Skip to main content
Log in

From verbal descriptions to graphic representations: Stability and change in students' alternative conceptions

  • Published:
Educational Studies in Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigated students' conceptions and misconceptions relating to the construction of graphs. Participants were 92 eighth-grade students randomly selected from two schools. Students were tested before and after being exposed to formal instruction on graphing. Qualitative analysis of students' responses identified three main kinds of alternative conceptions: (a) constructing an entire graph as one single point; (b) constructing a series of graphs, each representing one factor from the relevant data; and (c) conserving the form of an increasing function under all conditions. In addition, the following kinds of errors were displayed by less than 10% of the subjects: conceiving a generalized, stereotypic idea of a graph, using arrows or stairs to represent the direction of the covariation, and connecting the ticks on the axes by lines or curves. Quantitative analyses of the data indicated that overall students did not enter the learning situation as a tabula rasa. On the pretest, about a quarter of the students constructed correctly graphs representing increasing, constant, curvilinear, and decreasing functions, and many more students represented correctly at least one kind of function. Further analyses showed the stability and change in students' alternative conceptions after students were exposed to formal instruction about graphing. The theoretical and practical implications of the findings are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • Abimbola, I.O.: 1988, ‘The problem of terminology in the study of student conceptions in science’, Science Education 72, 175–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aspis, A.: 1985, Algebra and Statistics for Students in Seventh Grade, Author Publication (Hebrew), Tel-Aviv.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beichner, R.J.: 1993, ‘Testing student interpretation of kinematics graphs’, Paper presented at the 5th European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, France.

  • Bell, A., Brekke, G. and Swan, M.: 1987a, ‘Diagnostic teaching: 4 graphical interpretations’, Mathematics Teaching 119, 56–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A., Brekke, G. and Swan, M.: 1987b, ‘Diagnostic teaching: 5 graphical interpretations teaching styles and their effects’, Mathematics Teaching 120, 50–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bell, A. and Janvier, C.: 1981, ‘The interpretation of graphs representing situations’, For the Learning of Mathematics 2, 34–42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brassel, H. M. and Rowe, M. B.: 1993, ‘Graphing skills among high school physics students’, School Science and Mathematics 93, 63–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S. and VanLehn, K.: 1980, ‘Repair theory: A generative theory of bugs in procedural skills’, Cognitive Science 4, 379–426.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clement, J.: 1989, ‘The concept of variation and misconceptions in Cartesian graphing’, Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics 11, 77–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Confrey, J.: 1990, ‘A review of the research on student conceptions in mathematics, science, and programming’, in C.E. Cazden (ed.), Review of Research in Education 16, 3–56.

  • Confrey, J.: 1993, ‘Learning to see children's mathematics: Crucial challenges in constructivist reform’, in K. Tobin (ed.), The practice of constructivism in science education, AAAS Press, Washington, DC, pp 299–321.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, T. and Eisenberg, T.: 1982, ‘The function concept in college students: Linearity, smoothness and periodicity’, Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics 5, 119–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eylon, B. and Linn, M. C.: 1988, ‘Learning and instruction: An examination of four research perspectives in science education’, Review of Educational Research 58, 251–302.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kerslake, D.: 1981, ‘Graphs’, in K. M. Hart (ed.), Children's Understanding of Mathematical Concepts, John Murray, London, pp. 11–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • King, A.: 1994, ‘Guiding knowledge construction in the classroom: Effects of teaching children how to question and how to explain’, American Educational Research Journal 31, 338–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krabbendam, H.: 1982, ‘The non-qualitative way of describing relations and the role of graphs: Some experiments’, in G. van Barnveld and H. Krabbendam (eds.), Report 1, Foundation for Curriculum Development, Enschede, The Netherlands, pp. 125–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leinhardt, G., Zaslavsky, O. and Stein, M. K.: 1990, ‘Functions, graphs, and graphing: Tasks, learning, and teaching’, Review of Educational Research 60, 1–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovell, K.: 1971, ‘Some aspects of growth of the concept of a function’, in M. F. Rosskopf, L. P. Steffe and S. Taback (eds.), Piagetian Cognitive Development Research and Mathematical Education, National Council of Teachers of Mathematics, Washington, DC, pp. 12–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markovitz, Z., Eylon, B. and Brukheimer, M.: 1983, ‘Functions: Linearity unconstrained’, in R. Hershkowitz (ed.), Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference of the International Group for the PME, Rehovot, Israel, pp. 271–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, L. C., Rosenquist, M. L. and van Zee, E. H. van: 1987, ‘Student difficulties in connecting graphs and physics: Examples from kinematics’, American Journal of Physics 55, 503–513.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKenzie, D. L. and Padilla, M. J.: 1986, ‘The construction and validation of the Test of Graphing in Science (TOGS)’, Journal of Research in Science Teaching 23, 571–579.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mokors, J. R. and Tinker, R. F.: 1987, ‘The impact of microcomputer-based labs on children's ability to interpret graphs’, Journal of Research in Science Teaching 24, 369–383.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moschkovich, J., Schoenfeld, A. H. and Arcavi, A.: 1993, ‘Aspects of understanding: On multiple perspectives and representations of linear relations and connections among them’, in T. A. Romberg, E. Fenemma and T. P. Carpenter (eds.), Integrating Research on the Graphical Representation of Functions, Lawrence Erlbaum Association, NY, pp. 69–100.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesher, P.: 1987, ‘Towards an instructional theory: The role of students' misconceptions’, For the Learning of Mathematics 7, 33–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nisbett, R. and Ross, L.: 1980, Human Inference: Strategies and Shortcomings of Social Judgment, Prentice-Hall, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novak, J. D.: 1985, ‘Metalearning and metaknowledge strategies to help students learn how to learn’, in L. West and A. Pines (eds.), Cognitive Structure and Conceptual Change, Academic Press, New York, pp. 189–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osborne, R. J. and Wittrock, M. C.: 1983, ‘Learning science: A generative process’, Science Education 67, 498–508.

    Google Scholar 

  • Padilla, M. J., McKenzie, D. L. and Show, E. L. Jr.: 1986, ‘An examination of the line graphing ability of students in grades seven through twelve’, School Science and Mathematics 86, 20–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preece, J.: 1983, ‘Graphs are not straightforward’, in T. R. G. Green and S. J. Payne (eds.), The Psychology of Computer Use: A European Perspective, Academic Press, London, pp. 41–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, L.: 1987, Education and Learning to Think, National Academy Press, Washington, DC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, E. and Mevarech, Z. R., ‘Children's understanding of successive halving in practical and abstract contexts’, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology (in press).

  • Smith, J. P., DiSessa, A. A. and Roschelle, J.: 1993/94, ‘Misconceptions reconceived: A constructivist analysis of knowledge in transition’, The Journal of the Learning Science 3, 115–163.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strauss, S. and Schneider, A.: 1993, ‘The development of young children's concepts of graphs: A case of constructing a system of convention’, Paper presented at the 5th European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, Aix-en-Provence, France.

  • Vandeputte, C.: 1993, ‘Misconceptions in space-time graphs’, Paper presented at the 5th European Association for Research on Learning and Instruction, Aix-en-Provence, France.

  • Vinner, S.: 1983, ‘Concept definition, concept image and the notion of function’, International Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 20, 356–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wainer, H.: 1992, ‘Understanding graphs and tables’, Educational Researcher 21, 14–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wavering, M. J.: 1989, ‘Logical reasoning necessary to make line graphs’, Journal of Research in Science Teaching 26, 373–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yerushalmy, M.: 1991, ‘Effects of computerized feedback on performing and debugging algebraic transformations’, Journal of Educational Computing Research 7, 309–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zaslavsky, O.: 1987, ‘Conceptual obstacles in the learning of quadratic functions’, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Technion, Haifa, Israel.

Download references

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Mevarech, Z.R., Kramarsky, B. From verbal descriptions to graphic representations: Stability and change in students' alternative conceptions. Educational Studies in Mathematics 32, 229–263 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1002965907987

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1002965907987

Keywords

Navigation