1 Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a neurodegenerative disorder with an insidious onset that progresses through cognitive, functional, and behavioural impairment [1, 2]. AD is the second leading cause of death in Australia, accounting for 70–80% of dementia diagnoses [3]. Generally, by the time AD is diagnosed, the neurological damage caused by amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles is deemed irreversible [4]. Despite the overwhelming burden of disease, the cause of AD is unknown, and treatments are largely unsuccessful [5]. Therefore, as the population continues to age, society is increasingly faced with an impending public health crisis [6]. Consequently, there is a critical need for AD detection methods that can identify at-risk individuals early in the disease progression and help facilitate effective treatment.

In the literature, researchers often focus on pre-dementia conditions, such as mild cognitive impairment (MCI), to observe AD progression and identify at-risk individuals. MCI is the transitional stage between normative aging and dementia, where an individual can maintain most functional abilities despite evidence of neuropsychological impairment [7, 8]. In turn, MCI is recognised as an important risk state for AD [7]. The prevalence of MCI in adults over the age of 65 is reported to be anywhere between 3 and 36% [7, 9]. Individuals diagnosed with MCI are at higher risk of progression to dementia, with an estimated 11–33% of individuals progressing to AD dementia within two years [6, 7]. Accordingly, MCI enables researchers to easily study a large sample of individuals that will progress to AD. In turn, there is a vast literature that examines MCI as a predictor or transitional stage of AD. For Example, psychological tests have been devised that selectively focus on the cognitive domains affected by MCI and dementia to separate healthy individuals from those with clinical symptoms [9, 10]. Nonetheless, evidence suggests that MCI is not the earliest stage of prodromal AD and that earlier disease detection is necessary for significant intervention [11, 12].

In 2018, the National Institute on Aging and Alzheimer's Association (NIA-AA) included subjective experiences of cognitive decline as a transitional phase between healthy cognition and preclinical dementia [4, 13,14,15]. Increasingly, evidence suggests that subjective memory complaints (SMC) may represent the first preclinical manifestation of AD or capture a ‘pre-MCI’ stage [11, 12]. Accordingly, SMC is a critical area of research that could help researchers to better understand, predict, and treat AD. However, SMC lacks substantive research in comparison to other similar conditions such as MCI. For example, a search for MCI returns approximately 80,000 results in the PubMed database while the term SMC nets less than 2000 results. Accordingly, in this perspective article, we review key papers associated with SMC and the prediction of MCI or AD. These articles were chosen via our research team for review in summer 2021 while situated at Western Sydney University. Each article was found using online databases (e.g., PubMed) and was included via reviewer consensus if the paper discussed SMC as a predictor or prodromal stage of MCI or AD. Accordingly, this perspective review aims to discuss SMC as a precursor to dementia and argue that it is a critical area of AD research. In turn, we outline the nature of SMC, discuss how SMC is measured, explore the presence of SMC in individuals with MCI, introduce some approaches to SMC treatment, and discuss future directions for SMC research. Consequently, we aim to highlight the current literature on SMC, discuss how SMC is a risk factor or transitional stage for MCI and AD, and stress avenues for future research.

2 What is SMC?

Many individuals report noticing subtle changes in memory and cognitive function before the detection of cognitive impairment (e.g., MCI) [16]. SMC has therefore been highlighted as important for diagnosing prodromal AD [17, 18]. SMC is defined as the subjective perception of cognitive decline compared to previous levels of functioning in individuals with normal cognition [17, 19]. Individuals with SMC experience subjective problems or changes with their memory that are rarely measurable by traditional cognitive tests [20]. These complaints can be present in individuals with cognitive impairment, such as those with amnestic or non-amnestic MCI, as well as in people with no objective cognitive decline. However, individuals with these complaints usually have greater objective memory decline, as well as lower overall cognitive performance and perceived health than individuals without SMC [21]. The relationship between SMC and actual memory function is not always clear. This lack of understanding may occur because SMC represents subtle cognitive changes that are small enough that they cannot be detected by standard cognitive assessments [22]. SMC is described elsewhere in the literature as subjective cognitive decline (SCD), subjective memory impairment (SMI), and subjective cognitive complaint (SCC). All these terms are different definitions of the same concept and generally refer to an individual’s subjective rating of their cognitive ability [23]. In this paper, we use the term SMC to encompass the overarching subjective experience of cognitive decline.

SMC is relatively common in older adults and increases in numbers and severity as people age. A review by Brigola et al. [24] found that approximately 50% of people aged 50 to 59 experience SMC, as do 63% of 80 to 100-year-olds. Brigola et al. [24] also found that there is an association between SMC and cognitive impairment. Specifically, they found that in some individuals, SMC was predictive of dementia at a two year follow up. In another study, Palmer et al. [25] found that 51% of people who developed dementia had SMC at baseline. Accordingly, it is suggested that SMC may be predictive of a progression to dementia. For example, Parfenov et al. [19] found that individuals who reported SMC were 2.15 times more likely to develop MCI and 2.17 times more likely to develop dementia than healthy controls. Furthermore, when compared to healthy controls, individuals with SMC have a greater risk of AD-associated biomarker abnormalities, regional brain hypometabolism, and atrophy of the medial temporal lobe [19, 26,27,28].

In a meta-analysis, Mitchell [14] showed that SMC is found in 42.8% of individuals with dementia and 38.2% of those with MCI, compared to a rate of 17.4% in healthy controls. The rate of SMC was significantly higher in dementia than in MCI. This meta-analysis also determined that the presence or lack of SMC alone could be an effective way to rule out a diagnosis of dementia or MCI. This discernment of dementia could be an important clinical application of SMC. However, it is difficult to draw such a causal link because SMC-related decline can also be related to other factors such as an individual’s emotional functioning and personality [29]. SMC research is further complicated by an estimated 60% of people who have cognitive impairments but do not report SMC [18]. It is unclear whether these missed reports are a mistake (e.g., lack of awareness of SMC) or because these individuals do not perceive SMC-related changes as significant or requiring attention.

Current epidemiological studies have found SMC prevalence to be 88% in individuals aged over 85 years, 25 to 50% in individuals over 65 years, and 20% in individuals under 65 years of age [23]. Evidence also suggests that SMC is linked to increased risk of MCI and AD [30, 31]. Accordingly, SMC must be further studied as a transitional stage or risk factor for AD. In turn, advancements in SMC research could lead to the development of detection methods that can identify individuals with SMC and direct future treatment [32]. Research into SMC is also important to other domains beyond AD treatment and progression. For example, research into SMC could also contribute to other topics of interest such as memory decline, AD biomarkers, and disease prediction. Further research may also be beneficial not only to those with memory or cognitive impairments but also to individuals who experience SMC without associated cognitive decline or impairment. For example, Miley-Akerstedt et al. [32] found that individuals with SMC and no objective cognitive decline had more years of education, more sleep problems, and a lot to do in their daily lives when compared to those who had SMC and objective memory problems. These participants also had increased beta-amyloid levels in their cerebrospinal fluid, indicating they had a biomarker profile that was nonpathological. Accordingly, SMC must be further researched to understand the condition and its links to AD.

3 Measuring SMC

Measures of SMC are generally neuropsychological tests in which individuals rate their current memory, compare current and previous memory performance, or rate their memory in comparison to peers [18, 30]. Tests of SMC have been found to predict accelerated cognitive decline; however, research into the relationship between SMC and objective performance on cognitive measures is limited [10, 27]. Nonetheless, longitudinal testing has shown that individuals with SMC are more likely to progress to MCI and dementia than those without SMC [33, 34].

While it is well researched that MCI precedes AD, the significance of SMC in the continuum remains unclear [19]. This is due, in part, to the fact that SMC is not clearly defined or uniformly measured in the literature [30]. Assessments of SMC generally include brief questions regarding an individual’s perceived memory function as well as how changes in memory may be affecting daily living [18]. However, these measures are not always comparative and may ask individuals to perform various tasks such as rating their current memory, comparing current and previous memory performance, or rating their memory in comparison to peers [30]. This variability makes comparing study results difficult and can lead to the interference of other variables such as anxiety and depression [30, 35, 36].

Rabin et al. [36] conducted a systematic review of self-report measures of SMC. Their results found that 34 different self-report measures were used in SMC research and that studies varied significantly in the method, range, format, and timeframe when assessing the condition [36]. They also found that the Memory Complaint Questionnaire (MAC-Q) [37] and the Measurement of Everyday Cognition (ECog) [38] were the most common tests used to assess SMC. In another study, Opdebeeck et al. [35] compared the agreement of five different SMC measures in identifying individuals over the age of 65 with SMC. Opdebeeck et al. [35] found that there was limited overlap in the ability of tests to identify individuals with SMC. Critically, they found that only one individual (out of 204 participants) with SMC was identified by all five measures. This finding highlighted that no scale fully encapsulates SMC and that different measures address different symptoms of the condition. These results are worrying as SMC measures are used interchangeably in clinical practice and research. Accordingly, there is conflicting evidence regarding the ability of neuropsychological tests to measure SMC. We suggest that this contention may occur due to a lack of consistent definition and operationalisation of SMC.

While there is some argument about the validity of some SMC measures, there are also some tests that are known to accurately detect SMC. For example, some scales—such as the Subjective Memory Complaints Questionnaire (SMCQ)—have been specifically developed to evaluate the level of cognitive impairment in people presenting with memory complaints [39]. However, these measures are not widely used because most SMC research is conducted in reference to AD. In turn, valid tests such as the SMCQ are disregarded for popular tests of AD or MCI (e.g., MMSE). Accordingly, it is important to use valid tests for SMC when possible; however, it is also important to determine if more commonly used tests can detect behavioural, clinical, and cognitive differences between those with SMC and individuals in the later stages of cognitive decline. By understanding the capacity of current AD tests, researchers would be able to know whether they can assess SMC, MCI, and AD with one test or whether separate scales for SMC are required. These findings would drastically improve the validity of SMC and AD research.

4 SMC in MCI

Individuals with SMC experience subjective problems or changes in their memory. These complaints can be present in individuals with cognitive impairment, such as those with amnestic or non-amnestic MCI, as well as in people without MCI. One of the greatest differences between MCI and SMC is that, while memory complaints can be present in individuals with both conditions, objective memory decline can only be conclusively determined in those with MCI [14]. Accordingly, SMC is proposed to precede MCI and both conditions are recognised as prodromal phases of AD [40]. In the literature, SMC is regularly used as a measure when detecting MCI in research studies [18]. However, the inclusion of SMC in MCI definitions is deemed controversial, with research highlighting that SMC measures may lack sensitivity and specificity as a criterion for MCI [41]. Accordingly, definitions of MCI also require a measure of objective impairment in cognition, intact general cognitive functioning, and absence of dementia [18]. The inclusion of SMC in the classification of MCI reduces the prevalence of MCI as research has found that up to 62% of patients experiencing cognitive decline do not report SMC [18]. Explanations for this discrepancy include an individual’s ability to adapt to and cope with cognitive decline, where some people do not perceive change as significant [18].

One key area of research involving SMC and MCI is the effects of neuropsychiatric symptoms on the progression of these conditions. For example, Yates et al. [18] conducted a study to clarify the relationship between mood and SMC in people with MCI. They also sought to determine whether SMC is a prominent feature of MCI and whether SMC should be included in the definition of MCI. Yates et al. [18] assessed both mood and SMC in participants from the First Cognitive Function and Ageing Study funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC-CFAS). SMC was self-reported by the participant and included memory problems like difficulties with memory or increased forgetfulness. At baseline, 557 out of 1344 participants reported SMC. Moreover, all participants with MCI reported SMC, and there were no SMC present for individuals who had MCI without SMC due to study definitions. Yates et al. [18] showed that there was a significant relationship between SMC, MCI, and anxiety or depression at baseline. Specifically, they found that the presence of anxiety or depression (at baseline and at follow up) had a significant relationship, depression was associated with a change in cognitive status, and anxiety was not related to a change in cognitive status.

However, there is some contention concerning the relationship between SMC and neuropsychiatric disorders when assessing for MCI. In the same study, Yates et al. [18] found that anxiety and depression were more strongly related to SMC than objective cognitive impairment. Accordingly, Yates et al. [18] concluded that SMC may be a symptom of anxiety and depression, rather than related to objective cognitive function (e.g., MCI). As a result, Yates et al. [18] questioned whether SMC should be a criterion in the definitions of MCI. Consequently, this research highlights the need for the standardisation of SMC measures and definitions. Researchers should also seek to longitudinally research neuropsychiatric symptoms and their association with preclinical AD, SMC, MCI, and AD dementia.

5 SMC treatment

The identification of SMC could play a crucial role in the treatment and diagnosis of AD [32]. For example, individuals with SMC may not require a full AD or dementia evaluation and could be referred to treatment. We hypothesise that treatment for SMC would likely include psychological interventions targeted at reducing stress, improving sleep, and trying to reduce their workload. If modifying these factors does not resolve SMC, an AD or dementia assessment could then be warranted. In turn, these SMC screenings could reduce the need to refer all patients with memory concerns to specialist dementia clinics, could reduce the costs of unnecessary procedures, and may lead to an improvement in their symptoms regardless of the presence of cognitive impairment.

Given the potential of these SMC diagnoses to improve AD screening, it is also important to investigate interventions for individuals with SMC. There is lacking research on interventions for SMC, yet some studies do exist. For example, Metternich et al. [42] investigated the effect of memory training (MT) and expectancy change interventions (EC) on SMC. MT and EC are aimed at changing an individual’s beliefs and attitudes about their memory through psychoeducation, cognitive restructuring, or physical training. Metternich et al. [42] demonstrated that EC interventions had significant effects on SMC compared to those on a waitlist or in a placebo group. However, they did find discrepancies between the treatment conditions. Specifically, Metternich et al. [42] concluded that EC interventions are likely to be more effective than MT in improving subjective memory. However, MT was the only intervention type that positively affected objective memory performance. Accordingly, Metternich et al. [42] suggested that MT does have some benefit as an intervention for those with memory difficulties. MT was also shown to be the preferred intervention by individuals with SMC compared to psychological interventions like EC, or medication. These results may demonstrate that a combination of MT and EC is likely to be more effective for SMC treatment. This study also establishes the need for further research into SMC and its treatment, as it is important to understand whether treating neuropsychiatric symptoms in those with SMC, treating SMC alone, or both, has benefits for those with SMC, and whether treatment impacts on progression to MCI or AD.

6 Conclusions and future directions

In this paper, we have outlined the concept of SMC, discussed current measures for SMC, discussed SMC in MCI, and touched on some approaches to SMC treatment. Throughout all these topics there has been a constant theme of promise and contention. Specifically, prior research indicates that SMC is a promising marker of early-stage AD that could be used to progress dementia research; However, corresponding research also states that SMC is poorly understood, measured, and investigated. In this study, we present both the contention and promise of SMC to encourage further investigation. As a conclusion to this paper, we will discuss areas for further research.

To progress SMC research, four main problems must be overcome. Firstly, there is a lacking standardisation of SMC. As previously discussed, SMC has multiple names and definitions throughout the literature. These different definitions can affect the generalisability and the operationalisation of SMC research. In turn, SMC-like concepts must be properly defined and consistent throughout the literature. Such standardisation could result in more valid, generalisable, and reliable research. Secondly, there is a lack of longitudinal SMC research. Longitudinal research is required to understand the progression of the condition, its comorbidities, and its role in AD progression. Without this long-term investigation, there will continue to be confusion about the role of SMC in cognitive and neurodegenerative decline. Studies such as Opdebeeck et al. [35] have also suggested that longitudinal studies could help standardise SMC definitions.

Thirdly, clinical tests for SMC are often misused and misunderstood. Further research is required to understand the ability of tests to measure SMC and AD-related disorders (as discussed in Sect. 3). Significant research is also required to identify which tests should be used to diagnose and monitor SMC. In turn, innovations in SMC testing could result in a better understanding of the condition as well as stronger screening and treatment strategies. Finally, the last common limitation of SMC research is that it lacks an understanding of the condition’s comorbidities and relationships. For example, it is understood that SMC is a precursor to AD, however, the relationship between SMC, MCI, and neuropsychiatric disorders is not understood. Accordingly, there is a critical need for research that investigates the interactions between SMC and markers of AD. Moreover, there is also a need to understand the role of SMC in the AD continuum and its relationships to the stages of AD.

In conclusion, the standardisation, longitudinal analysis, development of testing material, and investigation into SMC’s relationships are all key areas of research. We hope that future investigation can properly detect, treat, and understand SMC as part of the AD continuum. We also hope that further research can use SMC to detect early-stage AD and help improve the lives of individuals with dementia.