Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Object Concepts and Their Functional Core: Material Engagement and Canonical Uses of Objects in Early Childhood Education

  • Arena of Development
  • Published:
Human Arenas Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Concept formation is a crucial milestone for cognitive development. In the last 40 years, empirical evidence obtained in laboratory settings suggested that babies have a rich conceptual system that expresses in responses to stimuli. However, very little is still known about how concepts develop in every day, ecological contexts. This is due to a lack of studies addressing (i) the intersubjective contexts of activity in which concepts develop and (ii) the meanings that objects that are part of those contexts acquire through material engagement. This paper presents a qualitative analysis of six observations from a longitudinal pilot study carried out at an infant school in Madrid (Spain), with children from ages 5 to 17 months. Children’s interactions with each other, with their teacher, and with objects were recorded every 21 days for 6 months. Results show that when children, in their first year of life, grasp the canonical uses of objects they stop interacting with isolated things and start to interact with members of functional classes. Knowledge about the canonical function of objects grants children access to new forms of interaction with the material world, with others, and with themselves. I, therefore, propose that, in early childhood, general forms of material engagement that take place within culturally organised activities are central to conceptual development. Specifically, I hold that canonical uses of objects are conceptual in nature. Results are discussed in relation to the need for more studies addressing the active role of material culture for cognition in early childhood.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Due to European data protection regulations, the videos analysed in this study are not available in public repositories.

Notes

  1. For a contemporary and richer view on affordances, see Rietveld and Kiverstein 2014. The criticism of the adaptationist model does not hold for this emerging enactive-ecological approach.

  2. It is not equivalent either to the permanence of the object referred to by perspectives adhering to the existence of an innate core knowledge about objects that is mainly dependent on phylogenesis (e.g. Baillargeon et al. 1985; Spelke 2000).

References

  • Alessandroni, N., & Malafouris, L. (2020). The materiality of concepts: From material engagement to conceptual thinging. Manuscript in preparation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alessandroni, N., & Rodríguez, C. (2017). Is CONTAINER a natural and embodied image schema? A developmental, pragmatic, and cultural proposal. Human Development, 60(4), 144–178. https://doi.org/10.1159/000478841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alessandroni, N., & Rodríguez, C. (2019). The development of categorization and conceptual thinking in early childhood: methods and limitations. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Alessandroni, N., & Rodríguez, C. (in press). On perception as the basis for object concepts: a critical analysis. Pragmatics & Cognition.

  • Alessandroni, N., Moreno-Núñez, A., Rodríguez, C., & Del Olmo, M. J. (2019). Musical dynamics in early triadic interactions. A case study. Psychological Research. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-019-01168-4.

  • Amin, T. G., & Levrini, O. (Eds.). (2018). Converging perspectives on conceptual change. Mapping an emerging paradigm in the learning sciences. Routledge.

  • Anderson, A., & Prawat, R. S. (1983). When is a cup not a cup? A further examination of form and function in children’s labeling responses. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 4, 375–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E. S., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20(3), 191–208. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90008-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baquero, R. (1996). Vigotsky y el aprendizaje escolar. Aique.

  • Barsalou, L. W., Dutriaux, L., & Scheepers, C. (2018). Moving beyond the distinction between concrete and abstract concepts. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170144. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barthes, R. (1985/1994). Semantics of the object. In R. Howard (Trans.), The semiotic challenge (pp. 179–190). University of California Press.

  • Basilio, M., & Rodríguez, C. (2017). How toddlers think with their hands: Social and private gestures as evidence of cognitive self-regulation in guided play with objects. Early Child Development and Care, 187(12), 1971–1986. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2016.1202944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Behl-Chadha, G. (1996). Basic-level and superordinate-like categorical representations in early infancy. Cognition, 60(2), 105–141. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(96)00706-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, C. (1997). Catégorisation, formation de concepts et induction: Rôle des informations perceptives et conceptuelles chez le jeune enfant. L’Année Psychologique, 97(3), 495–517. https://doi.org/10.3406/psy.1997.28972.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blomberg, O. (2018). Practical knowledge and acting together. In J. A. Carter, A. Clark, J. Kallestrup, S. O. Palermos, & D. Pritchard (Eds.), Socially extended epistemology (pp. 87–111). Oxford University Press.

  • Bomba, P. C., & Siqueland, E. R. (1983). The nature and structure of infant form categories. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35(2), 294–328. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(83)90085-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., & Arterberry, M. E. (2010). The development of object categorization in young children: hierarchical inclusiveness, age, perceptual attribute, and group versus individual analyses. Developmental Psychology, 46(2), 350–365. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018411.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Brinck, I., Reddy, V., & Zahavi, D. (2017). The primacy of the ‘we’? In C. Durt, T. Fuchs, & C. Tewes (Eds.), Embodiment, enaction, and culture (pp. 131–148). The MIT Press.

  • Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1956/2009). A study of thinking. Transaction Publishers.

  • Byers, P. (2016). Knowledge claims in cognitive development research: problems and alternatives. New Ideas in Psychology, 43, 16–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2016.03.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casasola, M., & Ahn, Y. A. (2018). What develops in infants’ spatial categorization? Korean infants’ categorization of containment and tight-fit relations. Child Development, 89(4), e382–e396. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12903.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chaigneau, S., & Barsalou, L. W. (2008). The role of function in categories. Theoria et Historia Scientiarum, 8(1), 33–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind. Oxford University Press.

  • Cohen, L. B., & Strauss, M. S. (1979). Concept acquisition in the human infant. Child Development, 50(2), 419–424. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129417.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1985). The zone of proximal development: where culture and cognition create each other. In J. Wertsch (Ed.), Culture, comunication, and cognition: Vygotskian approaches (pp. 146–161). Cambridge University Press.

  • Cole, M., & Engeström, Y. (1993). A cultural-historical approach to distributed cognition. In G. Salomon (Ed.), Distributed cognitions (pp. 1–46). Cambridge University Press.

  • Cole, M., & Wertsch, J. (1996). Beyond the individual-social antinomy in discussion of Piaget and Vygotsky. Human Development, 39, 250–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costall, A. (2012). Canonical affordances in context. Avant, 3(2), 85–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuccio, V., & Gallese, V. (2018). A Peircean account of concepts: grounding abstraction in phylogeny through a comparative neuroscientific perspective. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 373(1752), 20170128. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2017.0128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Díaz Escribano, M. (2019). The philosophy of affordances. Palgrave Macmillan.

  • Durt, C., Fuchs, T., & Tewes, C. (Eds.). (2017). Embodiment, enaction, and culture. The MIT Press.

  • Eimas, P. D., & Quinn, P. C. (1994). Studies on the formation of perceptually based basic-level categories in young infants. Child Development, 65(3), 903–917. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00792.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, A. E., & Oakes, L. M. (2006). Infants flexibly use different dimensions to categorize objects. Developmental Psychology, 42(6), 1000–1011. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.6.1000.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y. (1987). Learning by expanding: an activity-theoretical approach to developmental research. Orienta-Konsultit.

  • Estrada, L. (2019). Materialidad y prácticas educativas en la Escuela Infantil con niños entre 1–-2 años: Una aproximación cultural, semiótica y pragmática (Ph.D. Dissertation, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid). Retrieved from https://repositorio.uam.es/handle/10486/688559

  • Fagan, J. F. (1976). Infants’ recognition of invariant features of faces. Child Development, 47(3), 627–638. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferry, A. L., Hespos, S. J., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). Categorization in 3- and 4-month-old infants: an advantage of words over tones. Child Development, 81(2), 472–479. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01408.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gabora, L., Rosch, E., & Aerts, D. (2008). Toward an ecological theory of concepts. Ecological Psychology, 20(1), 84–116. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407410701766676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2017). Enactivist interventions. Oxford University Press.

  • Gergen, K. J. (2003). Self and community in the new floating worlds. In K. Nyíri (Ed.), Communication in the 21st century: Essays on society, self and politics (pp. 103–114). Passagen Verlag.

  • Gibson, J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin.

  • Gibson, J. (1979/2015). The ecological approach to visual perception. Psychology Press.

  • Greco, C., Hayne, H., & Rovee-Collier, C. (1990). Roles of function, reminding, and variability in categorization by 3-month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(4), 617–633. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.4.617.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, C. W., Clark, M. G., Hroar Klempe, S., & Valsiner, J. (Eds.). (2015). Constraints of agency: explorations of theory in everyday life. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-10130-9.

  • Harnad, S. (2005). To cognize is to categorize: cognition is categorization. In H. Cohen & C. Lefebvre (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (1st ed., pp. 19–44). Elsevier https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-008044612-7/50056-1.

  • Hayne, H., Rovee-Collier, C., & Perris, E. E. (1987). Categorization and memory retrieval by three-month-olds. Child Development, 58(3), 750. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130212.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hetherington, S. (2011). How to know. A practicalist conception of knowledge. Wiley-Blackwell.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1900). Psychology. Henry Holt and Company.

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1996). Beyond modularity. A developmental perspective on cognitive science. The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemler Nelson, D. G. (1990). When experimental findings conflict with everyday observations: reflections on children’s category learning. Child Development, 61(3), 606–610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klausmeier, H. J., & Allen, P. S. (1978). Cognitive development of children and youth: a longitudinal study. Academic Press.

  • Lakoff, G. (1990). Women, fire and dangerous things. University of Chicago Press.

  • Lausberg, H., & Sloetjes, H. (2009). Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES-ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 41(3), 841–849. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.3.841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991/2008). Situated learning. Cambridge University Press.

  • Le Groupe µ., Édeline, F., & Klinkenberg, J. (2013). Sémiotique de l’outil. Anasémiose et catasémiose instrumentées. Signata. Annales des Sémiotiques, 4, 409–436. https://doi.org/10.4000/signata.1029.

  • Leont’ev, A. N. (1981). The problem of activity in psychology. In J. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of activity in soviet psychology (pp. 37–71). Sharpe.

  • Locke, J. (1690/1998). An essay concerning human understanding (2nd ed.). Penguin.

  • Malafouris, L. (2004). The cognitive bases of material engagement: where brain, body and culture conflate. In E. DeMarrais, C. Gosden, & C. Renfrew (Eds.), Rethinking materiality: the engagement of mind with the material world (pp. 53–62). McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research.

  • Malafouris, L. (2008a). Between brains, bodies and things: tectonoetic awareness and the extended self. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), 1993–2002. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0014.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2008b). At the potter’s wheel: an argument for material agency. In C. Knappett & L. Malafouris (Eds.), Material agency: towards a non-anthropocentric approach (pp. 19–37). Springer.

  • Malafouris, L. (2010). Grasping the concept of number: how did the sapient mind move beyond approximation? In I. Morley & C. Renfrew (Eds.), The archaeology of measurement (pp. 35–42). Cambridge University Press.

  • Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind. A theory of material engagement. The MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2014). Creative thinging: the feeling of and for clay. Pragmatics & Cognition, 22(1), 140–158. https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.22.1.08mal.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2015). Metaplasticity and the primacy of material engagement. Time and Mind, 8(4), 351–371. https://doi.org/10.1080/1751696X.2015.1111564.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2016). On human becoming and incompleteness: a material engagement approach to the study of embodiment in evolution and culture. In G. Etzelmüller & C. Tewes (Eds.), Embodiment in evolution and culture (pp. 289–306). Mohr Siebeck.

  • Malafouris, L. (2017). Material engagement and the embodied mind. In T. Wynn & F. L. Coolidge (Eds.), Cognitive models in Palaeolithic archaeology (pp. 69–88). Oxford University Press.

  • Malafouris, L. (2019a). Mind and material engagement. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 18(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-018-9606-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L. (2019b). Thinking as “thinging”: psychology with things. Current Directions in Psychological Science. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419873349.

  • Malafouris, L., & Koukouti, M. D. (2018). How the body remembers its skills. Memory and material engagement. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 25(7–8), 158–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M., & Bauer, P. J. (1988). The cradle of categorization: is the basic level basic? Cognitive Development, 3(3), 247–264. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(88)90011-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M., & McDonough, L. (1998). Studies in inductive inference in infancy. Cognitive Psychology, 37(1), 60–96. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0691.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J. M., Fivush, R., & Reznick, J. S. (1987). The development of contextual categories. Cognitive Development, 2(4), 339–354. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(87)80012-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mareschal, D., & Quinn, P. C. (2001). Categorization in infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(10), 443–450. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01752-6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Melser, D. (2004). The act of thinking. The MIT Press.

  • Millikan, R. G. (2000). On clear and confused ideas. An essay about substance concepts. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Moreno-Núñez, A., Rodríguez, C., & Del Olmo, M. J. (2017). Rhythmic ostensive gestures: how adults facilitate infants’ entrance into early triadic interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 49, 168–181. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.09.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C. (2014). Le référent dans l’intersubjectivité secondaire: Un objet aussi ignoré que ‘l’autre face de la lune’? In C. Moro, N. Muller Mirza, & P. Roman (Eds.), L’intersubjectivité en questions. Agrégat ou nouveau concept fédérateur pour la psychologie? (pp. 69–106). Antipodes.

  • Moro, C. (2015). Material culture: Still ‘terra incognita’ for psychology today? Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 11(2), 172–176. https://doi.org/10.5964/ejop.v11i2.995.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Moro, C., & Rodríguez, C. (2005). L’objet et la construction de son usage chez le bébé: Une approche sémiotique du développement préverbal. Peter Lang.

  • Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. The MIT Press.

  • Nelson, K. (1973). Some evidence for the cognitive primacy of categorization and its functional basis. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 19(1), 21–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1983). Concepts, words, and experiments: Comment on “When is a cup not a cup?” by Anderson and Prawat. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 29(4), 387–394.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (1985). Making sense. Academic.

  • Noë, A. (2005). Against intellectualism. Analysis, 65(4), 278–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Madagain, C. (2018). Outsourcing concepts. Social externalism, the extended mind, and the expansion of our epistemic capacity. In J. A. Carter, A. Clark, J. Kallestrup, S. O. Palermos, & D. Pritchard (Eds.), Socially extended epistemology (pp. 24–35). Oxford University Press.

  • Oakes, L. M. (2008). Categorization skills and concepts. In M. M. Haith & J. B. Benson (Eds.), Encyclopedia of infant and early childhood development (pp. 249–259). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012370877-9.00043-8.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Oakes, L. M., & Rakison, D. H. (2003). Issues in the early development of concepts and categories. In D. H. Rakison & L. M. Oakes (Eds.), Early category and concept development (pp. 3–23). Oxford University Press.

  • Overmann, K. A., & Malafouris, L. (2018). Situated cognition. In H. Callan (Ed.), The International Encyclopedia of Anthropology (pp. 1–8). https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118924396.wbiea2201.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Palermos, S. O., & Tollefsen, D. P. (2018). Group know-how. In J. A. Carter, A. Clark, J. Kallestrup, S. O. Palermos, & D. Pritchard (Eds.), Socially extended epistemology (pp. 112–131). Oxford University Press.

  • Park, Y., & Casasola, M. (2015). Plain or decorated? Object visual features matter in infant spatial categorization. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 105–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.07.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, S. (2002). Evidence for knowledge-based category discrimination in infancy. Child Development, 73(4), 1016–1033. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pauen, S., Birgit, T., Hoehl, S., & Bechtel, S. (2015). Show me the world: Object categorization and socially guided object learning in infancy. Child Development Perspectives, 9(2), 111–116. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1907/1998). Pragmatism. In N. Houser (Ed.), The essential Peirce. Volume 2 (1893–-1913) (pp. 398–433). Indiana University Press.

  • Piaget, J. (1937/2000). The construction of reality in the child. Routledge.

  • Piaget, J. (1951/1999). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. Routledge.

  • Poulin-Dubois, D., & Pauen, S. (2017). The development of object categories: what, when, and how? In H. Cohen & C. Lefebvre (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science (2nd ed., pp. 653–671). Elsevier.

  • Preucel, R. W. (2006). Archaeological semiotics. Blackwell Publishing.

  • Prior, J., & van Herwegen, J. (2016). Practical research with children. Psychology Press.

  • Quinn, P. C. (2004). Development of subordinate-level categorization in 3- to 7-month-old infants. Child Development, 75(3), 886–899. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00712.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, P. C., & Eimas, P. D. (1996). Perceptual cues that permit categorical differentiation of animal species by infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63(1), 189–211. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0047.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, P. C., & Johnson, M. H. (2000). Global-before-basic object categorization in connectionist networks and 2-month-old infants. Infancy, 1(1), 31–46. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0101_04.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Quinn, P. C., Eimas, P. D., & Rosenkrantz, S. L. (1993). Evidence for representations of perceptually similar natural categories by 3-month-old and 4-month-old infants. Perception, 22(4), 463–475. https://doi.org/10.1068/p220463.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rakison, D. H., & Yermolayeva, Y. (2010). Infant categorization. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(6), 894–905. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (1991). Playing with others’ expectations: teasing and mucking about in the first year. In A. Withen (Ed.), Natural theories of mind (pp. 143–158). Blackwell.

  • Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Harvard University Press.

  • Reddy, V., & Mireault, G. (2015). Teasing and clowning in infancy. Current Biology, 25(1), R20–R23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ribot, T. (1899). The evolution of general ideas. The Open Court Publishing Company.

  • Rietveld, E., & Kiverstein, J. (2014). A rich landscape of affordances. Ecological Psychology, 26(4), 325–352. https://doi.org/10.1080/10407413.2014.958035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C. (2006). Del ritmo al símbolo. Los signos en el nacimiento de la inteligencia. Horsori.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C. (2007). Object use, communication, and signs: the triadic basis of early cognitive development. In J. Valsiner & A. Rosa (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of sociocultural psychology (pp. 257–276). Cambridge University Press.

  • Rodríguez, C. (2012). The functional permanence of the object: a product of consensus. In E. Martí & C. Rodríguez (Eds.), After Piaget (pp. 123–150). Transaction Publishers.

  • Rodríguez, C. (2015). The connection between language and the world: a paradox of the linguistic turn? Integrative Psychological and Behavioral Science, 49(1), 89–103. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-014-9274-2.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (1998). El uso convencional también hace permanentes a los objetos. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 84, 67–83. https://doi.org/10.1174/021037098760378793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (1999). El mágico número tres. Cuando los niños aún no hablan. Paidós.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., & Moro, C. (2008). Coming to agreement: object use by infants and adults. In J. Zlatev, T. P. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind (pp. 89–114) John Benjamins.

  • Rodríguez, C., & Palacios, P. (2007). Do private gestures have a self-regulatory function? A case study. Infant Behavior and Development, 30(2), 180–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.02.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodríguez, C., Benassi, J., Estrada, L., & Alessandroni, N. (2017). Early social interactions with people and objects. In A. Slater & G. Bremner (Eds.), An introduction to developmental psychology (3rd ed., pp. 213–258). Wiley.

  • Rodríguez, C., Basilio, M., Cárdenas, K., Cavalcante, S., Moreno-Núñez, A., Palacios, P., & Yuste, N. (2018). Object pragmatics: culture and communication, the bases for early cognitive development. In A. Rosa & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of socio-cultural psychology (2nd ed., pp. 223–244). Cambridge University Press.

  • Rolla, G. (2019). Reconceiving rationality: situating rationality into radically enactive cognition. Synthese. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02362-y.

  • Rosch, E. (1999). Reclaiming concepts. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11–12), 61–77.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ross, B. H. (1997). The use of categories affects classification. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(2), 240–267. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2515.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruba, A. L., Johnson, K. M., Harris, L. T., & Wilbourn, M. P. (2017). Developmental changes in infants’ categorization of anger and disgust facial expressions. Developmental Psychology, 53(10), 1826–1832. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000381.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ruff, H. A. (1978). Infant recognition of the invariant form of objects. Child Development, 49(2), 293–306. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128690.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. Hutchinson House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Pollak, S. D., Seibel, R. L., & Shkolnik, A. (2007). Dog is a dog is a dog: infant rule learning is not specific to language. Cognition, 105(3), 669–680. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sakharov, L. S. (1990). Methods for investigating concepts. Soviet Psychology, 28(4), 35–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Säljö, R. (2018). Conceptual change, materiality and hybrid minds. In T. G. Amin & O. Levrini (Eds.), Converging perspectives on conceptual change. Mapping an emerging paradigm in the learning sciences (pp. 113–120). Routledge.

  • Schlesinger, I. M. (1982). Steps to language: toward a theory of native language acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Schmuckler, M. A. (2001). What is ecological validity? A dimensional analysis. Infancy, 2(4), 419–436. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327078IN0204_02.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwitzgebel, E. (2013). A dispositional approach to attitudes: thinking outside of the belief box. In N. Nottelmann (Ed.), New essays on belief (pp. 75–99). Palgrave Macmillan.

  • Shaughnessy, J. J., Zechmeister, E. B., & Zechmeister, J. S. (2003). Research methods in psychology (9th ed.). McGraw Hill.

  • Smith, E. E., & Medin, D. L. (1981). Categories and concepts. Harvard University Press.

  • Spelke, E. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychologist, 55(11), 1233–1243. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.11.1233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stavans, M., & Baillargeon, R. (2018). Four-month-old infants individuate and track simple tools following functional demonstrations. Developmental Science, 21(1), e12500. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12500.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Stahl, D. (2005). Sensitivity to triadic attention in early infancy. Developmental Science, 8(4), 333–343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00421.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tapparel, S. (2014). Se développer en situation éducative dans une institution de la petite enfance: Le rôle de l’objet matériel en situation peinture. In C. Moro & N. Muller Mirza (Eds.), Sémiotique, culture et développement psychologique (pp. 141–158). Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.

  • Tilley, C. (2006). Objectification. In C. Tilley, W. Keane, S. Küchler, M. Rowlands, & P. Spyer (Eds.), Handbook of material culture (pp. 60–73). SAGE.

  • Toulmin, S. (1999). Knowledge as shared procedures. In Y. Engeström, R. Miettinen, & R.-L. Punamäki-Gitai (Eds.), Perspectives on activity theory (pp. 53–64). Cambridge University Press.

  • Vaesen, K. (2012). From individual cognition to populational culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(4), 245–262. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11002196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2007). Culture in minds and societies. SAGE.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vega Encabo, J. (2010). Los saberes de Odiseo. Una filosofía de la técnica. EUdeBA.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Uexküll, J. (1928). Theoretische Biologie. Springer-Verlag.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1930/1998). Pedology of the adolescent. In R. W. Rieber (Ed.), The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky (1896–-1934). Volume 5: Child psychology (pp. 3–186). Springer.

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/2008). Thought and language. The MIT Press.

  • Wallon, H. (1942/1970). De l’acte à la pensée. Flammarion.

  • Wallon, H., & Ascoli, G. (1950). Comment l’enfant sait classer les objets. Enfance, 3(1), 411–433. https://doi.org/10.3406/enfan.1950.2196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wertsch, J. (1998). Mind as action. Oxford University Press.

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953/2009). Philosophical investigations (G. E. M. Anscombe, P. M. S. Hacker, & J. Schulte, Trans). Wiley-Blackwell.

Download references

Acknowledgements

I am most grateful to Cintia Rodríguez and Lambros Malafouris for their comments, support and suggestions that improved previous versions of this paper. My thanks also go to the members of the DETEDUCA team (Desarrollo Temprano y Educación, UAM, Spain) and the participants of the METhODS (Material Engagement Theory Oxford Discussion Seminar, University of Oxford, UK) for the enriching discussions on this article and the topics it covers.

Funding

This research is part of the project FPU16/05358 funded by the Ministry of Education, Culture and Sport (Spain) and the project EDU2015-64129-P funded by the Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (Spain).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

NA designed the study, collected the data, performed the data analysis and wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicolás Alessandroni.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

The research ethics committee of Universidad Autónoma de Madrid (Spain) has reported favourably on this research under the code CEI-88-1666 (July 5th, 2018).

Informed Consent

I declare that, for each child, parents signed a written informed consent to participate/for publication.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Alessandroni, N. Object Concepts and Their Functional Core: Material Engagement and Canonical Uses of Objects in Early Childhood Education. Hu Arenas 4, 172–195 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-020-00119-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-020-00119-5

Keywords

Navigation