1 Introduction

Individuals enrolled in higher education courses are often reported as experiencing somnolence and hypersomnolence [1], however, it is currently unclear whether this differs from the general population. Chervin and Hershner [2] defined somnolence as lapses into drowsiness, consequently leading to the inability to maintain wakefulness. This definition aligns with the American Academy of Sleep Medicine's (AASM) definition [3]. Conversely, hypersomnolence differs from this, as suggested by Lammers et al. [4], and refers to the experience of excessive daytime sleepiness. The following study investigates whether students perceive their somnolence and hypersomnolence to impact their lives more significantly than their general population counterparts.

Somnolence is a public health crisis, with the European Research Society investigating predictors that leave individuals vulnerable to somnolence [5]. Somnolence is a multidimensional state [6] frequently reported among the general population and has been demonstrated to impact safety and productivity [7, 8]. There are two ways to define somnolence: by examining physiological characteristics or by self-reported measures [9]. Within this study, introspective somnolence will be the focus, using self-report measures. Introspective somnolence refers to the self-reported and perceived experience of somnolence that individuals may be exposed to, due to health or behavioural origins such as bedtime procrastination or chronic sleep restriction [10]. The experience of somnolence can become intrusive to everyday life when the ability to maintain wakefulness cannot be sustained [11]. Somnolence often occurs alongside other sleep complaints. For example, sleep disturbance is commonly experienced alongside somnolence [12, 13] and is frequently observed in literature within a higher education environment [14,15,16,17]. However, sleep disturbance and somnolence are frequently viewed in these studies as a symptom of a depressive state, not as their own phenomenon.

Kim and Young [18] demonstrate that somnolence is a frequent issue that is also identified within the general population. However, risk factors such as being male, overweight, and frequent alcohol use can also increase the risk of experiencing somnolence further [19]. These factors may leave some populations more vulnerable to somnolence than others, including those suffering from sleep-disordered breathing [20], sleep disorder patients [21], older populations [22], and those with sleep co-morbidities [23]. Further identification is required to determine whether this is consistent for all vulnerable populations.

Increasing in severity, growing levels of somnolence in the population has led to the investigation of hypersomnolence [24]. However, as demonstrated by Perez Carbonell et al. [11], hypersomnolence frequently remains underdiagnosed as the experience, and treatment, tends to be subjective to the individual. Over recent years, various populations have been identified as at a higher risk of experiencing hypersomnolence, such as public transport drivers [25], shift workers [26], and finally those within higher education [27].

Higher education students have been highlighted as a vulnerable population for hypersomnolence by Hershner and Chervin [2] who identified somnolence as a significant problem for higher education students within the USA. Further, Alves et al. [28] demonstrated the prevalence of hypersomnolence among adolescents within education, attributing this effect to long and short sleep duration. However, this study did not highlight important individual factors in their student population, such as the potential differences in circadian preference. Circadian preference may significantly impact educational samples performance, and wellbeing, as ever-changing schedules result in students being forced to act against their natural preference [29]. Equally, Marta et al. [30] associates the link of sleep deprivation, insomnia, and somnolence within academic performance, identifying the trend that those suffering from sleep disturbances have lower academic performance. Dagnew [31] once again demonstrates the prevalence of hypersomnolence among higher education students. However, Dagnew [31] also stresses the requirement for further research investigating the contributing factors that make students more vulnerable to the effects of hypersomnolence. Dagnew [31] furthers this by suggesting that until some indication of what might make students more likely to suffer from hypersomnolence, our ability to put into place preventative strategies still needs to be improved.

As outlined, recent studies have shown that somnolence is a common experience in the general population, and has an increased prevalence among students [14,15,16,17]. Yet, to our knowledge, no studies compare whether there is a difference between these populations. This study will further the work of Hershner and Chervin [2] by identifying the differences in somnolence and hypersomnolence between a student and non-student sample in the United Kingdom. In addition, key sleep parameters related to and potentially predicting somnolence within a student sample will also be examined, such as circadian preference and sleep disturbance.

2 Methodology

Participants. G*Power (version 3.1.9.2) [32] was used to calculate the required sample size. The effect size (0.3), α error probability (0.05), and statistical power (1-β err prob = 0.95), were pre-set and left untouched. The G*Power calculation demonstrated non-centrally parameter λ of 25.2000000, critical t of 2.0644393, and df of 75. Therefore, the overall sample size per group required was 84. Participants were screened and excluded if they reported a diagnosed sleep or neurological disorder before the study took place. Exclusion criteria also included individuals who identified as having an intake of stimulants, hypnotics, benzodiazepines and/or antidepressants different from their habitual daily intake.

Data from 202 participants were collected online. 94 were non-students and 108 were currently enrolled in higher education. Further demographic information is provided in Table 1, age distribution for each sample is provided in Fig. 1, and employment, education, and qualification status are presented in Table 2.

Table 1 Demographic Information
Fig. 1
figure 1

Age distribution for students and non-students

Table 2 Employment, Education, and Qualification Information

Design. An independent measures design was employed. Within this study, participants were split into student and non-student groups to determine the difference in somnolence, hypersomnolence and sleep quality.

Materials. Four sleep scales were used in this study, the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, ESS [33], Stanford Sleepiness Scale, SSS [34], the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, PSQI [35] and the Morningness Eveningness Questionnaire, MEQ [36]. Details of these scales are provided in Table 3.

Table 3 Overview of Scales

Procedure. Ethical approval was granted by the University of Suffolk Ethics Committee on 29/07/21 (RETH20/070). Participants were presented with an appropriate information sheet and consent form, outlining the purpose of the project and the potential risks involved with the survey. All participants were asked to complete the following scales in a counterbalanced order: the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, ESS [33], Stanford Sleepiness Scale, SSS [34] Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, PSQI [35] and the Morningness and Eveningness Questionnaire, MEQ [36]. Participants were also asked to provide demographic information such as age, gender, and whether they were currently enrolled in higher education.

Data Analysis. Hypersomnolence was obtained from the Epworth Sleepiness Scale, ESS [33], somnolence was acquired from the Stanford Sleepiness Scale, SSS [34], sleep quality and sleep parameters were derived from the Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index, PSQI [35], and descriptive statistics were also recorded for both the student and non-student samples. To determine the differences between those currently enrolled in higher education institutions (students) and those not currently enrolled in higher education (non-students) t-tests were performed for sleep quality (PSQI), somnolence (SSS), and hypersomnolence (ESS), as well as circadian preference (MEQ). Finally, to determine the individual contributing factors of somnolence (SSS) and hypersomnolence (ESS) multiple linear regression analyses were performed with global sleep quality, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration, sleep latency and circadian preference as predictor variables. Predictor variables were obtained from the PSQI, with circadian preference being obtained from the MEQ.

3 Results

Sleep parameters were compared between students and non-students with each mean and standard deviation listed in Table 4 below.

Table 4 Mean scores and standard deviations for each sleep parameter

Global Sleep Quality. Mean scores for global sleep quality, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep disturbance and sleep duration for the student and non-student samples are shown in Table 4. The significance level was adjusted using the Bonferroni correction to control for multiple comparisons. The adjusted alpha level was set at 0.01. There were no significant findings for the independent samples t-tests with Bonferroni corrections for global sleep quality (t(200) = 0.907, p = 0.365), sleep efficiency (t(200) = 0.809, p = 0.419), daytime dysfunction (t(200) = 0.129, p = 0.897), sleep disturbance (t(200) = 0.720, p = 0.473), or sleep duration (t(200) = 0.342, p = 0.029) between the students and non-student samples.

Circadian Preference. Mean scores for circadian preference are shown in Table 4. An independent samples t-test for circadian preference between students (M = 49.51, SD = 11.37) and non-students (M = 51.23, SD = 11.82) was not significant, t(200) = 1.04, p = 0.294, demonstrating that there was no significant difference between the students and non-students samples.

Somnolence (SSS). Mean scores for somnolence using the SSS scores are shown in Table 4. An independent samples t-test for somnolence between students (M = 3.49, SD = 1.53) and non-students (M = 3.04, SD = 1.26) was significant, t(200) = 2.22, p = 0.027, demonstrating that students had significantly higher levels of somnolence than the non-student sample.

Hypersomnolence (ESS). Mean scores for hypersomnolence using the ESS scores are shown in Table 4, and the distribution of ESS scores are displayed in Fig. 2 below. There was no significant finding for the independent samples t-tests for hypersomnolence (t(200) = 1.73, p = 0.085) between students (M = 6.86, SD = 4.00) and non-students (M = 7.86, SD = 4.19), demonstrating that there was no significant difference between the students and non-students samples.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of ESS scores for students and non-students. Note: Scores over 10 indicate excessive daytime sleepiness

Predicting somnolence (SSS) in the student sample. A simultaneous multiple linear regression was used to investigate whether global sleep quality, circadian preference, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and sleep latency predicted somnolence within the student sample. The correlation matrix indicates that somnolence correlates with global sleep quality and daytime dysfunction within the student sample (see Table 5).

Table 5 Correlations between Somnolence (SSS), sleep quality parameters, and circadian preference for students

Overall, the multiple linear regression model was significant, F(7, 99) = 2.24, p < 0.05, R2 = 0.140, with 14% of the variance in somnolence being explained by circadian preference, global sleep quality, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and sleep latency. However, circadian preference (β = 0.071, p = 0.475), global sleep quality (β = 0.346, p = 0.566), sleep efficiency (β = -0.104, p = 0.684), sleep medication (β = -0.099, p = 0.539), sleep disturbance (β = 0.057, p = 0.724), daytime dysfunction (β = 0.184, p = 0.402), sleep duration (β = -0.188, p = 0.311), and sleep latency (β = 0.042, p = 0.851) were not significant predictors of somnolence in the student sample.

Predicting hypersomnolence (ESS) in the student sample. A simultaneous multiple linear regression was used to investigate whether global sleep quality, circadian preference, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and sleep latency predicted hypersomnolence within the student sample. The correlation matrix indicates that hypersomnolence correlates with global sleep quality, daytime dysfunction, and sleep disturbance in the student sample (see Table 6).

Table 6 Correlations between Hypersomnolence (ESS), sleep quality parameters, and circadian preference for student

Overall, the multiple linear regression model was significant, F(7, 99) = 2.65, p < 0.005, R2 = 0.179, with 17.9% of the variance in hypersomnolence being explained by circadian preference, global sleep quality, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration and sleep latency. However, circadian preference (β = -031, p = 0.753), global sleep quality (β = 0.065, p = 0.912), sleep efficiency (β = 0.025, p = 0.919), sleep medication (β = -0.099, p = 0.539), sleep disturbance (β = 0.194, p = 0.223), daytime dysfunction (β = 0.321, p = 0.135), sleep duration (β = -0.188, p = 0.311), and sleep latency (β = − 0.137, p = 0.532) were not significant predictors of hypersomnolence in the student sample.

Predicting somnolence (SSS) in the non-student sample. A simultaneous multiple linear regression was used to investigate whether global sleep quality, circadian preference, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and sleep latency predicted somnolence within the non-student sample. The correlation matrix indicates that somnolence correlates with global sleep quality, daytime dysfunction, and sleep latency within the non-student sample (see Table 7).

Table 7 Correlations between Somnolence (SSS), sleep quality parameters, and circadian preference for non-students

Overall, the multiple linear regression model was significant, F(8, 85) = 4.67, p < 0.01, R2 = 0.306, with 30.6% of the variance in somnolence being explained by circadian preference, global sleep quality, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and sleep latency. However, circadian preference (β = 0.043, p = 0.640), global sleep quality (β = 0.738, p = 0.181), sleep efficiency (β = − 0.479, p = 0.038), sleep medication (β = − 0.357, p = 0.041), sleep disturbance (β = 0.097, p = 0.543), daytime dysfunction (β = 0.196, p = 0.288), sleep duration (β = -0.138, p = 0.516), and sleep latency (β = − 0.018, p = 0.942) were not significant predictors of somnolence in the non-student sample.

Predicting hypersomnolence (ESS) in the non-student sample. A simultaneous multiple linear regression was used to investigate whether global sleep quality, circadian preference, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration, and sleep latency predicted hypersomnolence within the non-student sample. The correlation matrix indicates that hypersomnolence only correlates with daytime dysfunction in the non-student sample (see Table 8).

Table 8 Correlations between hypersomnolence (ESS), sleep quality parameters and circadian preference for non-students

Overall, the regression model was significant, F(8, 85) = 2.20, p < 0.035, R2 = 0.172, with 17.2% of the variance in hypersomnolence being explained by circadian preference, global sleep quality, sleep efficiency, daytime dysfunction, sleep medication, sleep disturbance, sleep duration and sleep latency. Daytime dysfunction significantly predicted hypersomnolence in the non-student sample, (β = 0.324, p = 0.003), demonstrating that as hypersomnolence increases so does daytime dysfunction within the non-student sample. However, circadian preference (β = 0.049, p = 0.628), global sleep quality (β = − 0.673, p = 0.263), sleep efficiency (β = 0.025, p = 0.919), sleep medication (β = 0.202, p = 0.287), sleep disturbance (β = 0.151, p = 0.386), sleep duration (β = 0.324, p = 0.164), and sleep latency (β = 0.023, p = 0.93) were not significant predictors of hypersomnolence in the non-student sample.

4 Discussion

This study identified that higher education students experience somnolence (SSS) in a distinctive way to their non-higher education enrolled counterparts. In summary, significant differences were found within somnolence but not facets of sleep quality or hypersomnolence (ESS) between the student and non-student sample. Additionally, daytime dysfunction predicted hypersomnolence, but only within the non-student sample, and not within the student sample.

We suggest no differences between global sleep quality, sleep efficiency, sleep disturbance, or sleep latency between a student and non-student population. Although our results do not indicate that students have a particularly high sleep quality, they do indicate that student’s sleep quality are similar to the general population. There was no significant difference found in sleep duration between students and non-students, however, non-students did report a lower number of hours sleep than the student sample, on average. This contrasts with research such as Lund et al. [16] who highlight insufficient sleep within a large student sample. Witkowski et al. [39] demonstrated how chronic sleep restriction can occur within a student population, with sleep duration decreasing through the duration of an academic semester. Therefore, it could be suggested that academic stress contributes to reduced sleep duration alongside the progression of their academic career and the lack of significance in this finding, could be indicative of variability in the sample. Further evidence from Liguori et al. [40] also demonstrates similar results, suggesting that sleep duration can increase or decrease depending on the semester schedule. Thus, demonstrating that although sleep duration may seem, on average, standard in a student population, there is a significant level of variability in these results. Therefore, examining hypersomnolence and somnolence may be more accurate in investigating sleep health in a student population.

Conversely, as our results highlighted that students have a higher sleep duration than the general population, longer sleep duration could indicate the presence of sleep inertia [41,42,43], which could contribute to our findings of daytime dysfunction, somnolence, and hypersomnolence. Sleep extension and, therefore, sleep inertia may occur due to increased sleep time due to circadian preference. Similarly, Onyper et al. [44] further investigated the impact of late class times on academic performance within a university sample. This research [44] found that later class times reduced the experience of daytime somnolence and reduced sleep disruption. However, this effect was also paired with increased alcohol use, which was suggested to impact academic attainment more significantly than the influence of somnolence. Yet, the findings of the current study demonstrated that circadian preference was not related to somnolence or hypersomnolence. Nevertheless, circadian preference was correlated with daytime dysfunction. This may indicate that students are using interventions such as sleep extension, alcohol, or stimulant use, as a method to accommodate for this incongruency. This is supported by Stock et al. [45] who demonstrated sleep extension increases students' academic performance and health indicators, such as systolic blood pressure. The current study suggests that students have a higher sleep duration than the non-student sample, likely to promote these physical requirements, as Stock et al. [45] suggested. This study contributes to the circadian preference and education debate highlighting that circadian preference may not be as maladaptive to higher education as previously thought.

Additionally, our results suggested a significant difference between students and non-students for somnolence, reporting that students have higher levels of somnolence than non-students in the UK. This finding is supported by evidence by Hershner and Chervin [2] who identify somnolence as a significant problem within higher education in the USA. This could be explained by unique determinants that apply to the student population, such as academic stress [46,47,48]. This academic stress may also be more extreme for medical courses, which may explain hypersomnolence being frequently reported among students in the medical field [27, 49, 50]. Furthermore, this result could also be due to the transitional phase of life that students experience, as identified by Brooman & Darwent [50]. This transitional period during higher education has previously been linked to sleep parameters such as sleep hygiene [51]. Poor sleep hygiene is a factor found within higher education across many studies [52,53,54,55]. Brown et al. [51] suggests that higher education students have inconsistent sleep schedules, unsatisfied physical needs, such as thirst or hunger before sleep, noisy sleep environments, and academic stress, all of which are factors that may contribute to sleep quality and somnolence. Although sleep hygiene was not investigated in the current study, our student sample demonstrated high levels of sleep duration and somnolence. Yet, sleep latency was not identified as a significant predictor of somnolence, suggesting that inconsistent sleep did not contribute to student somnolence in this study.

Although, previous research has suggested that hypersomnolence is common within a student population [2], no research to the best of our knowledge compared this finding to the general population. Within this study comparisons between a student and non-student sample, suggested no significant difference in levels of hypersomnolence. However, the average hypersomnolence levels for both groups were below the cut-offs for clinical hypersomnolence (< 11) [56]. Nonetheless, the focus of this study remained primarily on non-clinical levels of hypersomnolence. Therefore, further research would benefit from investigating these relationships within a clinical population.

Several limitations apply to this study. As identified by Boyes et al. [56], there are gender differences in the reporting of hypersomnolence; within the current study 84% of the sample (173/204) identified as female. Female participants are less likely to score highly within hypersomnolence or complain of somnolence symptoms, therefore it is likely that this has impacted our findings. Further research into this area, should attempt to balance the gender distribution within their sample to continue the investigation into this difference, as identified by Boyes et al. [56]. However, our research still offers important insights into the female student population and their self-reported somnolence. Within this study, age was unable to be controlled due to the unbalanced range in our sample. Moving forward, research into the area would benefit from separating and investigating different ages in isolation, for example, young and mature students. Although circadian preference showed no significant results in this study, it would be beneficial to control the time of day when future studies are completed. This in turn may be able to mediate the effects of wakefulness. Finally, this study focused on hypersomnolence and somnolence, it would be beneficial in future research to examine the longitudinal effects of sleepiness as students’ progress within education. Finally, this study focuses purely on the perceptions of somnolence, and the degree to which someone perceives themselves to be experiencing somnolence. Future research could therefore highlight how this may impact manifest sleepiness, as well as, how this would impact behavioural aspects such as one’s ability to maintain attention.

To summarise, this paper identified no significant differences between the sleep health structure of students and non-students, more specifically, in the form of sleep disruption, quality, latency, and efficiency, as well as daytime dysfunction. Despite finding no significant difference in sleep duration, on further examination, students did report higher sleep duration, than their non-student counterparts, which could lead to higher risks of comorbidities and mortality [57, 58]. Interestingly, a significant difference was found in somnolence between students and non-students. We suggest that this could be caused by factors such as academic stress, as well as the transition students experience within education. Furthermore, we did not find a significant difference in hypersomnolence between our student and non-student sample. In contrast to what was previously assumed, our finding suggest that students may not in fact be at a higher risk of hypersomnolence. Nevertheless our regression models highlight variation in the contributing factors of somnolence and hypersomnolence between students and non-students. Thus, highlighting the need for further examination of somnolence and hypersomnolence in a student population.