nei soggetti autistici: quali specificità comportamentali? Psychofenia I(2): 69-88.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chess, S. (1971). Autism in children with congenital rubella. J. Autism Child. Schizophr. 1: 33-47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, M. L. (1993a). Clinical review: Medical differential diagnosis and treatment of the autistic syndrome. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2(3): 161-168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coleman, M. L. (1993b). Second trimester of gestation: A time of risk for clinical autism. In 3rd Consensus Conference on Biological Basis and Clinical Perspectives in Autism. Dev. Brain Dysfunction 6: 343-361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Curcio, F. (1978). Sensorimotor functioning and communication in mute autistic children. J. Autism Child. Schizophr. 2: 264-287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Warrenburg, S., and Fuller, P. (1983). Hemisphere functioning and motor imitation in autistic persons. Brain Cogn. 2: 346-354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delacato, C. M. (1974). The Ultimate Stranger, Doubleday, Garden City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eaves, L. C., Ho, H. H., and Eaves, D. M. (1994). Subtypes of autism by cluster analysis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24: 3-22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, G. R., and Ciaranello, R. D. (1987). Neurochemical hypothesis of childhood psychoses. In Schopler, E. and Mesibov, G. B. (Eds.), Neurobiological Issues in Autism, Plenum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fombonne, E., and du Mazaubrun, C. (1992). Prevalence of infantile autism in four French regions. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 27(4): 203-210.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism Explaining the Enigma, Basil Blackwell, Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, G. (1984). Infantile autism and other childhood psychoses in a Swedish region: Epidemiological aspects. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 25: 35-43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C. (1989). The borderland of autism and Rett syndrome: Five case histories to highlight diagnostic difficulties. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 19(4): 545-559.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C. (1993). Autism and related behaviors. J. Intellect. Disab. Res. 37: 343-372.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., and Gillberg, I. C. (1983). Infantile autism: A total population study of reduced optimality in the pre-, peri-, and neonatal period. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 13: 153-166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., and Gillberg, I. C. (1991). Note on the relationship between population-based and clinical studies: The question of reduced optimality in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 21(2): 251-259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., and Coleman, M. (1992). The Biology of the Qutistic Syndrome, Cambridge University Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., and Coleman, M. (1996). Autism and medical disorders: A review of the literature. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 38: 191-202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillberg, C., and Rassmussen, P. (1994). Four case histories and a literature review of Williams syndrome and autistic behavior. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24(3): 381-393.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldfarb, W. (1961). Childhood Schizophrenia, Harvard University press, Cambridge, MA. Quoted from Ornitz and Naruse (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldfarb, W. (1963). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 8: 47-60. Quoted from Ornitz and Naruse (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenspan, S. I. (1997). Infancy and Early Childhood, International University Press, Madison.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F., and Harlow, M. K. (1969). Effects of various mother–infant relationships on rhesus monkey behaviors. In Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of Infants Behaviors, Vol. 4, Methuen, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hashimoto, T., Tamaya, M., Miyazaki, M., Murakawa, K., Shimakawa, S., Yoneda, Y., and Kuroda, Y. (1993). Brainstem involvement in high functioning autistic children. Acta Neurol. Scand. 88: 123-128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1989). Beyond cognition. A theory of Autism. In Dawson, G. (ed.), Autism Nature, Diagnosis and Treatment, Guilford Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1991). Methodological issues for experiments on autistic individuals' perception and understanding of emotion. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 32: 1135-1158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1993). Autism and the Development of Mind, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hove East Sussex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., and Lee, A. (1989). Emotion-related and abstracts concepts in autistic people: Evidence from the British Pictures Vocabolary Scale. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 19: 601-623.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howlin, P., Wing, L., and Gould, J. (1995). The recognition of autism in children with Down syndrome. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 37(5): 406-414.

    Google Scholar 

  • ICDL (2000). Clinical Practice Guidelines of the International Council on Developmental and Learning Disorders, ICDL Press, Bethesda.

    Google Scholar 

  • Innerhofer, P., and Klicpera, C. (1984). Diagnostik des frulikindlichen Autisms. Fruliforderung interdisciplinar 3(4): 165-171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivarsson, S. A., Bjerre, I., Vegfors, P., and Ahlfors, K. (1990). Autism as one of several disabilities in two children with congenital cytomegalovirus infection. Neuropediatrics 21: 102-103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, V., and Prior, M. P. (1985). Motor imitation abilities and neurological signs in autistic children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 15: 37-46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child 2: 217-250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kasary, C., Sigman, M., Mundy, P., and Yirmiya, N. (1990). Affective sharing in the context of joint attention interactions of normal, autistic and mentally retarded children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 20: 87-100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kent, L., Evans, J., Paul, M., and Sharp, M. (1999). Comorbidity of autistic spectrum disorders in children with Down syndrome. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 41(3): 153-158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A. (1991). Young autistic children's listening preference in regard to speech: A possible characterization of the symptom of social withdrawal. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 21: 29-42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kling, A. S., and Brothers, L. A. (1992). The Annygdala and social belavior. In Aggleton, J. P. (ed.), The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction, Wiley-Liss, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamendella, J. T. (1977). General principles of neurofunctional organization and their manifestation in primary and non primary language acquisition. Lang. Learn. 27: 155-196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landry, S., and Loveland, K. (1988). Communication behaviours in autism and developmental language delay. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 29(5): 621-634.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langdell, T. (1981). Face perception. An approach to the study of autism. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of London, England. Quoted from Attwood et al. (1988).

  • Le Doux, J. E. (1987). Emotion. In Plum, F. (ed.), Handbook of Physiology: Section 1. The Nervous System, Vol. 5. American Physiological Society, Bethesda, MD.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Doux, J. E. (1989). Cognitive–emotional interactions in the brain. Cogn. Emot. 3: 267-289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Doux, J. E. (1992). Emotion and the Amygdala. In Aggleton, J. P. (ed.), The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction, Wiley-Liss, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lelord, G., Hameury, S., and Roux, S. (1990). Una classificazione dimensionale e categoriale dell'autismo e dei disturbi generalizzati dello sviluppo: la classificazione multiassiale bioclinica. In Lelord, G. and Sauvage, D. (Eds.), L'Autisme de l'Enfant, Masson, Paris. (Trad. it. L'Autismo Infantile. Masson, Milano, 1994.)

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origin of ‘theory of mind.’ Psycholo. Rev. 94: 412-426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lotter, V. (1966). Epidemiology of autistic condition in young children: I—Prevalence. Soc. Psychiatry 1: 124-137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K., and Landry, S. (1986). Joint attention and communication in autism and language delay. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1: 335-349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Pearson, D. A., Brelsfort, K. A., Ortegon, J., and Chen, R. (1994). Imitation and expression of facial affect in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 6: 433-444.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKenna, J. J., and Mosoko, S. (1994). Sleep and arousal, synchrony and independence among mothers and infants sleeping apart and together (same bed): An experiment in evolutionary medicine. Acta Paediatr. Suppl. 397: 94-102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., and Kasari, C. (1993). The theory of mind and joint attention deficit in autism: The contribution of non-verbal communication. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 27(5): 657-669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., Ugerer, J., and Sherman, T. (1986). Defining the social deficits of autism: The contribution of non verbal communication measures. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 27(5): 657-669.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nissen, H. W. (1951). Phylogenetic comparision. In Stevens, S. S. (ed.), Hand Book of Experimental Psychology, Wiley, New York, pp. 346-385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ohta, M. (1987). Cognitive disorders of infantile autism: A study employing the WISC, spatial relationship conceptualization and gesture imitations. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 17: 45-62.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ollson, B., and Rett, A. (1985). Behavioral observations concerning differential diagnosis between the Rett syndrome and autism. Brain Dev. 7: 281-289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olsson, B., and Rett, A. (1988). Autism and Rett syndrome: Behavioral investigations and differential diagnosis. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 29(4): 429-441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornitz, E. M. (1973). Cal. Med. 118:21-47. Quoted from Ornitz and Naruse (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornitz, E. M. (1974). J. Autism Child. Schizophr. 4:197-215. Quoted from Ornitz and Naruse (1992).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ornitz, E. M., and Naruse, M. (1992). Introduction to biological research on infantile autism. In Ornitz, E. M. and Naruse, M. (Eds.), Neurobiology of Infantile Autism, Elsevier Science, Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff, S., Pennington, B. F., and Rogers, S. J. (1991). Executive function deficits in high-functioning autistic individuals: Relationship to theory of mind. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 32: 1081-1105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J., Nelson, E., and Bekkedal, M. (1997). Brain systems for the mediation of the separation distress and social reward: Evolutionary antecedents and neuropeptide intermediaries. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 807: 78-100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prior, M. R. (1977). Psycholinguistic disabilities of autistic and retarded children. J. Ment. Def. Res. 21: 37-45.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richer, J. M. (1978). The partial non-communication of culture to autistic children. In Rutter, M., and Schopler, E. (Eds.), Autism: A Reappraisal of Concepts and Treatment, Plenum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1978). Diagnosis and definition. In Rutter, M. and Schopler, E. (Eds.), Autism: A Reappraisal of Concepts and Treatment, Plenum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. (1983). Cognitive deficits in the pathogenesis of autism. J. Child Psychiatry 24(4): 513-531.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., and Bartak, L. (1971). Causes of infantile autism: Some considerations from recent research. J. Autism Child. Schizophr. 1: 20-32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Short, A. B., and Shopler, E. (1988). Factors relating to age of onset in Autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 18: 207-216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, B., Anders, T. F., Ciaraniello, R. D., Bienenstock, B., and Kramer, H. C. (1986). Empirically derived subclassification of the autistic syndrome. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 16: 275-294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M. (1993). Identifying the Core Deficitin Autism. Abstr. 3201 Consensus Conference on Biological Basis and Clinical Perspective in Autism. Dev. Brain Dysfunction 6: 343-361.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigman, M., and Ungerer, J. (1984). Cognitive and language skills in autistic, mantally retarded, and normal children. Dev. Psychol. 20: 293-302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smalley, S. L., Asarnow, R. F., and Spence, A. (1988). Autism and genetics: A decade of research. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 45: 953-961.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smalley, S. L., Tanguay, P. E., Smith, M., and Gutierrez, G. (1992). Autism and toberous sclerosis. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 22(3): 339-355.

    Google Scholar 

  • Srinath, S., Chowdhury, J., Bhide, A. V., and Narayanan, H. S. (1989). Descriptive study of infantile autism. Nimhans J.: 7(1): 77-81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steffenburg, S., and Gillberg, C. (1989). The etiology of autism. In Gillberg, C. (ed.), Diagnosis and Treatment of Autism, Plenum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., Lemanek, K. L., Fishel, P. T., Ferdandez, M. C., and Altmeir, W. A. (1990). Play and imitation skills in the diagnosis of autism in young children. Pediatrics 86: 267-272.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stubbs, E. G., Ash, E., and Williams, C. P. (1984). Autism and congenital cytomegalovirus infection. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 14: 183-189.

    Google Scholar 

  • Suomi, S. J. (1997). Early determinants of behavior: Evidence from primate studies. British Med. Bull. 53(1): 170-184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szamatari, P. (1992). The validity of the autistic spectrum disorders: A literature review. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 22: 583-600.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Fluesberg, H. (1985). Basic level and superordinate level categorization in autistic, mentally retarded, and normal children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 40: 450-469.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H. (1989). An analysis of discourse ability and internal state lexicons in a longitudinal study of autistic children. In Biennal Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Kansas City, April 1989.

  • Tanoue, Y., Oda, S., Asano, F., and Kawashima, K. (1988). Epidemiology of infantile autism in southern Ibaraky, Japan: Differences in prevalence rates in birth cohorts. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 18: 155-166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thaut, M. H. (1987). Visual versus auditory (musical) stimulus preference in children with autism: A pilot study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 17: 425-432.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In Bullow, M. (ed.), Before Speech: The Beginnings of Human Communication, Cambridge University Press, London, pp. 321-347.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1985). Neuroembriology and the development of perceptual mechanisms. In Falkner, F. and Tonner, J. M. (Eds.), Human Growth. A Comprensive Theatre, Plenum, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., Aitken, K., Papondi, D., and Robarts, J. (1996). Children With Autism, Jessica Kinsley, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., and Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiders and acts of meaning in the first year. In Lock, A. (ed.), Action, Gesture and Symbol, Academic Press, New York, pp. 183-229.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, J., and Sigman, M. (1987). Categorization skills and receptive language development in autistic children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 17: 3-16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A. (1985). L'imitation chez le nouveau-né, Delachaux et Niestlé, Paris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., Cohen, D. J., and Paul, R. (1986). An evaluation of DSM-III criteria for infantile autism. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 25: 190-191.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wetherby, A. M., and Prutting, C. A. (1984). Profiles of communicative and cognitive–social abilities in autistic children. J. Speech Hear. Res. 27: 364-377.

    Google Scholar 

  • WHO (1992). ICD-10. International Statistical Classification of Disease, Injures and Causes of Death, 10th rev., World Health Organization, Geneva.

    Google Scholar 

  • Windle, W. F. (1970). Development of neural elements in human embryos of four to seven weeks gestation. Exp. Neurol. 5: 44-83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L. (1976). Epidemiology and theories of aetiology. In Wing, L. (ed.), Early Childhood Autism: Clinical, Educational and Social Aspects, Pergamon Press, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wing, L., and Gould, L. (1979). Severe impairments of social interaction and associated abnormalities in children: Epidemiology and classification. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 9: 11-29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamagami, M. (1974). Characteristics of emotional behavior of autistic children: Clarification of the term ‘autistic’. Jap. J. Child Psychiatry 15(3): 132-144.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Kasari, C., Sigman, M., and Mundy, P. (1989). Facial expression of affect in autistic, mentally retarded and normal children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 30: 725-735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yirmiya, N., Sigman, M., Kasary, C., and Mundy, P. (1992). Empathy and cognition in high-functioning children with autism. Child Dev. 48: 555-562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zappella, M. (1996). Autismo infantile. Studi sull'affettività e le emozioni, La Nuova Italia Scientifica, Roma.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Celani, G. Comorbidity Between Autistic Syndrome and Biological Pathologies: Which Implications for the Understanding of the Etiology?. Journal of Developmental and Physical Disabilities 15, 141–154 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022875300575

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022875300575

Navigation