Skip to main content
Log in

Comparative Mental Development

  • Published:
Journal of Adult Development Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

To reconstruct the evolution of human cognitive development we are studying its comparative ontogeny in primate phylogeny (i.e., two monkey species, two chimpanzee species, and humans). The research has discovered primate conceptual universals including logical (e.g., classifying), arithmetic (e.g., quantitative substituting), and physical (e.g., causal) cognitions. But the ontogenetic covariation between these conceptual universals is progressively displaced or heterochronic in primate phylogeny. Heterochronic evolution produced the most synchronic and widest ontogenetic window for progressive cognitive development in humans.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Ahl, V. A. (1993). Cognitive development in infants prenatally exposed to cocaine. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1997). The role of immaturity in human development. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 153–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (2000). How flexible is the neurodevelopmental clock? In S. T. Parker, J. Langer, & M. L. McKinney (Eds.), Biology, brains and behavior: the evolution of human development. Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denney, N. (1972). Free classification in preschool children. Child Development, 43, 1161–1170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, R., Lanza, R. P., & Skinner, B. F. (1981). Self-awareness in the pigeon. Science, (1984) 212, 695–696.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finlay, B. L., & Darlington, R. D. (1995). Linked regularities in the development and evolution of mammalian brains. Science, 268, 1578–1584.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, K. R. (1990). New perspectives on instincts and intelligence: Brain size and the emergence of hierarchical mental constructional skills. In S. T. Parker & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes (pp. 97- 128). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, K. R. (1991). Myelination and behavioral development: A comparative perspective on questions of neoteny, altriciality and intelligence. In K. R. Gibson & A. C. Petersen (Eds.), Brain maturation and cognitive development (pp. 29–63). New York: de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould S. J. (1984). Relationship of individual and group care. Human Development, 27, 233–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenfield, P. M. & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1990). Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: process of learning and invention in the evolution and development of language. In S. T. Parker & K. R. Gibson (Eds.), “Language” and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp. 540–78). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grigorakis, K. (1999). Numerical architectures in early manipulatory classification. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of California at Berkeley.

  • Jacobsen, T. A. (1984). The construction and regulation of early structures of logic. A cross-cultural study of infant cognitive development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.

  • Kuhn, D., Langer, J., Kohlberg, L., & Haan, N. S. (1977). The development of formal operations in logical and moral judgment. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 95, 97–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1969). Theories of development. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1980). The origins of logic: 6 to 12 months. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1986). The origins of logic: One to two years. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1989). Comparison with the human child. In F. Antinucci (Ed.), Cognitive structure and development of nonhuman primates (pp. 229–242). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1993). Comparative cognitive development. In K. R. Gibson and T. Ingold (Eds.), Tools, language and cognition in human evolution (pp. 300–313). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1994). From acting to understanding: The comparative development of meaning. In W. F. Overton & D. Palermo (Eds.), The nature and ontogenesis of meaning (pp. 191–213). Norwood, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1996). Heterochrony and the evolution of primate cognitive development. In Russon, A. E., Bard, K. A. & Parker, S. T. (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The mind of the great apes (pp. 257–277). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (1998). Phylogenetic and ontogenetic origins of logic: Classification. In J. Langer & M. Killen (Eds.), Piaget, evolution and development (pp. 33–54). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J. (in press). The descent of cognitive development. Developmental Science.

  • Langer, J. (2000). The heterochronic evolution of primate cognitive development. In S. T. Parker, J. Langer & M. L. McKinney (Eds.), Biology, brains and behavior: The evolution of human development (pp. 213–233). Santa Fe, NM: School of American Research Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langer, J., Schlesinger, M., Spinozzi, G., & Natale, F. (1998). Developing classification in action: I. Human infants. Human Evolution, 13, 107–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (1979). Social cognition and the acquisition of self. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mayr, E. (1994). Recapitulation reinterpreted: The somatic program. Quarterly Review of Biology, 69, 223–232.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKinney, M. L. & McNamara, J. K. (1991). Heterochrony: The evolution of ontogeny. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montague, A. (1981). Growing young. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Natale, F. (1989) Causality II: The stick problem. In Antinucci, F. (Ed.), Cognitive structure and development in nonhuman primates (pp. 121–133). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1973). some evidence for the primacy of categorization and its functional basis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 19, 21–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paus, T., Zijdenbos, A., Worsley, K., Collins, D. L., Blumenthal, J., Giedd, J. N., Rapoport, J. L. & Evans, A. C. (1999). Structural maturation of neural pathways in children and adolescents: In vivo study. Science, 283, 1908–1911.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poti, P., Langer, J., Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. & Brakke, K. E. (1999). Spontaneous logicomathematical constructions by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes, Pan paniscus). Animal Cognition, 2, 147–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Purves, D. (1988). Body and brain: A trophic theory of neural connections. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riccuiti, H. N. (1965). Object grouping and selective ordering behavior in infants 12 to 24 months. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 11, 129–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R. A., Brakke, K. E., Williams, S. L., & Rumbaugh, D. M. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 3–4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slotnick, C. (1984). The organization and regulation of block constructions: A comparison of autistic and normal children's cognitive development. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California at Berkeley.

  • Smith, H. (1992). Life history and the evolution of human maturation. Evolutionary Anthropology, 1, 134–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinozzi, G. (1993). Development of spontaneous classificatory behavior in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107, 193–200.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinozzi, G., & Langer, J. (1999). Spontaneous classification in action by a human-enculturated and language-reared bonobo (Pan paniscus) and common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 286–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinozzi, G., & Natale, F. (1989). Classification. In F. Antinucci (Ed.), Cognitive structure and development of nonhuman primates (pp. 163–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinozzi, G., Natale, F., Langer, J., Brakke, K. (1999). Spontaneous class grouping behavior by bonobos (Pan paniscus) and common chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Animal Cognition, 2, 157–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinozzi, G., Natale, F., Langer, J., & Schlesinger, M. (1998). Developing classification in action: II. Young chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Human Evolution, 13, 125–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spinozzi, G. & Poti, P. (1989). Causality I: The support problem. In F. Antinucci (Ed.), Cognitive structure and development of nonhuman primates (pp. 113–120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starkey, D. (1981). The origins of concept formation: Object sorting and object preference in early infancy. Child Development, 52, 489–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sugarman, S. (1983). Children's early thought: Developments in classification. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1937). Process and achievement. Harvard Educational Review, 7, 353–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. New York: International Universities Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H. & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Langer, J. Comparative Mental Development. Journal of Adult Development 7, 23–30 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009598917468

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1009598917468

Navigation