Skip to main content
Log in

Developmental associations between verbal and visual short-term memory and the acquisition of decoding skill

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study longitudinally investigated the relationships between verbaland visual short-term memory (STM) and the acquisition of decoding from thepre-reading through the early acquisition stages in 63 Hebrew-speakingchildren Test waves occurred in kindergarten, first grade and second grade.IQ, visual and verbal STM and decoding ability were assessed. The dataindicated that while both verbal and visual STM in kindergarten were significantlycorrelated to later decoding skill, pre-reading visual STM was a stronger predictor.The results further showed that pre-reading performance on the WISC-R BlockDesign test predicted later decoding ability, while performance on theWISC-R Vocabulary test did not. Lastly decoding skill in grade 1 was foundto predict only visual in grade 2. These results indicate that visualparameters may make a crucial contribution to the acquisition of decodingskills. The size of pre-reading visual STM capacity appears to play a rolein this process. The relationship between visual STM and decoding may bebidirectional, as learning to decode appears to develop visual STM. It issuggested that either language-related or task-related factors may accountfor these counter-to-mainstream results.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A.D. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Dror, I., Frost, R. & Bentin, S. (1995). Orthographic representation in skilled readers: A cross-language comparison, Psychological Science 6: 176–181.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S. (1992). Phonological awareness, reading and reading acquisition: A survey and appraisal of current knowledge. In: R. Frost & L. Katz (eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology and meaning (pp. 193–210). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S., Bargai, N. & Katz, L. (1984). Orthographic and phonemic coding for lexical access: Evidence from Hebrew, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 10: 353–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentin, S. & Leshem, H. (1993). On the interaction between phonological awareness and reading acquisition: It's a two-way street, Annals of Dyslexia 43: 125–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. (1986). Short-term memory, phonological processing and reading ability, Annals of Dylexia 36: 138–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (1988). Nonsense word reading test. Unpublished test, University of Haifa, Israel.

  • Breznitz, Z. & Share, D.L. (1992). Effects of accelerated reading rate on memory for text, Journal of Educational Psychology 84: 193–197.

    Google Scholar 

  • Center of Educational Technology (1988). Without secrets. Reading instruction program, CET, Tel Aviv, Israel (in Hebrew).

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J.S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R.L., Netley, C. & Clark, M.A. (1984). On the generality of the short-term memory/reading ability relationship, Journal of Learning Disabilities 17: 218–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1986). The Psychology of reading. Hillsdale, New York: Erlbrum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, R. (1971). The chronology of the development of covert speech in children, Development Psychology 33: 386–404.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corcos, E. & Willows, D. (1993). The role of visual processing in good and poor readers' utilization of orthographic information in letter strings. In: S.F. Wright & R. Groner (eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp. 95–106). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crain, S., Shankweiler, D., Macaruso, P. & Bar-Shalom, E. (1990). Working memory and comprehension of spoken sentences: Investigations of children with reading disorder. In: G. Vallar & T. Shallice (eds.), Neuropsychological impairments of short-term memory (pp. 477–508). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crammond, J. (1991). Analyzing the basic cognitive-developmental processes of children with specific types of learning disability. In: R. Case (ed.), The mind's staircase (pp. 285–302). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 19: 450–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1981). Memory span: Sources of individual and developmental differences, Psychological Bulletin 89: 63–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1985). Short-term memory development in childhood and adolescence. In: C.J. Brainerd & M. Pressley (eds.), Basic processes in memory development (pp. 209–248). New York: Springer Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deutsch, J.A. (1978). Reading disability: Methodological problems in information processing analysis, Science 200: 802.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. (1991). The development of reading and spelling in children: An overview. In: M. Snowling & M. Thomson (eds.), Dyslexia: Integrating theory and practice (pp. 63–79). London: Whurr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (1990). Reading, phonological skills and short-term memory: interactive tributaries of development, Journal of Research in Reading 13: 107–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. & Large, B. (1987). The development of reading: As you seek so you shall find, British Journal of Psychology 78: 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. & Large, B. (1988). The early stages of reading: Longitudinal study, Applied Cognitive Psychology 2: 47–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, B.W., Brasdo, N. & Richards, I.L. (1993). Linking the sensory and motor visual correlates of dyslexia. In: S.F. Wright & R. Groner (eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp. 179–192). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feitelson, D. (1988). Facts and fads in beginning reading. New York: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J.M. (1985). Memory for verbal and nonverbal stimuli in learning disability subgroups: Analysis by selective reminding, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 40: 244–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In: K. Patterson, J. Marshall & M. Coltheart (eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301–330). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1986). Psychological aspects of orthographic skills: Development and disorder. In: G. Augst (ed.), New trends in graphemics and orthography (pp. 218–233). Berlin/New York: Walter de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R., Katz, L. & Bentin, S. (1987). Strategies for visual word recognition and orthographic depth: A multilingual comparison, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 13: 104–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S.E., Emslie, H. & Baddeley, A.D. (1990, January). Differential contributions of phonological memory to reading development between ages five and seven. Paper presented to the Experimental Psychological Society.

  • Gathercole, S.E., Willis, C. & Baddeley, A.D. (1991). Differentiating phonological memory and awareness of rhyme: Reading and vocabulary development in children, Journal of Psychology 82: 387–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geiger, G. & Lettvin, J. (1987). Peripheral vision in persons with dyslexia, New England Journal of Medicine 316: 1238–1243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. & Siegel, L. (1991). The role of orthography and cognitive factors in the concurrent development of basic reading skills in bilingual children. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Minneapolis, MN.

  • Geva, E., Wade-Wooley, L. & Shaney, M. (1993). The concurrent development of spelling and decoding in two different orthographies, Journal of Reading Behavior 25: 383–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glaub, V.E. & Kamphaus, R.W. (1991). Construction of a nonverbal adaption of the Stanford-Binet fourth edition, Educational and Psychological Measurement 51: 231–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, J.H. & Glencross, D.J. (1990). Do children with a specific reading disability have a general serial-order deficit?, Neuropsychologica 28: 271–278.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graf, E. (1993). A new theoretical framework for understanding reading and spelling tasks. In: S.F. Wright & R. Groner (eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp. 65–76). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, R. & Lovegrove, W. (1993). One word at a time: A solution to the visual deficit in the specific reading disabled? In: S.F. Wright & R. Groner (eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp. 65–76). Amsterdam: North-Hollandd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hitch, G.J. & Halliday, M.S. (1983). Working memory in children, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B302: 325–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hitch, G.J., Halliday, S., Schaafstal, P.M. & Schraagen, S. (1988). Visual working memory in young children, Memory and Cognition 10: 120–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • John, K.R. & Rattan, G. (1991). A comparison of short-term memory tests as predictors of reading achievement for learning disabled and educable mentally retarded students, Journal of School Psychology 29: 309–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jorm, A.F. (1983). Specific reading retardation and working memory: A review, British Journal of Psychology 74: 311–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jorm, A.F., Share, D.L. Maclean, R. & Matthews, R. (1984). Phonological confusability in short-term memory for sentences as a predictor of reading ability, British Journal of Psychology 75: 393–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. & Roper-Schneider, D. (1985). The influence of basal readers on first grade readers, Reading Research Quarterly 20: 134–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L.K. & Feldman, L.B. (1983). Relation between pronunciation and recognition of printed words in deep and shallow orthographies, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition 9: 157–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L.K. & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In: R. Frost & Katz (eds.), Orthography, phonology and meaning. Advances in psychology 94: 67–84.

  • Keeny, T.J., Carnizzo, S.R. & Flavell, J.H. (1967). Spontaneous and induced rehearsal in a recall task, Child Development 38: 953–966.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1984). Reading without vowels: Lexical access in Hebrew. In: H. Bouma & D.G. Bouwhuis (eds.), Attention and performance, Vol. 10: Control of language processes (pp. 227–242). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1985). Lexical access for low and high frequency words in Hebrew, Memory and Cognition 13: 37–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovegrove, W., Martin, F. & Slaghuis, W. (1986). A theoretical and experimental case for a residual deficit in specific reading disability, Cognitive Neuropsychology 3: 225–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V.A. & Ditunno, P. (1990). Phonological deficiencies: Effective predictors of future reading problems. In: G. Pavlidis (ed.), Perspectives on dyslexia: Cognition, language and treatment, Vol. 2. Chichester, UK: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V.A. & Liberman, I.Y. (1984). Phonological awareness and verbal short-term memory, Journal of Learning Disabilites 17: 592–599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. & Shimron, J. (1981). Does word naming involve grapheme-to-phoneme translation? Evidence from Hebrew, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20: 97–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon, D. & Shimron, J. (1985). Source and function of phonological codes in processing written Hebrew: Final report to US-Israeli Binational Science Foundation. Haifa, Israel: University of Haifa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R.K., Davidson, B.J., Kliegal, R. & Foltz, G. (1985). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In: T.G. Waller (ed.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 1–64). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortar, G. & Segev, N. (1980). Reading comprehension test for grades one and two. Jerusalem, Isreal: Ministery of Education and Culture.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1985). Reading ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rapala, M.M. & Brady, S. (1990). Reading ability and short-term memory: The role of phonological processing, Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 1–25.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raynr, K. & Pollatsek, A. (1989). The psychology of reading. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothschild-Yakar, L. (1989). A study of reading processes in Hebrew from a developmental perspective among children learning in two different instructional methods. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Haifa (in Hebrew).

  • Sattler, J.M. (1982). Assessment of children's intelligence and special abilities, 2nd edn. Boston: Allyn Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D. & Crain, S. (1986). Language mechanisms and reading disorder: A modular approach, Cognition 24: 139–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D.L. & Levin, I. (1998). Learning to read and write in Hebrew. In: M. Harris & G. Hatano (eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shimron, J. (1993). The role of vowels in reading: A review of studies of English and Hebrew, Psychological Bulletin 114: 52–67.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S. (1985). Constraining models of word recognition, Cognition 20: 63–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S. (1990). Dyslexia in a computational model of word recognition in reading. In: P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri & Trieman (eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 243–273). Hillsdale: NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, L.S. & Ryan, E.B. (1988). Development of grammatical sensitivity, phonological and short-term memory skills in normally achieving and disabled children, Developmental Psychology 24: 28–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegal, L.S. & Ryan, E.B. (1989). The development of working memory in normally achieving and subtypes of learning disabled children, Child Development 60: 973–980.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H.A. (1974). How big is a chuck?, Science 183: 482–488.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1985). Explaining the variance in reading ability in terms of psychological processes: What have we learned?, Annals of Dyslexia 35: 67–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy, Reading Research Quarterly 16: 32–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, H.C. (1994). Short-term memory and working memory: Do both contribute to our understanding of academic achievement in children and adults with learning disabilities?, Journal of Learning Disabilities 27: 34–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • The Psychological-Educational Learning Disability Team (1988). Reading test. Jerusalem, Israel: Muncipality of Jerusalem.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, R.L., Hagen, E.P. & Sattler, J.M. (1986). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, 4th edn. Chicago: Riverside.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W.E. (1989). The role of language-related factors in reading disability. In: D. Shankweiler & I.Y. Liberman (eds.), Phonology and reading disability (pp. 91–131). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vallar, G. & Shallice, T. (1990). Neuropsychological impairements of short-term memory Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K. (1987). Causal relations between the development of phonological processing abilities and the acquisition of reading skills: A meta-analysis, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 34: 261–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K. & Torgesen, J.K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills, Psychological Bulletin 101: 192–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1977). Weshsler Intelligence Scale for Children — Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willows, D.M., Corcos, E. & Kershner, J.R. (1993). In: S.F. Wright & R. Groner (eds.), Facets of dyslexia and its remediation (pp. 163–178). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willows, D.M., Kruk, R.S. & Corcos, E. (eds.) (1993). Visual processes in reading and reading disabilites. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright, S.F. & Groner, R. (eds.) (1993). Facets of dyslexia and its remediation, Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Meyler, A., Breznitz, Z. Developmental associations between verbal and visual short-term memory and the acquisition of decoding skill. Reading and Writing 10, 519–540 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007915517339

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1007915517339

Navigation