Skip to main content
Log in

The role of naming speed within a model of reading acquisition

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

That symbol naming speed is an importantcorrelate of reading skill has been generallyacknowledged. Just what contribution it makesand why is a much more difficult question. Wesuggest that the search for such answers isbest developed within a broad model of readingskill acquisition. We propose an informalmodel and review evidence for several of itslinks. The major lines of influence arehypothesized to flow from cognitive abilities(mediated by instructional factors) tointermediate and final reading outcomes. Theoutcomes at each level, however, are affectedby outcomes at other levels, and in other wayscombine to complicate the picture. Use of sucha model may focus our research questions morefinely and lead to a more preciseconceptualization of the basis for naming speed– reading relationships.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1981). What good is orthographic redundancy? In: O.J.L. Tzeng & H. Singer (eds.), Perception of print: Reading research in experimental psychology (pp. 197-221). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to Read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, T.A., Torgesen, J.K. & Wagner, R.K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks, Reading Research Quarterly 27: 334-345.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourassa, D.C., Levy, B.A., Dowin, S. & Casey, A. (1998). Transfer effects across contextual and linguistic boundaries: Evidence from poor readers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 71: 45-61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. (1993). Text reading and rereading: Predictors of fluency beyond word recognition, Journal of Reading Behavior 25: 133-153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. (1995a). A speculative account of several factors affecting the development of orthographic skill. Paper presented to the AERA, San Francisco.

  • Bowers, P.G. (1995b). Tracing symbol naming speed's unique contributions to reading disabilities over time, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 189-216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. (1995c). Implications for later reading of a naming speed deficit accompanying a phonemic awareness deficit. Paper presented to the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, San Francisco.

  • Bowers, P. (1996). The effects of single and double deficits in phonemic awareness and naming speed on new tests of orthographic knowledge. Paper presented to meetings of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, New York City.

  • Bowers, P.G., Golden, J.O., Kennedy, A. & Young, A. (1994). Limits upon orthographic knowledge due to processes indexed by naming speed. In: V.W. Berninger (ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 173-218). Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G. & Kennedy, A. (1993). Effects of naming speed differences on fluency of reading after practice, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 682: 318-320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G., Sunseth, K. & Golden, J. (1999). The route between rapid naming and reading progress, Scientific Studies of Reading 3: 31-53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P., Sunseth, K. & Newby-Clark, E. (April, 1998). Parametric explorations of single and double deficits in rapid naming and phonemic awareness in Grade 3. Poster presented to meetings of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, San Diego, California.

  • Bowers, P.G. & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links between naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 5: 69-85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1995). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 2-and 3-year follow-up and a new preschool trial, Journal of Educational Psychology 87: 488-503.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcos, E. & Willows, D.M. (1993a). The processing of orthographic information. In: D.M. Willows, R.S. Kruk & E. Corcos (eds.), Visual Processes in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 163-190). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcos, E. & Willows, D.M. (1993b). The role of visual processes in good and poor readers' utilization of orthographic information in letter strings. In: S. Wright & R. Groner (eds.), Studies in visual information processing: Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp. 95-106). Amsterdam: Elsevier, North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A.E. & Stanovich, K.E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience, Journal of EducationalPsychology 82: 733-740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M.B. & Rudel, R.G. (1974). 'Rapid automatized naming' of pictured objects, colors, letters, and numbers by normal children, Cortex 10: 186-202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In: P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri & R. Treiman (eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107-143). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 7: 295-326.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farmer, M.E. & Klein, R.M. (1995). The evidence for a temporal processing deficit linked to dyslexia: A review, Psychonomic Society 2: 460-493.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R.H. & Brown, I.S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 39-59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golden, J.O. (1997). Analysis of orthographic knowledge and its relationship to naming speed, phonological awareness, and single word identification. Dissertation presented to the University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario.

  • Kail, R. & Hall, L.K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading, Developmental Psychology 30: 949-954.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, A.B., Abello, B. & Lysynchuk, L. (1997). Transfer from word training to reading in context: Gains in reading fluency and comprehension, Learning Disabilities Quarterly 20: 173-188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B.A. & Bourassa, D. (1998). Slow versus fast namers: Benefits of segmentation and whole word training. Poster presented to meetings of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, San Diego, California.

  • Lovett, M.W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill, Child Development 58: 234-260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. & Steinbach, K. (1998). Remediating the core deficits of developmental reading disability: A double-deficit perspective. Paper submitted for publication.

  • Manis, F., Doi, L. & Bhadha (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness and orthographic knowledge in second graders, Journal of Learning Disabilities 33: 325-333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morris, R., Lovett, M. & Wolf, M. (1996). Treatment of developmental reading disabilities.

  • National Institute for Child Health and Human Development grant proposal.

  • Newby-Clark, E.D. & Bowers, P.G. (1998). Rapid naming skills and transfer of rime knowledge. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Olson, R.K., Kliegl, R., Davidson, B.J. & Foltz, G. (1985). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In: G.E. MacKinnon & T.G. Waller (eds.), Reading Research: Advances in Theory and Practice, Vol. 4. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 36: 321-339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ring, J., Wise, B.W. & Olson, R.K. (1998). An evaluation of R.A.N. and the double deficit hypothesis in computer-assisted remedial intervention. Poster presented to meetings of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, San Diego, California.

  • Rosner, J. & Simon, D.P. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial report, Journal of Learning Disabilities 4: 1-15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L.S., Share, D. & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics, Psychological Science 6: 780-786.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1986). 'Matthew effects' in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in acquisition of literacy, Reading Research Quarterly 4: 360-407.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sunseth, K. & Bowers, P.G. (August, 1996). A double-deficit hypothesis of reading: Phonemic awareness and naming speed. Poster presented at the annual meetings of the American Psychological Association, Toronto, Canada.

  • Sunseth, K. & Bowers, P.G. (March 1997). The relationship between digit naming speed and orthography in children with and without phonological deficits. Paper presented to the meetings of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Chicago, Illinois.

  • Swanson, H.L. & Alexander, J.E. (1997). Cognitive processes as predictors of word recognition and reading comprehension in learning-disabled and skilled readers: Revisiting the specificity hypothesis, Journal of Educational Psychology 89: 128-158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J.K. & Davis, C. (1996). Individual difference variables that predict response to training in phonological awareness, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 63: 1-21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J.K., Wagner, R.K., Rashotte, C.A., Burgess, S. & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatic naming ability to the growth of word-reading skills in second-to fifth-grade children, Scientific Studies of Reading 1: 161-185.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F.R., Scanlon, D.M. & Tanzman, M.S. (1994). Components of reading ability: Issues and problems in operationalizing word identification, phonological coding and orthographic coding. In: G.R. Lyon (ed.), Frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities: New views on measurement issues (pp. 279-324). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K. & Rashotte, C.A. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable lognitudinal study, Developmental Psychology 30: 73-87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wise, B.W., Ring, J. & Olson, R.K. (1999). Training phonological awareness with and without explicit attention to articulation, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 72: 271-304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Miller, L. & Donnelly, K. (1996). The RAVE-O Manual: Curriculum for the Development of Retrieval, Automaticity, Vocabulary Elaboration and Orthography. Manual for NICHD Project No. 1R55HD/OD30970-01A1.

  • Woodcock, R.W. & Johnson, M.B. (1989). Woodcock-Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery-Revised. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching Resources.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. (March, 1997). Relationship of phonological analysis and naming speed to training effects among dyslexic readers. Paper presented to meetings of the Society for the Scientific study of Reading, Chicago, Illinois.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Patricia G. Bowers.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bowers, P.G., Newby-Clark, E. The role of naming speed within a model of reading acquisition. Reading and Writing 15, 109–126 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013820421199

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1013820421199

Navigation