Skip to main content
Log in

High proficient bilinguals bring in higher executive control when encountering diverse interlocutors

  • Research Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined if bilinguals are sensitive to contextual factors with regard to the presence of interlocutors and if this reflects in how they modulate their executive control. First, we introduced Telugu–English bilinguals to monolingual, bilingual and neutral interlocutors in the form of cartoon characters through an interactive session. Following this, they performed the attention network task (ANT) with the image of interlocutors appearing on every trial before the flankers. High proficient bilinguals (in L2) were overall faster on the ANT (indicating higher executive control) when different interlocutors appeared randomly in a mixed block compared to the low proficient bilinguals. However, this effect was not found when the appearance of the interlocutors along-side the ANT task was blocked. These data demonstrate that high proficient bilinguals brought in higher executive control when the context required higher monitoring (different interlocutors appearing randomly) compared to the low proficient bilinguals. We interpret the findings with regard to the adaptive control hypothesis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Data availability

All relevant data analysed in this study are available at https://osf.io/eusqc/?view_only=85e2ae6f1b9d48f1985435085592d02b.

References

  • Abutalebi, J., Della Rosa, P. A., Ding, G., Weekes, B., Costa, A., & Green, D. W. (2013). Language proficiency modulates the engagement of cognitive control areas in multilinguals. Cortex, 49(3), 905–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Antón, E., Duñabeitia, J. A., Estévez, A., Hernández, J. A., Castillo, A., Fuentes, L. J., et al. (2014). Is there a bilingual advantage in the ANT task? Evidence from children. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Antón, E., García, Y. F., Carreiras, M., & Duñabeitia, J. A. (2016). Does bilingualism shape inhibitory control in the elderly? Journal of Memory and Language, 90, 147–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bhatia, D., Prasad, S. G., Sake, K., & Mishra, R. K. (2017). Task irrelevant external cues can influence language selection in voluntary object naming: evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. PloS One, 12(1), e0169284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A., Hernández, M., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Costa, A., Hernández, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartanto, A., & Yang, H. (2016). Disparate bilingual experiences modulate task-switching advantages: A diffusion-model analysis of the effects of interactional context on switch costs. Cognition, 150, 10–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartsuiker, R. J. (2015). Visual cues for language selection in bilinguals. Attention and Vision in Language Processing (pp. 129–145). New Delhi: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(4), 625–658.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Iluz-Cohen, P., & Armon-Lotem, S. (2013). Language proficiency and executive control in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(04), 884–899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapiley, K., & Mishra, R. K. (2018). Iconic culture-specific images influence language non-selective translation activation in bilinguals: Evidence from eye movements. Translation, Cognition & Behavior, 1(2), 221–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapa, L. L., & Colombo, J. (2013). Attentional control in early and later bilingual children. Cognitive development, 28(3), 233–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kapiley, K., & Mishra, R. K. (2019). What do I choose? Influence of interlocutor awareness on bilingual language choice during voluntary object naming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(5), 1029–1051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, Y., Yang, J., Scherf, K. S., & Li, P. (2013). Two faces, two languages: An fMRI study of bilingual picture naming. Brain and Language, 127(3), 452–462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, F., Chen, P., Guo, T., & Kroll, J. F. (2017). When late second language learners access the meaning of L2 words: Using ERPs to investigate the role of the L1 translation equivalent. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 41, 50–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McMurray, B., Samelson, V. M., Lee, S. H., & Tomblin, J. B. (2010). Individual differences in online spoken word recognition: implications for SLI. Cognitive Psychology, 60(1), 1–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mishra, R. K., Hilchey, M. D., Singh, N., & Klein, R. M. (2012). On the time course of exogenous cueing effects in bilinguals: higher proficiency in a second language is associated with more rapid endogenous disengagement. The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 65(8), 1502–1510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Molnar, M., Ibáñez-Molina, A., & Carreiras, M. (2015). Interlocutor identity affects language activation in bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 81, 91–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ooi, S. H., Goh, W. D., Sorace, A., & Bak, T. H. (2018). From Bilingualism to Bilingualisms: Bilingual experience in Edinburgh and Singapore affects attentional control differently. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(4), 867–879.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences? Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26(6), 615–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, S. D., & Abrams, L. (2014). Cognitive advantages and disadvantages in early and late bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(2), 313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Qu, L., Low, J. J. W., Zhang, T., Li, H., & Zelazo, P. D. (2016). Bilingual advantage in executive control when task demands are considered. Bilingualism, 19(2), 277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roychoudhuri, K. S., Prasad, S. G., & Mishra, R. K. (2016). Iconic native culture cues inhibit second language production in a non-immigrant population: evidence from Bengali-English bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K. (2012). Does language proficiency modulate oculomotor control? Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Bilingualism Language and Cognition, 15(04), 771–781.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K. (2013). Second language proficiency modulates conflict-monitoring in an oculomotor Stroop task: Evidence from Hindi-English bilinguals. Frontiers in psychology, 4, 322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singh, N., & Mishra, R. K. (2014). The modulatory role of second language proficiency on performance monitoring: Evidence from a saccadic countermanding task in high and low proficient bilinguals. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 1481.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singh, J. P., & Mishra, R. K. (2015). Effect of bilingualism on anticipatory oculomotor control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(550), 562.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: the role of age of acquisition. Bilingualism and cognitive control, 10, 83.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woumans, E., Martin, C. D., Bulcke, C. V., Van Assche, E., Costa, A., Hartsuiker, R. J., et al. (2015). Can faces prime a language? Psychological Science, 26(9), 1343–1352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, Y. J., & Thierry, G. (2013). Fast modulation of executive function by language context in bilinguals. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(33), 13533–13537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14(3), 412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ye, Y., Mo, L., & Wu, Q. (2016). Mixed cultural context brings out bilingual advantage on executive function. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20, 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, S., Morris, M. W., Cheng, C. Y., & Yap, A. J. (2013). Heritage-culture images disrupt immigrants’ second-language processing through triggering first-language interference. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(28), 11272–11277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank Keerthana Kapiley, Manasa Padmanabhuni and Sangeeth for assistance with the cartoons and the sentences during the familiarization/interaction phase.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ramesh Kumar Mishra.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOC 20 kb)

Appendices

Appendix 1: Correlational analysis for the executive control network

 

RT (Mixed context)

RT (pure context)

Exposure to L2

AoA of L2

L2 proficiency

RT (mixed context)

     

RT (pure context)

0.08

    

Exposure to L2

− 0.06

0.01

   

AoA of L2

0.55***

− 0.22

− 0.05

  

L2 proficiency

− 0.54***

0.26

− 0.08

− 0.65***

 
  1. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05

Appendix 2: analysis of RT in pure context

To examine whether different types of interlocutors (bilingual vs. monolingual vs. neutral) induced differential demands on performance, we performed an ANOVA on RT (only) in the pure context with group as between-subject factor and interlocutor (monolingual, bilingual and neutral cartoons) and flanker (congruent, incongruent and neutral) as within-subject factors.

There was a significant effect of flanker type, F (2, 68) = 200.44, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.855 but no significant main effect of group, F (1, 34) = 1.23, p = 0.275, η2 = 0.03. The interaction between group and interlocutor condition was not significant either, F (2,68) = 0.23, p = 0.792, η2 = 0.01. None of the other two-way or three-way interactions were significant (p > 0.1).

See Table

Table 3 Descriptive statistics of data from pure context

3.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bhandari, P., Prasad, S. & Mishra, R.K. High proficient bilinguals bring in higher executive control when encountering diverse interlocutors. J Cult Cogn Sci 4, 201–215 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-020-00060-7

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-020-00060-7

Keywords

Navigation