Skip to main content
Log in

Who's Credible? Expressions of Consensus and Conflict in Focus Groups about DNA Patenting

  • Article
  • Published:
BioSocieties Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this article I explore attributions of credibility, important to designations of ‘expertise’, within the area of DNA patenting. I define ‘credibility’ as a relationship of recognition people establish to other actors. Through repeated interviews, interspersed with the provision of information, I have identified two different ways in which respondents position themselves in relation other actors as a way of assessing their credibility: when assuming a ‘partisan position’, they explicitly draw on ideological convictions; when assuming a ‘hierarchical position’, the ranking of credibility depends on how informed and objective others are perceived to be. The positions embody different roles for the respondents as ‘citizens’ and as ‘lay persons’, respectively; that is, they indicate the possession of contrasting ideas about their own relevance in policy matters. This raises the question of how to interpret the fact that people disqualify themselves from policy making, seeing themselves as incompetent. Challenging current conflict-focused models in the public understanding of science (PUS) literature, I argue that the hierarchical position can be interpreted as a broader desire for consensus. In the patent area, I show that paying attention to the hierarchical position is crucial to understanding why an industry representative is regarded by some to be a credible ‘expert’. The study sheds light on the ‘public understanding of expertise’ more broadly, and within the bioeconomic field in particular.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. 1 I use the concept ‘the silent majority’ to denote the part of the public that has remained uninvolved in the matter of DNA patenting. This need not imply that they are in fact ‘silent’ as testified by the increasing use of public consultation (e.g. Gottweis et al., 2008).

  2. 2 This extremely brief sketch of cPUS and tPUS is bound to stereotype the traditions; I do this for analytic purposes so as to bring out some common ontological assumptions, attention to which may help us see where our explanations have limited explanatory power. This, I return to in the discussion. For a more elaborate discussion of the contrast between cPUS and tPUS, see e.g. Horst (2003).

  3. 3 To those who find the definition of ‘expertise’ in terms of who gets to define an issue in public a somewhat tendentious redefinition of the concept, I partly agree: even when the account of a phenomenon (say, the risk of pesticides) given by a faction of ‘experts’ does not become the favoured one, we would still consider them to be ‘experts’—indeed, this will be a point in what follows. But reserving the concept for this meaning may nonetheless serve analytic purposes in foregrounding certain aspects of the formation of ‘expertise’.

  4. 4 I am, however, sympathetic to the idea of a normative definition of ‘expertise’, i.e. of what should count as ‘expertise’ in isolation from its definition in public (cf. Collins and Evans, 2002; Evans and Collins, 2008), but this is not the focus here.

  5. 5 Clearly, the provision of information about genetics is underpinned by the assumption that people are ‘deficient’, but does not thereby reproduce the ‘deficit model’ (understood as the assumption that attitudes informed by certain ‘facts’ are necessarily more politically valid) because respondents (1) are allowed to openly reflect on and contextualize the information and (2) are not told what conclusions to draw from the information. Thus, while risking framing the genetics issue, it was mainly intended as a vocabulary for reading about the regulation of biotech patenting.

  6. 6 The symmetry, however, was not perfect in two respects: two were individuals and the others were organizations. The latter authored their own texts, whereas the former were interviewed as a basis of accessible texts. This may have led to these being the preferred references. Nevertheless, in terms of credibility, they were treated on an equal footing.

  7. 7 Given the range of concerns expressed later in the interviews, it was somewhat puzzling to hear this respondent's impression that the provision of information, at the previous session, had in effect made the respondents ‘realize that it is quite secure’.

References

  • Andreasen M. (forthcoming). Two stories about biotech patenting from ‘the silent majority’ in Europe. Public Understanding of Science.

  • Beck U. (1992). Risk society—Towards a new modernity. London: SAGE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blok A., Jensen M., & Kaltoft P. (2008). Social identities and risk: Expert and lay imaginations on pesticide use. Public Understanding of Science, 17, 189–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boltanski L., & Thévenot L. (1999). The sociology of critical capacity. European Journal of Social Theory, 2, 359–377.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caulfield T., Einsiedel E., Merz J.F., & Nicol D. (2006). Trust, patents and public perceptions: The governance of controversial biotechnology research. Nature Biotechnology, 24, 1352–1354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins H.M., & Evans R. (2002). The third wave of science studies: Studies of expertise and experience. Social Studies of Science, 32, 235–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daston L., & Galison P. (1992). The image of objectivity. Representation, 40, 81–128.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demant J. (n.d.). Focus groups as social experiments: The making of interesting data. Qualitative Inquiry.

  • Douglas M. (1978). Cultural bias. London: Royal Anthropological Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas M. (1992). Risk and blame: Essays in cultural theory. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Douglas M. (1996). Thought styles. London: SAGE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutfield G. (2003). Intellectual property rights and the life science industries. Aldershot: Ashgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Einsiedel E.F. (2008). Publics and gene patents. In Einsiedel E.F. (Ed.), Emerging technologies: Hindsight and foresight, 51–63. Vancouver: University of British Colombia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans R. (2008). The sociology of expertise: The distribution of social fluency. Sociology Compass, 2, 281–298.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans R., & Collins H. (2008). Expertise: From attribute to attribution and back again? In Hackett E.J. Amsterdamska O. Lynch M. & Wajcman J. (Eds), Handbook of science and technology studies, 609–630. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gaskell G.S.S., Allansdottir A., Allum N.C., Corchero C., Fischler C., Hampel J. et al. (2006). Europeans and biotechnology in 2005: Patterns and trends. Eurobarometer 64.3. Bruxelles: DG Research, European Commission.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giddens A. (1991). The consequences of modernity. Cambridge: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gieryn T.F. (1995). Boundaries of science. In Jasanoff S., Markle G.E., Petersen J.C., & Pinch T. (Eds), Handbook of science and technology studies, 393–443. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gieryn T.F. (1999). Cultural boundaries of science - Credibility on the line. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gold R.E., & Gallochat A. (2001). The European biotech directive—Past as prologue. European Law Journal, 7, 331–366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottweis H., Braun K., Haila Y., Hajer M., Loeber A., Metzler I. et al. (2008). Participation and the new governance of life. BioSocieties, 3, 265–286.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haddow G., Laurie G., Cunningham-Burley S., & Hunter K.G. (2007). Tackling community concerns about commercialisation and genetic research: A modest interdisciplinary proposal. Social Science & Medicine, 64, 272–282.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halkier B. (2002). Fokusgrupper. Frederiksberg: Samfundslitteratur and Roskilde Universitetsforlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgartner S. (1990). The dominant view of popularization: Conceptual problems, political uses. Social Studies of Science, 20, 519–539.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horst M. (2003). Controversy and collectivity—Articulations of social and natural order in mass mediated representations of biotechnology. PhD thesis, Doctoral School on Knowledge and Management. Copenhagen Business School. Copenhagen: Samfundslitteratur.

  • Irwin A., & Michael M. (2003). Science, social theory and public knowledge. Maidenhead: Open UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin A., & Wynne B. (1996). Introduction. In Irwin A., & Wynne B. (Eds), Misunderstanding science?, 1–17. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin A. (2006). The politics of talk. Social Studies of Science, 36, 299–320.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasanoff S. (2005). Designs on nature—Science and democracy in Europe and the United States. Princeton, NJ: Princeton UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasanoff S. (2003a). (No?) Accounting for expertise. Science and Public Policy, 30, 157–162.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasanoff S. (2003b). Breaking the waves in science studies: Comment on H.M. Collins and Robert Evans, ‘The third wave of science studies’. Social Studies of Science, 33, 389–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kevles D. (2002). A history of patenting life with comparative attention to Europe and Canada—A report to the European Group on Ethics in Science and New Technologies. Brussels: European Commission.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michael M. (1996). Ignoring science: Discourses of ignorance in the public understanding of science. In Irwin A., & Wynne B. (Eds), Misunderstanding science?, 107–125. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moody M., & Thévenot L. (2000). Comparing models of strategy, interest, and the public good in French and American environmental disputes. In Lamont M., & Thévenot L. (Eds), Rethinking comparative cultural sociology—Repertoires of evaluation in France and the United States, 273–306. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • NOP Family & Research Business International (1998). Public perspectives on human cloning: A social research study. London: The Wellcome Trust.

  • Nowotny H., Scott P., & Gibbons M. (2001). Re-thinking science: Knowledge and the public in an age of uncertainty. Cambridge: Policy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowotny H. (2003). Democratizing expertise and socially robust knowledge. Science and Public Policy, 30, 151–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pálsson G., & Rabinow P. (2001). The Icelandic genome debate. Trends in Biotechnology, 19, 166–171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parthasarathy S. (2005). The patent is political: The consequences of patenting the BRCA genes in Britain. Community Genetics Supplement, 8, 235–242.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters H. (2000). From information to attitudes? Thoughts on the relationship between knowledge about science and technology and attitudes towards technologies. In Dierkes M., & von Grote C. (Eds), Between understanding and trust—The public, science and technology, 265–287. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Porter T.M. (1992). Introduction. Social Studies of Science, 22, 595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sturgis P., & Allum N.C. (2004). Science in society: Re-evaluating the deficit model of public attitudes. Public Understanding of Science, 13, 55–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner S. (2001). What is the problem with experts? Social Studies of Science, 31, 123–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright N., & Nerlich B. (2006). Use of the deficit model in a shared culture of argumentation: The case of foot and mouth science. Public Understanding of Science, 15, 331–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne B. (1991). Knowledges in context. Science Technology and Human Values, 16, 111–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne B. (1995). The public understanding of science. In Jasanoff S., Markle G.E. Petersen J.C., & Pinch T. (Eds), Handbook of science and technology studies, 361–388. London: SAGE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne B. (1996). Misunderstood misunderstandings: Social identities and public uptake of science. In Irwin A., & Wynne B. (Eds), Misunderstanding science?, 19–47. Cambridge: Cambridge UP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne B. (2003). Seasick on the Third Wave? Subverting the hegemony of propositionalism. Social Studies of Science, 33, 401–417.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I thank Klaus Høyer for his never-ending comments and help in improving the manuscript. I also thank Henriette Langstrup for very useful inputs to the conceptualization of the article. Finally, I thank Maja Horst for her comments on an earlier version.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Andreasen, M. Who's Credible? Expressions of Consensus and Conflict in Focus Groups about DNA Patenting. BioSocieties 4, 25–43 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1017/S1745855209006413

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S1745855209006413

Keywords

Navigation