Skip to main content
Log in

Debating Experimental Psychology’s Frontiers: Re-discovering Wilhelm Wundt’s Contribution to Contemporary Psychological Research

  • Arena of ideas
  • Published:
Human Arenas Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Can the contemporary academic discipline of psychology, strongly relying on experiment as ideal way of psychological research, learn from Wilhelm Wundt’s strictly limited methodical understanding of the psychological experiment? Addressing this question, I firstly draw on Wundt’s early proposal of his research programme of experimental self-observation and then proceed with his methodical argument against the Würzburg school’s application of introspection on complex psychological phenomena. Centrally, Wundt aimed at showing the unprofessionalism of the Würzburg school’s introspective approach. Holzkamp’s early analysis suggested that this strong focus on showing the “wrongness” of introspective methods will in the long-term block addressing the more important underlying question regarding psychology’s research object(s)—what can and should be accessed by introspection? Against the backdrop of cultural theoretical approaches, this seems confirmed for today’s academic landscape of psychology: These approaches namely point to the fact that is exactly the relation between the researcher, the research subject and the research object that is still undetermined in answering this question. Beyond Wundt’s methodical approach that strongly limited the experiment’s scope to the objectifiable “simple” mental phenomena—and with it reducing the introspective encounter of the researcher and the research subject to a minimum—today, experimental, quantitative approaches encompass a much broader field of application, frequently working with self-reporting questionnaires that do not address the topic of introspection at all anymore. I therefore point to the fact that Wundt's objection against the Würzburg’s school’s research practice is more topical than ever: The question of a person’s relatedness with the (cultural) world seems to also demand that researcher understand psychological data that were quantified by questionnaire also as dialogical and not per se as a purely objectified third-person perspective on complex psychological phenomena.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Observation, as Brentano characterises it, involves dedicating full attention to a phenomenon, to understand and examine it. He however argues that fully concentrating one’s attention to a phenomenon simultaneously alters the phenomenon (‘If someone is in a state in which he wants to observe his own anger raging within him, the anger must already be somewhat diminished, and so his original object of observation would have disappeared’ (Brentano, 2015/[1874], p. 30)). Brentano’s solution to this is the method of retrospection (using memory for retrieving an inner perception). This is where the approaches of Wundt and Brentano centrally diverge, as Wundt’s aim was first and foremost: keeping the moment of introspection as measurable, i.e. as controllable as possible (see above).

  2. “Um die Beobachtung von der bloßen Wahrnehmung zu trennen, genügt es, wie ich glaube, sie als seine absichtliche Richtung der Aufmerksamkeit auf die Erscheinungen zu definiren [sic], wobei dahingestellt bleiben mag, ob diese Richtung dem Eintritt der Erscheinungen vorausgeht oder sie begleitet. Nur das muss wiederum als Bedingung festgehalten werden, dass sie nicht erst den Erscheinungen nachfolgt “ (Wundt, 1888, p. 296, translation: “To separate observation from mere perception, it is sufficient, I believe, to define it as an intentional directedness of attention to the phenomena, regardless of whether this direction precedes or accompanies the occurrence of the phenomena. It is a necessary condition that it does not come after the phenomena”).

  3. Calling him an ‘opposing sparring partner’ does not imply judging their personal relationship. Indeed, Külpe was known as warm as amiable person when he worked under Wundt, during his time as research assistant in Leipzig. And even though their approaches tremendously varied when Külpe established his own research centre in Würzburg, the two seem to have kept a friendly relationship. For instance, in a letter to Külpe, Wundt (1907) wrote that he is “absolutely convinced that one could disagree in questions of science and even that it is possible to run in a fundamentally different direction, without harming the feelings of personal friendship and sincere scientific respect” (as cited in Wontorra, 2009, p. 157–158, translated by the author of this article; see also the scans of the original document available at https://psychologie.biphaps.uni-leipzig.de/wundt/letters/frmwundt/ku1907.htm). The fact that they strongly disagreed in terms of their proposed research programmes, but seemed to manage to worship each other’s position (both: in science and personally), their relationship per se appears as an interesting object of a historical analysis, that has—to my knowledge—not yet systematically been accounted for.

  4. Of course, also ideas and concepts of gestalt psychology were more heterogenous. For instance, the Berlin school of Gestalt psychology (e.g. Wertheimer, 1912) worked on quite the opposite, addressing “Gestalt” more strongly focussed on introducing purely physiological models of Gestalt.

  5. Also Messer (1906) published important work explicitly on thought in his “experimental psychological exploration on thought (“Experimentell-psychologische Untersuchungen über das Denken”).

  6. See also Mandler (2007, p. 78).

  7. This is informed by Müller’s and Pilzecker’s (1900) work on memory, and the concept that mental processes remain present in one’s conscious for a short period of time after it occurred; see also Brentano (2015/[1874]).

  8. Translation by the author of the contribution, original: “(1) Der Beobachter muß womöglich in der Lage sein, den Eintritt des zu beobachtenden Vorganges selbst bestimmen zu können. (2) Der Beobachter muß, soweit möglich, im Zustand gespannter Aufmerksamkeit die Erscheinungen auffassen und in ihrem Verlauf verfolgen. (3) Jede Beobachtung muß zum Zweck der Sicherung der Ergebnisse unter den gleichen Umständen mehrmals wiederholt werden können. (4) Die Bedingungen, unter denen die Erscheinung eintritt, müssen durch Variation der begleitenden Umstände ermittelt und, wenn sie ermittelt sind, in den verschiedenen zusammengehörigen Versuchen planmäßig verändert werden, indem man sie teils in einzelnen Versuchen ganz ausschaltet, teils in ihrer Stärke oder Qualität abstuft.“ (Wundt, 1907, 2. Die allgemeinen Regeln der experimentellen Methode in ihren psychologischen Anwendungen, available at https://psychologie.biphaps.uni-leipzig.de/wundt/viewerz.htm).

  9. In contrast to his strict method programme of experimental psychology, Wundt did not propose a strict methodical setting for his Völkerpsychologie but rather broadly suggested analysing cultural products such as language, historical sources, religious practices, and art (Fahrenberg, 2013, p. 59).

  10. Translated from French by the authors of this contribution.

  11. Translated from German by the authors of this contribution.

  12. Translated from French by the authors of this contribution.

References

  • Ach, N. (1905). Über Willenstätigkeit und das Denken. Eine experimentelle Untersuchung mit einem Anhange: Über das Hippsche Chronoskop. Vandenhoeck & Rupprecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benetka, G. (2018). Franz Brentano—revisited: (Nach-)Wirkungen auf die Psychologie. In G. Benetka & H. Werbik (Eds.), Die philosophischen und die kulturellen Wurzeln der Psychologie. Traditionen in Europa, Indien und China (pp. 155–175). Psychosozial.

  • Benetka, G. (2020). The hidden presence. Einige Bemerkungen zur Tradition des Reaktionszeitversuchs in der Psychologie. Manuscript accepted for publication in Psychologie und Gesellschaftskritik.

  • Benetka, G. (2021). Preface. In G. M. Ruggiero, G. Caselli, & S. Sassaroli (Eds.), CBT case formulation as therapeutic process. Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-63587-9.

  • Bohnsack, R. (2014). Documentary method. In U. Flick (Ed.), The SAGE handbook of qualitative data analysis (pp. 217–233). Sage.

  • Boring, E. G. (1953). A history of introspection. Psychological Bulletin, 50(3), 169–189. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0090793.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brentano, F. (2015/1874). Psychology from an empirical standpoint. Routledge.

  • Brock, A. (1991). Imageless Thought or Stimulus Error? The Social Construction of Private Experience. In W. R. Woodward & R. S. Cohen (Eds.), World views and scientific discipline formation: science studies in the German Democratic Republic Papers from a German-American Summer Institute, 1988 (pp. 97–106). Springer Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-3164-3_9.

  • Bühler, K. (1907). Tatsachen und Probleme zu einer Psychologie der Denkvorgänge: I Über Gedanken. Archiv Für Die Gesamte Psychologie, 9, 297–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (1908). Nachtrag: Antwort auf die von W. Wundt erhobenen Einwände gegen die Methode der Selbstbeobachtung an experimentell erzeugen Erlebnissen. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 12, 93–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (1909). Zur Kritik der Denkexperimente. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 51, 108–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clegg, J. (2009). Considering the foundations for a holistic empirical psychology. In J. Clegg (Ed.), The observation of human systems (pp. 167–175). Transaction Publishers.

  • Danziger, K. (1980). The history of introspection reconsidered. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 16(3), 241–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Donders, F. C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. Acta Psychologica, 30, 412–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918(69)90065-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eatough, V., & Smith, J. (2012). Interpretative phenomenological analysis.

  • Ehrenfels, C. V. (1890). Über Gestaltqualitäten. Vierteljahrsschrift für wissenschaftliche Philosophie, 14(3), 249–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fahrenberg, J. (2013). Wundts Programm und Methodik der Völkerpsychologie. In G. Jüttemann (Ed.), Die Entwicklung der Psyche in der Geschichte der Menschheit: Auf dem Weg zu einem integrativen Ansatz (pp. 55–67). Pabst Science Publishers.

  • Flick, U. (2014). An Introduction to Qualitative Research (5th edition). Sage Publications.

  • Foucault, M. (1969). Archäologie des Wissens. Suhrkamp.

  • Freeman, M. (2018). Qualitative psychology’s coming of age are there grounds for hope? In Situating Qualitative Methods in Psychological Science (pp. 100–111). Routledge.

  • Friedrich, J. (2008). La psychologie de la pensée de l’école de Würzburg – Analyse d’un cas de marginalisation. L’Homme la Societe, n° 167168169(1), 251–278.

  • Garfinkel, H. (1967). Studies in ethnomethodology. Prentice Hall.

  • Giorgi, A., Giorgi, B., & Morley, J. (2017). The descriptive phenomenological psychological method (pp. 176–192).

  • Holzkamp, K. (1979). Zu Wundts Kritik an der experimentellen Erforschung des Denkens. Forum Kritische Psychologie, 06, 156–165.

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1985). Phänomenologie der Lebenswelt: Ausgewählte Texte, 1. Reclam.

  • Husserl, E. (1986). Phänomenologie der Lebenswelt: Ausgewählte Texte, 2. Reclam.

  • Kazak, A. E. (2018). Editorial: Journal article reporting standards. American Psychologist, 73(1), 1. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • König, H.-D. (1997). Tiefenhermeneutik als Methode kultursoziologischer Forschung. In R. Hitzler & A. Honer (Eds.), Sozialwissenschaftliche Hermeneutik: Eine Einführung (pp. 213–241). VS Verlag für Sozialwissenschaften. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-663-11431-4_9.

  • Külpe, O. (1912). Über die moderne Psychologie des Denkens. Internationale Monatsscrift für Wissenschaft. Kunst u. Technik, 6, 1069–1110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, G. (2007). A History of Modern Experimental Psychology: From James and Wundt to Cognitive Science. MIT Press.

  • Marbe, K. (1901). Experimentell-psychologische Untersuchungen über das Urteil. Engelmann: Eine Einleitung in die Logik. W.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marková, I. (2003). Dialogicality and Social Representations: The Dynamics of Mind. Cambridge University Press.

  • Marx, K. (1990). Capital: Volume I. Penguin Classics.

  • Mayer, A., & Orth, J. (1901). Zur qualitativen Untersuchung der Association. Zeitschrift für Psychologie und Physiologie der Sinnesorgane, 26, 1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mey, G., & Mruck, K. (Eds.). (2010). Handbuch Qualitative Forschung in der Psychologie. Springer VS.

  • Messer, A. (1906). Experimentell-psychologische Untersuchungen über das Denken. Archiv Für Die Gesamte Psychologie, 8, 1–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Müller, G. E., & Pilzecker, A. (1900). Experimentelle Beiträge zur Lehre vom Gedächtnis. Zeitschrift für Psychologie. Ergänzungsband, 1, 1–300.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicolas, S., & Ferrand, L. (1999). Wundt’s laboratory at Leipzig in 1891. History of Psychology, 2(3), 194–203. https://doi.org/10.1037/1093-4510.2.3.194.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orth, J. (1903). Gefühl und Bewusstseinslage: eine kritisch-experimentelle Studie. Reuther & Reichard.

  • Parker, I. (2005). Qualitative Psychology: Introducing Radical Research. Open University Press.

  • Potter, J., & Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse and social psychology: beyond attitudes and behaviour. SAGE.

  • Przyborski, A., & Wohlrab-Sahr, M. (2014). Qualitative Sozialforschung. Oldenbourg: Ein Arbeitsbuch.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Radovic, S. (2005). Introspecting representations. Acta Universitatis Gothoburgensis.

  • Rosenthal, G. (2004). Biographical research. In C. Seale, G. Gobo, J. F. Gubrium, & D. Silverman (Eds.), Qualitative research practice (pp. 48–64). Sage.

  • Sacks, H. (2010). Lectures on conversation, Volumes I and II (G. Jefferson & E. A. Schegloff (Eds.)). Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Schütz, A. (2016). Der sinnhafte Aufbau der sozialen Welt. Eine Einleitung in die verstehende Soziologie , (7), Suhrkamp.

  • Schütze, F. (1983). Biographieforschung und narratives Interview. Neue Praxis, 13(3), 283–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Titchener, E. B. (1910). A text-book of psychology. Macmillan.

  • Tolman, D. L., & Brydon-Miller, M. (1997). Transforming psychology: interpretive and participatory research methods. Journal of Social Issues, 53(4), 597–603. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1997.tb02450.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torre, M. E., Fine, M., Stoudt, B. G., & Fox, M. (2012). Critical participatory action research as public science. In H. Cooper, P. M. Camic, D. L. Long, A. T. Panter, D. Rindskopf, & K. J. Sher (Eds.), APA handbook of research methods in psychology, Vol. 2. Research designs: quantitative, qualitative, neuropsychological, and biological (p. 171–184). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/13620-011.

  • Valsiner, J., Marsico, G., Chaudhary, N., Sato, T., & Dazzani, V. (Eds.). (2016). Psychology as the science of human being: The Yokohama Manifesto (Annals of Theoretical Psychology). Springer.

  • Valsiner, J. (2017). From Methodology to Methods in Human Psychology. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-61064-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vermersch, P. (2009). Describing the practice of introspection. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 16(10–11), 20–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wendt, A. (2019). Lösung oder Einfall? Über die verlorenen Spuren der Phänomenologie in der Denkpsychologie. In T. Kessel (Ed.), Philosophische Psychologie um 1900 (pp. 189–214). J.B. Metzler. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-476-05092-2

  • Wertheimer, M. (1912). Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen von Bewegung. Barth: J.A.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willig, C. (2001). Introducing qualitative research in psychology: adventures in theory and method. Incorporated: McGraw-Hill Companies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willig, C. (2019). What can qualitative psychology contribute to psychological knowledge? Psychological Methods, 24(6), 796–804. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000218.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witt, H., & Burkart, T. (2010). Introspektion. In G. Mey & K. Mruck (Eds.), Handbuch Qualitative Forschung in der Psychologie (pp. 1–18). Springer VS. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-658-18387-5_36-2.

  • Wontorra, M. (2009). Wilhelm Wundts europäische Kontakte im Spiegel seiner Korrespondenz. In I. Kästner (Ed.), Wisssenschaftskommunikation in Europa im 18. Und 19. Jahrhundert. Beiträge der Tagung vom 5. Und 6. Dezember 2008 an der Akademie gemeinnütziger Wissenschaften zu Erfurt (pp. 147–160). Shaker Verlag.

  • Wundt, W. (1888). Selbstbeobachtung und innere Wahrnehmung. Philosophische Studien, 4, 292–309.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1907). Über Ausfrageexperimente und über die Methoden zur Psychologie des Denkens. Psychologische Studien, 3, 301–360.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1908). Kritische Nachlese zur Ausfragemethode. Archiv für die gesamte Psychologie, 9, 445–459.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1918). Grundriss der Psychologie. Alfred Körner Verlag.

  • Wundt, W. (1921). Probleme der Völkerpsychologie (2nd ed.). Alfred Kröner Verlag.

  • Ziche, P. (Ed.). (1999). Introspektion: Texte zur Selbstwahrnehmung des Ichs. Springer.

  • Ziche, P. (2019). Philosophische Psychologie jenseits von Psychologismus, Phänomenologie und deskriptiver Psychologie: Oswald Külpes experimentelle Untersuchung philosophischer Probleme. In T. Kessel (Ed.), Philosophische Psychologie um 1900 (pp. 173–188). J.B. Metzler. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-476-05092-2

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Natalie Rodax.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rodax, N., Benetka, G. Debating Experimental Psychology’s Frontiers: Re-discovering Wilhelm Wundt’s Contribution to Contemporary Psychological Research. Hu Arenas 4, 48–63 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-020-00173-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-020-00173-z

Keywords

Navigation