Skip to main content
Log in

Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

There is some controversy in the research literature as towhether the development of reading skills in differentorthographies varies primarily as a function of common underlyingcognitive processes (`the central processing hypothesis'), oralternatively, as a function of orthographic transparency (`thescript dependent hypothesis'). These alternative views wereexamined by studying the reading skills of 245 children in grades1--5, learning to read concurrently in English, their firstlanguage (L1) and Hebrew, their second language (L2). Childrenwere administered a non-verbal intelligence task, parallel L1 andL2 memory tasks, and word recognition and pseudoword readingtasks in both languages. Ratings of Hebrew oral proficiency wereprovided by teachers. The central processing hypothesis waspartially supported in that regardless of orthography, memoryexplained a small proportion of the variance on L1 and L2 readingmeasures. Though L2 oral proficiency was a significant predictorof L2 reading, it explained only a small proportion of thevariance. The script dependent hypothesis was supported by thefact that (a) children could read more accurately voweled Hebrew(a `transparent' orthography) than English (a `deep'orthography), (b) the developmental profiles associated withEnglish word recognition and pseudoword decoding was much steeperthan the one depicting Hebrew word recognition and Hebrewpseudoword word decoding, and (c) decoding error categories wereorthography-specific. We conclude that the two alternatives arecomplementary: When the script is less complex young childrenappear to develop their word recognition skills with relativeease, even in the absence of sufficient linguistic proficiency.At the same time, a more accurate picture of what facilitates L1and L2 reading development is enhanced when individualdifferences in underlying cognitive skills are considered aswell.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Assink, E.M.H. & Kattenberg, G. (1994). Higher-order linguistic influences on development of orthographic knowledge: Illustrations from spelling problems in Dutch and assess-ment tools. In: V.W. Berninger (ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, Volume 1: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 111–136). Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Akamatsu, N. (1999). The effects of first language orthographic features on word recognition processing in English as a second language, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 11: 381–403.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balmuth, M. (1992). The roots of phonics: A historical introduction. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V.W. & Abbott, R.D. (1994). Multiple orthographic and phonological codes in literacy acquisition: An evolving research program. In: V.W. Berninger (ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, Volume 1: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 277–317). Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. (1978). Modern Hebrew structure. Tel Aviv: University Publishing Projects, Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berman, R. (1985). The acquisition of Hebrew. In: D. Slobin (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Volume 1: The data (pp. 255–371). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P., Golden, J., Kennedy, A. & Young, A. (1994). Limits upon orthographic knowledge due to processes indexed by naming speed. In: V.W. Berninger (ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge, Volume 1: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 173–218). Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection, Nature 301: 419–421.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G.D.A. & Hulme, C. (1992). Cognitive psychology and second-language processing: The role of short-term memory. In: R.J. Harris (ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 105–122). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P.E. & Bradley, L.L. (1985). Children's reading problems. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, L.B. (1981). Twenty-five years of research on foreign language aptitude. In: K.C. Diller (ed.), Individual differences and universals in language learning aptitude (pp. 83–118). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Case, R. (1985). Intellectual development: Birth to adulthood. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J.S. (1996). Stages of reading development, 2nd edn. Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cossu, G., Shankweiler, D. Liberman, I.Y., Tola G. & Katz, L. (1988). Awareness of phon-ological segments and reading ability in Italian children, Applied Psycholinguistics 9: 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, L. (1984). Bilingualism and special education: Issues in assessment and pedagogy. Clevedon, Avon: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M. & Blennerhasset, A. (1984). How to assess the listening comprehension skills of prereaders, Journal of Educational Psychology 76: 1372–1381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19: 450–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A.Y. & Hancin, B.J. (1992). An overview of cross-language transfer in bilin-gual reading. In: R.J. Harris (ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 391–412). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A.Y., Nagy, W.E. & Hancin, B.J. (1993). Cross-language transfer of phonemic awareness, Journal of Educational Psychology 85: 453–465.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doctor, E.A. & Klein, D. (1992). Phonological processing in bilingual word recognition. In: R.J. Harris (ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 237–252). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fang Hu, C. & Catts, H.W. (1993). Phonological recoding as a universal process? Evidence from beginning readers of Chinese, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 5: 325–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., Katz, L. & Bentin S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and ortho-graphic depth: A multilingual comparison, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13: 104–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. (1999). Issues in the development of second language reading: Implications for instruction and assessment. In T. Nunes (ed.), Learning to read: An integrated view from research and practice. Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. & Clifton, S. (1993). The development of first and second language reading skills in early French immersion, Canadian Modern Language Review 50: 646–667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva E. & Ryan, E.B. (1993). Linguistic and cognitive correlates of academic skills in first and second language, Language Learning 43: 5–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. & Wade-Woolley, L. (1994). Methodological issues in the study of basic reading processes in bilingual learners. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans.

  • Geva, E., Wade-Woolley, L. & Shany, M. (1993). The concurrent development of spelling and decoding in two different orthographies, Journal of Reading Behavior 25: 383–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. & Wade-Woolley, L. & Shany, M. (1997). The development of reading efficiency in first and second language, Scientific Studies of Reading 1: 119–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gholamain, M. & Geva, E. (1998). The concurrent development of word recognition skills in English and Farsi, under review.

  • Gleitman, L.R. (1985). Orthographic resources affect reading acquisition if they are used, RAE 6: 24–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herringbone, M. (1992) Working memory capacity as a constraint on L2 development. In: R.J. Harris (ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 123–136). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jastak, J. & Jastak, S. (1984). The Wide Range Achievement Test - Revised. Wilmington, DE: Jastak Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, N.E. & Biemiller, A.J. (1985). Letter, word, and text reading times of precocious and average readers, Child Development 56: 196–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, J.C. (1992). Critical periods in second language acquisition: The effects of written versus auditory material on the assessment of grammatical competence, Language Learning 42: 217–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P.L. & Gough, P.B. (1985). Reading and spelling strategies of first-grade children. In: J.A. Niles & R. Latik (eds.), Issues in literacy: A research perspective (pp. 306–309). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L. & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In: R. Frost & L. Katz (eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 67–84). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (1989). Effects of L1 orthographic representation on L2 phonological coding strategies, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 18: 201–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (1994). Second language reading research: Problems and possibilities, Applied Psycholinguistics 15: 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C.K. (1986). What does accessing a morphemic script tell us about reading and reading disorders in an alphabetic script?, Annals of Dyslexia 36: 82–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C.K., Cheng, P.W. & Mulcahy, R. (1987). Automatic processing of morphemic orthography by mature readers, Language and Speech 30: 181–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, Y. (1988). The nature of early langauge: Evidence from the development of Hebrew morphology. In: Y. Levy, I. Schlesinger & M. Braine (eds.), Categories and processes in language acquisition (pp. 73–96). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindgren, S.D., De Renzi, E. & Richman, L.C. (1985). Cross-national comparisons of development dyslexia in Italy and the United States, Child Development 56: 1404–1417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, I. (1988). From print to meaning: Logographs versus alphabetic words. Paper presented at the McLuhan Conference on Literacy: East meets West, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

  • Mann, V.A. (1986). Why some children encounter reading problems: The contribution of difficulties with language processing and phonological sophistication to early reading disability. In: J.K. Torgesen & B.Y.L. Wong (eds.), Psychological and educational perspectives on learning disabilities (pp. 133–159). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKeough, A. (1982). The development of complexity in children's narratives. Unpublished M.A. thesis, University of Toronto (OISE), Canada.

  • McLaughlin, B. (1991). The relationship between first and second languages: Language profi-ciency and language aptitude: In: B. Harley, P. Allen, J. Cummins & M. Swain (eds.), The development of second langauge proficiency. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, B. (1992). Working memory capacity as a constraint on L2 development. In: R.J. Harris (ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals (pp. 123–135). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, J. & Willows, D. (1996). Early phonological awareness training for at-risk children in junior kindergarten. Paper presented at the National Reading Conference, as part of the symposium Systematic phonics within a balanced literacy program, Charleston, SC.

  • Naslund, J.C. & Schneider, W. (1991). Longitudinal effects of verbal ability, memory capa-city and phonological awareness on reading performance, The European Journal of Psychology of Education 6: 375–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Obler, L.K. (1989). The boustrophedal brain: Laterality and dyslexia in bidirectional readers. In: K. Hyltenstam & L.K. Obler (eds.), Bilingualism across the life-span: Aspects of acquisition, maturity, and loss (pp. 155–172). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peereman, R. (1992). Lecture, ecritures, orthographes [Reading, writing and orthographies]. In: P. Lecocq (ed.), La lecture: Processus, appretissage, troubles [Reading: Processes, acquisition and difficulties] (pp. 55–77). Lille, France: Presses Universitaires de Lille.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In: P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri & R. Treiman (eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 145–174). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. & Zhang, S. (1991). Phonological processes in reading Chinese characters, Journal of Experimental Psychology 17: 633–643.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrie, A. & Geva, E. (1991). A framework for second language remediation of reading disabled adults: Implications from a case study, Journal of Dynamic Assessment 2: 67–80.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segalowtiz, N. (1986). Skilled reading in a second language. In: J. Vaid (ed.), Language processing in bilinguals: Psycholinguistic and neuropsychological perspectives (pp. 3–19). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J.C. (1965). The Colored Progressive Matrices Test. London: Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sergent, J. & Hellige, J. (1986). Role of input factors in visual-field asymmetries, Brain and Cognition 5: 174–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. (1992). Reading, word recognition and dyslexia. In: P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri & R. Treiman (eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 243–274). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S. Waters, G.S., Barnes, M.A. & Tanenhaus, M.K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition?, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23: 383–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. (1993). The role of vowels in reading: A review of studies of English and Hebrew, Psychological Bulletin 114: 52–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. & Navon, D. (1982). The dependency on graphemes and their translation to phonemes in reading: A developmental perspective, Reading Research Quarterly 17: 210–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. & Sivan, T. (1994). Reading proficiency and orthography: Evidence from Hebrew and English, Language Learning 44: 5–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shohamy, E. (1984). Toward a valid and reliable measure of oral proficiency: From research results to educational policy. Unpublished Manuscript.

  • Siegel, L.S. & Ryan, E.B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children, Child Development 60: 973–980.

    Google Scholar 

  • So, Dominica Chi-Ho (1989). Learning to read Chinese: Semantic, syntactic, and phono-logical processing skills in normally achieving and reading disabled Chinese children. Unpublished Masters of Arts Thesis, University of Toronto (OISE), Canada.

  • Stanovich, K., Cunningham, A.E. & Feeman (1984). Intelligence, cognitive skills, and early reading progress, Reading Research Quarterly 19: 278–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H.W., Stigler, J.W., Lucker, G.W., Hsu, C.C. & Kitamura, S. (1982). Reading disabilities: The case of Chinese, Japanese and English, Child Development 53: 1164–1181.

    Google Scholar 

  • The Tenth Mental Measurements Yearbook (1989). J.C. Conoley & J.J. Kramer (eds.) (pp. 897–905). The Buros Institute of Mental Measurements, The University of Nebraska-Lincoln.

  • Turvey, M.T., Feldman, L.B. & Lukatela, G. (1984). The Serbo-Croatian orthography constrains the reader to a phonologically analysis strategy. In: L. Henderson (ed.), Orthographies and reading (pp. 81–89). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R.L. (1970). The structure of English orthography. The Hague, Paris: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L.T. (1990). Acquisition of reading in a second language, Reading Research Quarterly 25: 90–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wade-Woolley, L. (1996). Phonological processing in L2 reading. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto (OISE), Canada.

  • Wade-Woolley, L. & Geva, E. (1994). The role of L2 phonology in L2 reading. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New Orleans.

  • Wade-Woolley, L. & Geva, E. (1999). Processing inflected morphology in second language word recognition: Russian-speakers and English-speakers read Hebrew, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 11: 321–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K., Torgesen, J.K. & Rashotte, C.A. (1994). Development of reading-related phon-ological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study, Developmental Psychology 30: 73–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, 3rd edn. New York: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiss, C.A. (1987). Issues in the assessment of learning problems in children from French immersion programs: A case study illustration in support of Cummins, Interchange 9: 73–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R.W. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Tests - Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Services, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Geva, E., Siegel, L.S. Orthographic and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in two languages. Reading and Writing 12, 1–30 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008017710115

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008017710115

Navigation