Advertisement

Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science

, Volume 3, Issue 2, pp 235–255 | Cite as

Language history attenuates syntactic prediction in L1 processing

  • Michael C. SternEmail author
  • Christen N. MadsenII.
  • LeeAnn M. Stover
  • Cass Lowry
  • Gita Martohardjono
Research Paper

Abstract

An eye-tracking experiment in the Visual World Paradigm was conducted to examine the effects of language history on the predictive parsing of sentences containing relative clauses in the first-learned language of fluent bilingual adults. We compared heritage speakers of Spanish (HSs)—who had spent most of their lives immersed in an English-dominant society—to Spanish–English late bilinguals (LBs), who did not begin immersion in an English-dominant society until adulthood. Consistent with studies of monolinguals, the LBs demonstrated a subject/object relative clause processing asymmetry, i.e. a processing advantage during subject relative clauses and a processing disadvantage during object relative clauses. This suggests that the LBs actively predicted the syntactic structure of subject relative clauses, consistent with the active filler hypothesis. The HSs, on the other hand, did not exhibit this processing asymmetry, suggesting less active prediction. We conclude, therefore, that decreased exposure to the first-learned language causes less active prediction in first-language processing, which causes both disadvantages, and interestingly, advantages, in processing speed.

Keywords

Bilingual processing Relative clause processing Active filler Prediction Visual world paradigm Language dominance 

Notes

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank our study participants, Richard G. Schwartz, and Second Language Acquisition Lab research assistants Daniela Castillo, Omar Ortiz, Christina Dadurian, Andrea Monge, Matthew Stuck, and Armando Tapia.

Compliance with ethical standards

Conflict of interest

On behalf of all authors, Michael C. Stern states that there is no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language,38(4), 419–439.Google Scholar
  2. Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference. Cognition,73, 247–264.Google Scholar
  3. Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linking anticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language,57, 502–518.Google Scholar
  4. Arnon, I. (2005). Relative clause acquisition in Hebrew: Toward a processing-oriented account. In A. Brugos, M. R. Clark-Cotton, & S. Ha (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 37–48). Somerville: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
  5. Bar, M. (2007). The pro-active brain: Using analogies and associations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,11, 280–289.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Bar, M. (2009). The pro-active brain: Memory for predictions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B,364, 1235–1243.Google Scholar
  7. Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software,67(1), 1–48.Google Scholar
  8. Benmamoun, E., Montrul, S., & Polinsky, M. (2013). Heritage languages and their speakers: Opportunities and challenges for linguistics. Theoretical Linguistics,39(3–4), 129–181.Google Scholar
  9. Betancort, M., Carreiras, M., & Sturt, P. (2009). The processing of subject and object relative clauses in Spanish: An eye-tracking study. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology,62(10), 1915–1929.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  10. Birdsong, D., Gertken, L. M., & Amengual, M. (2012). Bilingual language profile: An easy-to-use instrument to assess bilingualism. COERLL: University of Texas at Austin.Google Scholar
  11. Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science,14(2), 256–269.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  12. Borovsky, A., Elman, J., & Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,112(4), 417–436.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  13. Brouwer, S., Özkan, D., & Küntay, A. C. (2017a). Semantic prediction in monolingual and bilingual children. In E. Blom, L. Cornips, & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Cross-linguistic influence in bilingualism. In honor of Aafke Hulk (pp. 49–73). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  14. Brouwer, S., Sprenger, S., & Unsworth, S. (2017b). Processing grammatical gender in Dutch: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,159, 50–65.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  15. Caplan, D., Alpert, N., Waters, G., & Olivieri, A. (2000). Activation of Broca’s area by syntactic processing under conditions of concurrent articulation. Human Brain Mapping,9, 65–71.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Caplan, D., Vijayan, S., Kuperberg, G., West, C., Waters, G., Greve, D., & Dale, A. M. (2002). Vascular responses to syntactic processing: Event-related fMRI study of relative clauses. Human Brain Mapping,15(1), 26–38.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Carreiras, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., Vergara, M., de la Cruz-Pavía, I., & Laka, I. (2010). Subject relative clauses are not universally easier to process: Evidence from Basque. Cognition,115, 79–92.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Chan, A., Chen, S., Matthews, S., & Yip, V. (2017). Comprehension of subject and object relative clauses in a trilingual acquisition context. Frontiers in Psychology,2017, 8.Google Scholar
  19. Chang, F., Dell, G. S., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review,113(2), 234–272.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  20. Clifton, C., & Frazier, L. (1989). Comprehending sentences with long-distance dependencies. In G. N. Carlson & M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.Google Scholar
  21. Cooke, A., Zurif, E. B., DeVita, C., Alsop, D., Koenig, P., Detre, J., et al. (2002). Neural basis for sentence comprehension: Grammatical and short-term memory components. Human Brain Mapping,15, 80–94.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  22. Cooper, R. M. (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology,6, 84–107.Google Scholar
  23. Dahan, D., Swingly, D., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Magnuson, J. S. (2000). Linguistic gender and spoken-word recognition in French. Journal of Memory and Language,42, 465–480.Google Scholar
  24. Dussias, P. E., Kroff, J. R. V., Tamargo, R. E. G., & Gerfen, C. (2013). When gender and looking go hand in hand: Grammatical gender processing in L2 Spanish. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,35, 353–387.Google Scholar
  25. Dussias, P. E., & Sagarra, N. (2007). The effect of exposure on syntactic parsing in Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism Language and Cognition,10(1), 101–116.Google Scholar
  26. Ezeizabarrena, M. J., Munarriz, A., & Loidi, U. (2017). Bilingual production of relative clauses in languages with opposite head-complement directionality. In K. Bellamy, M. W. Child, P. González, A. Muntendam, & M. C. P. Cuoto (Eds.), Multidisciplinary approaches to bilingualism in the Hispanic and Lusophone world (pp. 283–309). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
  27. Federmeier, K. D. (2007). Thinking ahead: The role and roots of prediction in language comprehension. Psychophysiology,44(4), 491–505.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  28. Federmeier, K. D., McLennan, D. B., De Ochoa, E., & Kutas, M. (2002). The impact of semantic memory organization and sentence context information on spoken language processing by younger and older adults: An ERP study. Psychophysiology,39, 133–146.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Fernandez, E. M. (2002). Relative clause attachment in bilinguals and monolinguals. In R. R. Heredia & J. Altarriba (Eds.), Bilingual sentence processing (pp. 187–215). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
  30. Foucart, A., Martin, C. D., Moreno, E. M., & Costa, A. (2014). Can bilinguals see it coming? Word anticipation in L2 sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory, and Cognition. Google Scholar
  31. Frazier, L. (1987). Syntactic processing: Evidence from Dutch. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory,5, 519–559.Google Scholar
  32. Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language,58(3), 787–814.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  33. Grüter, T., Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2012). Grammatical gender in L2: A production or real-time processing problem? Second Language Research,28(2), 191–215.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  34. Grüter, T., Rohde, G., & Schafer, & A. J. (2014). The role of discourse-level expectations in non-native speakers’ referential choices. Proceedings of the annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Google Scholar
  35. Guillelmon, D., & Grosjean, F. (2001). The gender marking effect in spoken word recognition: The case of bilinguals. Memory and Cognition,29(3), 503–511.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  36. Hopp, H. (2012). Grammatical gender in adult L2 acquisition: Relations between lexical and syntactic variability. Second Language Research,29(1), 33–56.Google Scholar
  37. Hu, S., Gavarró, A., Vernice, M., & Guasti, M. T. (2016). The acquisition of Chinese relative clauses: Contrastic two theoretical approahces. Journal of Child Language,43(1), 1–21.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  38. Huettig, F., Rommer, J., & Meyer, A. J. (2011). Using the visual world paradigm to study language processing: A review and critical evaluation. Acta Psychologica. Google Scholar
  39. Hutcheson, G., & Sofroniou, N. (1999). The multivariate social scientist: Introductory statistics using generalized linear models. London: Sage Publication.Google Scholar
  40. Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1993). The intensity dimension of thought: Pupillometric indices of sentence processing. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology,47(2), 310–399.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Just, M., Carpenter, P., & Keller, T. (1996). Brain activation modulated by sentence comprehension. Science,274, 114–116.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  42. Kahraman, B., Sato, A., Ono, H., & Sakai, H. (2010). Relative clauses processing before the head noun: Evidence for strong forward prediction in Turkish. In H. Maezawa & A. Yokogoshi (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th Workshop on Altaic Formal Linguistics (WAFL6). MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 61 (pp. 155–170). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  43. Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. L. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing: Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language,49, 122–156.Google Scholar
  44. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). A functional approach to child language: A study of determiners and reference. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  45. King, J., & Kutas, M. (1995). Who did what and when? Using word- and clause-level ERPs to monitor working memory usage in reading. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,7, 376–395.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  46. Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software,82(13), 1–26.Google Scholar
  47. Kwon, N., Kluender, R., Kutas, M., & Polinsky, M. (2013). Subject/object processing asymmetries in Korean relative clauses: Evidence from ERP data. Language (Baltim),89(3), 537–585.PubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  48. Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2007). Young children learning Spanish make rapid use of grammatical gender in spoken word recognition. Psychological Science,18(3), 193–198.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  49. Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish speakers. Journal of Memory and Language,63(4), 447–464.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  50. Li, P., Sepanski, S., & Zhao, X. (2006). Language history questionnaire: A Web-based interface for bilingual research. Behavior Research Methods,38(2), 202–210.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. MacWhinney, B., & Pleh, C. (1988). The processing of restrictive relative clauses in Hungarian. Cognition,29, 95–141.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  52. Mak, W. M., Vonk, W., & Schriefers, H. (2002). The influence of animacy on relative clause processing. Journal of Memory and Language,47(1), 50–68.Google Scholar
  53. Mani, N., & Huettig, F. (2012). Prediction during language processing is a piece of cake—but only for skilled producers. Journal of Experimental Psychology,38(4), 843–847.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  54. Martin, C. D., Thierry, G., Kuipers, J., Boutonnet, B., Foucart, A., & Costa, A. (2013). Bilinguals reading in their second language do not predict upcoming words as native readers do. Journal of Memory and Language,69, 574–588.Google Scholar
  55. Miyamoto, E., & Nakamura, M. (2003). Subject/object asymmetries in the processing of relative clauses in Japanese. In G. Garding & M. Tsujimura (Eds.), Proceedings of the west coast conference on formal linguistics 22 (pp. 342–355). Somerville: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
  56. Montrul, S. (2010). Dominant language transfer in adult second language learners and heritage speakers. Second Language Research,26(3), 293–327.Google Scholar
  57. Nakamura, C., Arai, M., & Mazuka, R. (2012). Immediate use of prosody and context in predicting a syntactic structure. Cognition,125, 317–323.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Nation, K., Marshall, C. M., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2003). Investigating individual differences in children’s real-time sentence comprehension using language-mediated eye movements. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,86, 314–329.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  59. O’Grady, W., Schafer, A. J., Perla, J., Lee, O., & Wieting, J. (2009). A psycholinguistic tool for the assessment of language loss: The HALA project. Language Documentation and Conservation,3(1), 100–112.Google Scholar
  60. Ospina, R., & Ferrari, S. L. P. (2012). A general class of zero-or-one inflated beta regression models. Computational Statistics and Data Analysis,56(6), 1609–1623.Google Scholar
  61. Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2007). Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension? Trends in Cognitive Sciences,11, 105–110.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  62. Piquado, T., Isaacowitz, D., & Wingfield, A. (2010). Pupillometry as a measure of cognitive effort in younger and older adults. Psychophysiology,47(3), 560–569.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  63. Polinsky, M. (2011). Reanalysis in adult heritage language: New evidence in support of attrition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,33, 305–328.Google Scholar
  64. Puig-Mayenco, E., Cunnings, I., Bayram, F., Miller, D., Tubau, S., & Rothman, J. (2018). Language dominance affects bilingual performance and processing outcomes in adulthood. Frontiers in Psychology,2019, 9.Google Scholar
  65. R Core Team (2017). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.R-project.org/.
  66. Rigby, R. A., & Stasinopoulos, D. M. (2005). Generalized additive models for location, scale and shape, (with discussion). Appl. Statist.,54(3), 507–554.Google Scholar
  67. Scherag, A., Demuth, L., Rösler, F., Neville, H. J., & Röder, B. (2004). The effects of late acquisition of L2 and the consequences of immigration on L1 for semantic and morpho-syntactic language aspects. Cognition,93(3), B97–B108.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  68. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime (Version 2.0). [Computer software and manual]. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools Inc.Google Scholar
  69. Scontras, G., Fuchs, Z., & Polinsky, M. (2015). Heritage language and linguistic theory. Frontiers in Psychology,6, 1545.PubMedPubMedCentralGoogle Scholar
  70. Slobin, D. (1985). The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, Vol. I and II. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  71. Stover, L. M., Stern, M. C., Lowry, C., Martohardjono, G., & Madsen II, C. N. Effects of language dominance on L1 relative clause processing(Manuscript in preparation).Google Scholar
  72. Stowe, L. A. (1986). Parsing WH-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes,1, 227–245.Google Scholar
  73. Stromswold, K., Caplan, D., Alpert, N., & Rauch, S. (1996). Localization of syntactic comprehension by positron emission tomography. Brain and Language,52, 452–473.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science,268(5217), 1632–1634.Google Scholar
  75. Tolentino, L. C., & Tokowicz, C. (2011). Across language, space, and time: A review of the role of cross-language similarity in L2 (morpho)syntactic processing as revealed by fMRI and ERP methods. Studies in Second Language Acquisition,33(1), 91–125.Google Scholar
  76. Traxler, M. J., Morris, R. K., & Seely, R. E. (2002). Processing subject and object relative clauses: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language,47, 69–90.Google Scholar
  77. White, L., Goad, G., Goodhue, D., Hwang, G., & Lieberman, M. (2013). Syntactic ambiguity resolution in L2 parsing: Effects of prosodic boundaries and constituent length. In Proceedings of the annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Google Scholar
  78. Wu, F., Luo, Y., & Zhou, X. (2014). Building Chinese relative clause structures with lexical and syntactic cues: Evidence from visual world eye-tracking and reading times. Language Cognition and Neuroscience,29(10), 1205–1226.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2019

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Linguistics ProgramThe Graduate Center, City University of New YorkNew YorkUSA

Personalised recommendations