Skip to main content
Log in

Function Word Repetitions Emerge When Speakers Are Operantly Conditioned to Reduce Frequency of Silent Pauses

  • Published:
Journal of Psycholinguistic Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Beattie and Bradbury (1979) reported a study in which, in one condition, they punished speakers when they produced silent pauses (by lighting a light they were supposed to keep switched off). They found speakers were able to reduce silent pauses and that this was not achieved at the expense of reduced overall speech rate. They reported an unexpected increase in word repetition rate. A recent theory proposed by Howell, Au-Yeung, and Sackin (1999) predicts that the change in word repetition rate will occur on function, not content words. This hypothesis is tested and confirmed. The results are used to assess the theory and to consider practical applications of this conditioning procedure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Arnoff, M. (1976). Word formation in generative grammar. In L. J. Morgan and K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to syntax: Bootstrapping from speech to grammar in early acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Au-Yeung, J., Howell, P., & Pilgrim, L. (1998). Phonological words and stuttering on function words. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1019-1030.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beattie, G. W., & Bradbury, R. J. (1979). An experimental investigation of the modifiability of the temporal structure of spontaneous speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 8, 225-248.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blackmer, E. R., & Mitton, J. L. (1991). Theories of monitoring and the timing of repairs in spontaneous speech. Cognition, 39, 173-194.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L. (1970). Language development: Form and function in emerging grammars. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, K., & Levelt, W. (1994). Language production: Grammatical encoding. In M. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (pp. 945-978). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, S. F. (1945). The loci of stutterings in the speech sequence. Journal of Speech Disorders, 10, 182-192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (1975). Hesitation and semantic planning in speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 1, 75-78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A., & Miozzo, M. (1997). The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the “tip-of-the-tongue” phenomenon. Cognition, 64, 309-343.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H., & Clark, E. (1977). Psychology and language. An introduction to psycholinguistics.New York: Harcourt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283-321.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gee, J. P., & Grosjean, F. (1983). Performance structures: A psycholinguistic and linguistic apprasial. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 411-458.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Eisler, F. (1972). Pauses, clauses, sentences. Language and Speech, 15, 103-113.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hartmann, R. R. K., & Stork, F. C. (1972). Dictionary of language and linguistics. London: Applied Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howell, P., Au-Yeung, J., & Sackin, S. (1999). Exchange of stuttering from function words to content words with age. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 345-354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Howell, P., Au-Yeung, J., & Sackin, S. (2000). Internal structure of content words leading to lifespan differences in phonological difficulty in stuttering. Journal of Fluency Disorders, pp. 1-20.

  • Howell, P., Sackin, S., & Glenn, K. (1997). Development of a two-stage procedure for the automatic recognition of dysfluencies in the speech of children who stutter. I. Psychometric procedures appropriate for selection of training material for lexical dysfluency classifiers. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 40, 1073-1084.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H., & Francis, N. (1967). A computational analysis of present day English.Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Brain and Behavioral Sciences, 16, 217-265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1992). Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes, and representations. Cognition, 42, 1-22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (1997). Retrieval of lexical-syntactic features in tip-of-thetongue states. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 1410-1423.

    Google Scholar 

  • Onslow, M., Packman, A., Stocker, S., Van Doorn, J., & Siegel, G. M. (1997) Control of children's stuttering with response-contingent time-out: Behavioral, perceptual, and acoustic data. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 121-133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1995). Language acquisition. In L. R. Gleitman, M. Liberman, & D. N. Osherson (Eds.), An Invitation to Cognitive Science, 2nd edn. Vol. 1: Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartvik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, A. (1990). Syntactic theory and the acquisition of English syntax. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sander, E. K. (1972). When are speech sounds learned? Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 37, 55-63.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Selkirk, E. (1984). Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S., Monsell, S., Knoll, R. L., & Wright, C. E. (1978). The latency and duration of rapid movement sequences: Comparison of speech and typewriting. In G. E. Stelmach (Ed.), Information processing in motor control and learning.New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strenstrom, A.-B., & Svartvik, J. (1994). Imparsable speech: Repeats and other nonfluencies in spoken English. In N. Oostdijk & P. de Haan (Eds.), Corpus-based research into language. Atlanta, GA: Rodopi Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wingfield, A. (1968). Effects of frequency on identification and naming of objects. American Journal of Psychology, 81, 226-234.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Howell, P., Sackin, S. Function Word Repetitions Emerge When Speakers Are Operantly Conditioned to Reduce Frequency of Silent Pauses. J Psycholinguist Res 30, 457–474 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010432012241

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010432012241

Navigation