We found that a population of 7720 individuals committed violence (with injury) during T2. Of those, 2004 are also present in the population identified at T1 (27.7%). In this paper, we focus on this latter set of individuals (target individuals) and reconstruct their network of associates at time T1: 497 individuals (out of the 2004) have an associate who has committed violence at T1 (24.8%); 279 have an associate who has carried a knife at T1 (13.9%); and 119 have an associate who has carried a weapon at T1 (23.9%). Of those target individuals, 712 have also committed violence at T1 (35.5%). Figure 1 shows the network of the target individuals (large dark grey dots) and their associates (small light grey dots).
Estimating the Determinants of Violence Through Logistic Regression Models
Next, we look at the determinants of violence at time T2 through a series of logistic regression models capturing behaviour at time T1.Footnote 4 We start by considering only the violence-related behaviour of target individuals (Table 1).
Table 1 Predicting violence in Merseyside: target individuals Model 1 is akin to a baseline model: we know from the literature that previous violence tends to be associated with higher chances of committing violence in subsequent periods, and we found support for this relationship also in our data. The odds of observing violence at T2 for an individual who has committed violence at T1 are equal to 2.37. In terms of percentage changes, the odds for an individual who has committed violence at T1 to also commit violence at T2 are 137.5% higher than for those who have not committed violence at T1 (we remind the reader that in our models we are comparing against non-violent offenders, not against the general population).
Model 2 adds the effect of having been flagged at time T1 for (a) a weapon-involved incident and (b) a knife-involved incident. Having committed previous violence remains the strongest predictor; both knife and weapon have a positive effect, but only having been flagged for a weapon-involved incident is statistically significant. In terms of percentage changes, a weapon incident at T1 increases the odds of violence at T2 by 43%. (Model A1 in the Appendix Table 3 looks at the effect of knife flagging and weapon flagging when no information on previous violence is considered; in sum, both have a positive effect and show a percentage increase in the odds of violence of 87% for weapon and 43% for knife).
Next, we assess the effect of network-based indicators (Table 2).
Table 2 Predicting violence in Merseyside: target individuals and their associates Model 3 shows that the behaviour of an offender’s associates does matter in explaining future violence. This holds true also when controlling for the past violent behaviour of a target individual. In other words, the information about the associates adds to our understanding of the phenomenon. Prior association with a violent individual at time T1 increases the odds of committing violence at time T2 by 28%. (In Model A2 in the Appendix Table 4, we estimate the model considering only the information on the associates’ behaviour without any information on the past violent behaviour of a target individual: the positive effect holds true, and points to an increase in the odds of committing violence at T2 of 61%).
In Model 4, we jointly consider the full set of indicators: individual-based and network-based. Previous violence by a target individual remains a strong predictor of future violence: there is a + 124% increase in the odds of committing violence at T2 compared with an offender who has not committed violence at T1. Secondly, the behaviour of the associates continues to matter: prior association with an individual who has committed violence at T1 increases the odds of committing violence at T2 by 16%; prior association with an individual who has been flagged for knife incident increases the odds by slightly more (+ 20%).
We do not find, however, any effect in Model 4 for prior association with an individual flagged for a weapon incident. Finally, when adding the associates’ behaviour to the model, the fact that a target individual has been flagged for a knife incident loses predictive power—showing no effect in Model 4. Weapon flagging at time T1 still shows a positive effect (+ 22% increase in odds), but loses its statistical significance. This is partially due to the relative small number of weapon-related incidents in the datasets (we return on this point in the sensitivity/specificity analysis below).Footnote 5
Sensitivity and Specificity Analysis of the Indicators
Next, we contextualise the relevance of each predictor under consideration through a sensitivity/specificity analysis. We ask to what extent the fact that an individual is flagged or not with a given regressor at T1 can help a practitioner assess whether such individual will commit or not commit violence at T2.
We remind the reader that sensitivity indicates the extent to which actual positive cases are correctly classified: true positives are correctly identified and false negatives minimised. Specificity indicates the extent to which negative occurrences are correctly classified: actual negatives are identified and false positives minimised. In our context, a true positive is when a knife flag at T1 is associated with violence at T2; conversely, we expect no knife flag at T1 to be associated with no violence at T2 (true negative). A false positive is when a knife flag at T1 is associated with an individual who will not commit violence at T2; a false negative is when no knife flag at T1 is associated with violence at T2.
As there is normally a trade-off between the two measures, we need to look at them jointly to draw conclusions on the strength of each indicator relative to the predicted behaviour of individuals. Figure 2 reports the results of both sensitivity and specificity for the six predictors under consideration.
For our purposes, the key measure of interest is sensitivity as we seek to answer the following question: if we rely on, say, knife flagging at T1, what is our ability to correctly identify individuals who will commit violence at T2? To put it in another way, we are trying to minimise false negatives (e.g. no knife at T1 and violence at T2) while capturing as many true positives as possible.
The indicators show a striking heterogeneity in the degree of sensitivity. The best performer is past violence (violence T1): 36% of all individuals observed at T1 who then commit violence at T2 are flagged with violence T1. This is followed by prior association with a violent individual, which correctly identifies 25% of cases. Prior association with a knife-flagged individual correctly identifies 14% of cases. Knife flagging at T1, prior association with a weapon-flagged individual, and weapon flagging at T1 show the lowest level of sensitivity (this is in line with the results of the regression models discussed above). Weapon flagging at T1 (sensitivity level 3%) performs 91% worse than the best available regressor (violence T1). On the other hand, the sensitivity levels of network-based counterparts of these regressors are on average 98% higher.Footnote 6
As for the question of who will not commit violence with injury, all regressors considered in our analysis record a very high level of specificity. In other words, for all regressors, individuals for which violence at T2 = 0 (i.e. they do not commit violence at T2) are in most cases correctly identified by a flag (regressor) equal to 0. Overall, the maximum deviation among indicators is just 18%. While this result is partially driven by the fact that the population of interest is denoted by a relatively small number of individuals committing violence at T2 and, similarly, a relatively small number of individuals for which the flags (regressors) are positive, it is still a reassuring dimension of police legitimacy. For police to miss so few people who will commit violence with injuries when the prediction says they will not, they can justify limiting the scope of proactive policing efforts to cases in which evidence does predict future violence.
It is also important to note that the network-based indicators still perform well on this measure vis-à-vis individual-based indicators, recording a level of specificity never lower than 83%.
In sum, the joint specificity and sensitivity analysis shows that a better understanding of an individual’s co-offending network allows a practitioner to cast a more robust judgement relative to future violent behaviour. In particular, we remark that violence associates at T1 does a good job in both sensitivity and specificity tests, thus efficiently complementing the individual violence T1. For these two variables, both the presence and the absence of the flag are informative of future behaviour. Prior association with knife-flagged individuals performs slightly less strongly, but it can still offer some potentially useful operational guidance.