Skip to main content
Log in

The Importance of Worldviews for Developmental Psychology

  • Arena of Development
  • Published:
Human Arenas Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This article aims to examine the impact of worldviews on research in developmental psychology. To do so, we use the notion of the epistemic framework and recover aspects of other epistemological approaches to meta-theoretical problems. First, we differentiate the epistemic framework of the split from the relational epistemic framework, which underlie different investigations. We argue in favour of the latter for the promotion of research on knowledge development. Then, we raise the question of ethical and political values and their epistemic significance for developmental psychology. After that, we specify the modalities of the intervention of ontological and epistemological presuppositions and values in the circumscription of the object of enquiry, the formulation of problems, and the definition of methodological aspects. In this respect, we reflect upon the social regulation of the intervention of epistemic frameworks. Finally, we discuss a central question: does giving a relevant space to values in research force us to abandon the objectivity of knowledge or to reformulate it in non-positivist terms?

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Not applicable.

Notes

  1. We refer to ideology in a very wide sense, which is not limited to the Marxist version of a legitimization of the social order, but rather that refers to symbols and meanings with which natural and social life is apprehended (for a wider discussion of the different meanings of “ideology”; see Eagleton 1997). It should be noted that, in earlier works, Piaget always showed himself in favour of a conception of science as opposed to ideology. In this sense, it can be assumed that this is a significant change in Piaget’s approach, which originated in the collaboration with García.

References

  • Adams, G., Gómez Ordóñez, L., Kurtiş, T., Molina, L. E., & Dobles, I. (2017). Notes on decolonizing psychology: from one special issue to another. South African Journal of Psychology, 47(4), 531–541. https://doi.org/10.1177/0081246317738173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amin, T. G., & Valsiner, J. (2004). Coordinating operative and figurative knowledge. Piaget, Vigotsky, and Beyond. In J. Carpendale, & U. Müller (Eds.), Social interaction and the development of knowledge (pp. 87–109). Lawrence Associates Publishers.

  • Alessandroni, N. (2020). Object concepts and their functional core: Material engagement and canonical uses of objects in early childhood education. Human Arenas. (in press).

  • Anderson, E. (2004). Uses of value judgments in science: a general argument, with lessons from a case study of feminist research on divorce. Hypatia, 19(1), 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bang, J. (2008). Commentary: building a new house out of old materials and sharpened tools. Culture & Psychology, 14(1), 37–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X07085811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R. (1987). Objet permanence in 3½-and-4½-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 655–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhaskar, R. (1993). Reclaiming reality: a critical introduction to contemporary philosophy. Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (2003). El oficio de científico. Anagrama.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P., & Wacquant, L. (2006). Hacia una sociología reflexiva. Siglo XXI.

  • Brizuela, B. M., & Scheuer, N. (2015). Investigating cognitive change as a dynamic process. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 39(4), 627–660. https://doi.org/10.1080/02103702.2016.1223710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: domain specificity in cognition and culture (p. 169–200). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511752902.008.

  • Castorina, J. A. (2019). El marco epistémico en la teoría constructivista de Rolando García. En J. González (Ed.), ¡No está muerto quien pelea! Homenaje a Rolando García (pp. 230–251). Instituto de Investigaciones Interdisciplinarias. Facultad de Humanidades. Universidad Autónoma de México.

  • Castorina, J. A. (2016). Las concepciones del mundo y los valores en la investigación psicológica. Cadernos de Pesquisa, 46(160), 362–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Castorina, J. A. (2014). La explicación para las novedades del desarrollo psicológico y su relación con las metateorías. In A. Talak (Ed.), Las Explicaciones en Psicología Prometeo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castorina, J. A., & Baquero, R. (2005). La dialéctica y la psicología del desarrollo. Amorrortu.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castorina, J. A., Faigenbaun, G., & Clemente, F. (2002). El enfoque piagetiano en el enfoque del juicio moral: alternativas frente al naturalismo y el relativismo. Estudios de Psicología, 24(2), 205–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, M. (1988). Contextuality and directionality of cognitive development. Human Development, 31, 92–106. https://doi.org/10.1159/000275800.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danziger, K. (1997). Constructing the subject: historical origins of psychological research. Cambridge University Press.

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind: three stages in the evolution of culture and cognition. Harvard University Press.

  • Duveen, G. (1997). Psychological development as a social process. In L. Smith, J. Drokwell, & P. Tomlinson (Eds.), Piaget, Vygotski, and beyond (pp. 67–90). Routledge.

  • Douglas, H. (2004). The irreducible complexity of objectivity. Synthese, 138(3), 453–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dupré, J. (2007). Fact and value. In H. Kincaid, J. Dupré, & A. Wylie (Eds.), Value-free science? Ideals and illusions (pp. 27–41). Oxford University Press.

  • Eagleton, T. (1997). Ideología. Una introducción. Paidós.

  • Elgaard Jensen, T. (2019). Exploring the knowledge practices of psychology: reflections on a field study. Theory & Psychology, 29(4), 466–483. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354319853630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elias, N. (1989). Engagement et distanciation. Flammarion.

  • Enriquez, V. G. (1992). From colonial to liberation psychology: the Philippine experience. University of the Philippine Press.

  • Fox, D., Prilleltensky, I., & Austin, S. (Eds). (2009). Critical psychology. SAGE.

  • Garcia, L. (2010). Historia, valores políticos y conocimientos psicológicos: el caso de Vigotsky y la psicología vigotskyana. Buenos Aires: II Congreso Iberoameriano de Filosofía de la Ciencia y la Tecnología.

    Google Scholar 

  • García, R. (2000). El conocimiento en construcción. Gedisa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, R. (2014). La dimensión valorativa de las ciencias. Universidad Nacional de Quilmes.

  • Gramsci, A. (1999). Cuadernos de la cárcel. Benemérita Universidad Autónoma de Puebla.

  • Ho, D. Y. F. (1998). Indigenous psychologies: Asian perspectives. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29(1), 88–103. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022198291005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horkheimer. (1992). Traditional and critical theory. In D. Ingram & J. Simon-Ingram (Eds.), Critical theory: the essential readings (pp. 239–254). New York: Parangon House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inagaki, K., & Hatano, G. (2008). Conceptual change in naïve biology. In S. Vosniadou (Ed.), International handbook of research on conceptual change (pp. 240–262). Routledge.

  • Jameson, F (2009) Valences of the dialectic. Verso.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koyre, A. (1994). Pensar la ciencia. Paidós.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, T. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago University Press.

  • Lacey, H. (2003). The behavioral scientist qua scientist make value judgments. Behavior and Philosophy, 31, 209–223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laudan, J. (1986). El progreso y sus problemas. Encuentro.

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice. Cambridge University Press.

  • Longino, H. (2015). The social dimensions of scientific knowledge. In E. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved from https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/scientific-knowledge-social

  • Longino, H. (2002). The fate of knowledge. Princeton University Press.

  • Longino, H. (1998). Values and objectivity. In M. Courd & J. Cover (Eds.), Philosophy of science. The central issues (pp. 170–191) W. Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martí, E, & Rodriguez, C. (2012) (Eds.) After Piaget. Transaction Publishers.

  • Martí, E. (2012). Thinking with signs: from symbolic actions to external systems of representations. In E. Marti & C. Rodriguez (Eds.), After Piaget (pp. 151–170). Transaction Publishers.

  • Marx, K. (1867/2001). El capital. Siglo XXI.

  • Matusov, E. (2008). Applying a sociocultural approach to Vygotskian academia: “Our tsar isn’t like yours, and yours isn’t like ours”. Culture & Psychology, 1, 5–36. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X07085808.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Matusov, E. (1998). When solo activity is not privileged: participation and internalization. Human Development, 41, 326–349. https://doi.org/10.1159/000022595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moscovici, S. (1976) La psychanalyse, son image et son public. PUF.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K. (2014). Pathways from infancy to the community of shared minds. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 37, 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/02103702.2014.881654.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Omodeo, P. D. (2016). Political epistemology. Springer.

  • Overton, W. (1994). Contexts of meaning: the computational and the embodied mind. In W. Overton & D. Palermo (Eds.), The nature and ontogenesis of meaning (pp. 1–18). Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Overton, W. (2006). Developmental psychology: philosophy, concepts and methodology. In W. Damon & R. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp.18–88). Wiley.

  • Overton, W. (2007). A coherent metatheory for dynamic systems: relational organicism-contextualism. Human Development, 59, 154–159. https://doi.org/10.1159/000100944.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Overton, W. (2012). Evolving scientific paradigms: retrospective and prospective. In L. L’Abate (Ed.), Paradigms in theory construction. Springer.

  • Overton, W. F. (2014). The process-relational paradigm and relational-developmental-systems metamodel as context. Research in Human Development, 11(4), 323–331. https://doi.org/10.1080/15427609.2014.971549.

  • Piaget, J., & García, R. (1983). Psicogénesis e historia de la ciencia. Siglo XXI.

  • Psaltis, C., Duveen, G., & Perret-Clermont, A. (2009). The social and the psychological: structure and context in intellectual development. Human Development., 52, 291–312. https://doi.org/10.1159/000233261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, H. (2002). The collapse of fact-values dichotomy. Harvard University Press.

  • Rogoff, B. (1998). Cognition as a collaborative process. In W. Damon, D. Kuhn, & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 2 (pp. 674–744). Wiley.

  • Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. Oxford University Press.

  • Rogoff, B. (1997). Los tres planos de la actividad sociocultural: apropiación participativa, participación guiada y aprendizaje. In J. Wertsch, P. Del Río, & A. Álvarez (Eds), La mente sociocultural. Aproximaciones teóricas y aplicadas (pp. 111–128). FIA.

  • Rodriguez, C. (2012). The functional permanence of the object: a product of consensus. In E. Marti & C. Rodriguez (Eds.), After Piaget (pp. 123–150). Transaction Publishers.

  • Taylor, C. (1995). Philosophical arguments. Harvard University Press.

  • Teo, T. (2011). Radical philosophical critiques and critical thinking in psychology. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 11(3), 193–199. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024708.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teo, T. (2008). From speculation to epistemological violence in psychology: a critical-hermeneutic reconstruction. Theory & Psychology, 18(1), 47–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354307086922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2019). Cultural psychology as a theoretical project. Estudios de Psicología, 40(1), 10–47. https://doi.org/10.1080/02109395.2018.1560023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valsiner, J. (2012). A guided science. Transaction Publishers.

  • Valsiner, J. (2006). Dangerous curves in knowledge construction within psychology: fragmentation of methodology. Theory & Psychology, 16(5), 597–612. https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354306067439.

  • Varela, F.; Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1992). De cuerpo presente. Gedisa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vigotsky, L. S. (1931/1991). El significado histórico de la crisis en psicología. In Obras Escogidas, Vol. 1. Visor/MEC.

  • Weber, M. (1946). Science as vocation. In H. Gerth & W. Mill (Eds.), Max Weber: essays in sociology. Oxford University Press.

  • Witherington, D. C. (2007). The dynamic systems approach as metatheory for developmental psychology. Human Development., 50, 127–153. https://doi.org/10.1159/000100943.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wylie, A., & Nelson, L. H. (2007). Coming to terms with the values of science: insights from feminist science studies scholarship. In H. Kinkaid, J. Dupré, & A. Wylie (Eds.), Value-free science? Ideals and illusions (pp. 58–86). Oxford University Press.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The author wishes to thank Nicolás Alessandroni, editor of the special section in which this article is published, for his collaboration and suggestions on previous versions of the manuscript. Without his support, these pages would not have been possible.

Code Availability

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

JAC wrote the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to José Antonio Castorina.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethics Approval

Not applicable.

Consent to Participate/for Publication

Not applicable.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Castorina, J.A. The Importance of Worldviews for Developmental Psychology. Hu Arenas 4, 153–171 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-020-00115-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s42087-020-00115-9

Keywords

Navigation