Skip to main content
Log in

“It Would Never Happen in My Country I Must Say”: A Corpus-Pragmatic Study on Asian English Learners’ Preferred Uses of Must and Should

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Corpus Pragmatics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This paper aims to investigate Asian English learners’ preferred uses of the modal auxiliaries must and should by using both syntactic, topical and semantic annotation as well as various corpus linguistic tools. Our primary goal is to identify culture-specific motivations in the use of the two modal verbs and to demonstrate how those relate to the use of the two items in language produced by L1 speakers of English. We adhere to Hinkel’s study (TESOL Q 29(2):325–343. 1995, doi: 10.2307/3587627) and her findings as a springboard into our analysis. She suggested that differences in the use of modals between Asian English learners (AELs) and English L1 speakers may be culturally founded. We see the essential need to question and deepen the understanding of such implications, arguing that a count of tokens and their correlation with essay topics alone cannot be sufficiently informative. This is why we pursue a more in-depth approach which combines both corpus and pragmatic tools. First of all, we will conduct a contrastive analysis of quantitative data from two different learners’ corpora on the one hand, and three available corpora of contemporary English on the other hand. The goal of this analysis is to carve out certain AEL and/or English native speakers’ patterns in using must and should in their deontic and epistemic senses. Secondly, we will evaluate the AEL data in regard to whether and how they reflect cultural values different from the ones expressed in native English speech. Our findings show that, whereas ENSs and AELs alike do demonstrate a preference in the less face-threatening modal should over the more direct must in deontic uses, both must and should tend to be used more deliberately and more purposefully by AELs than by ENSs, who seem to apply them much more vaguely and polysemously. With regard to Hinkel’s major claim that AELs’ use of must and should reflects cultural values different from the ones expressed in native English speech, we were able to affirm this hypothesis. AELs in our datasets use must and should significantly differently than ENSs due to their alleged culturally intrinsic sense of togetherness and joint responsibility for their society.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In this study we exclude the negative forms mustn’t, shouldn’t.

  2. For details on their morphosyntactic characteristics and behavior as well as their etymological evolvement, cf. e.g. Palmer (1979); Krug (2000).

  3. Dutra adds ‘advisability’ to the dynamic framework of modality, which “is another root meaning not as close to obligation and necessity as [necessity and obligation] may be to each other” (1998: 22).

  4. Palmer (1990) and Papafragou (1998) add dynamic modality as a third category, which we shall refrain from in this paper.

  5. Note that we only consider relevant what the OED refers to as the lexeme must, v.1 and should only in its entries below shall, v., senses 18., 19. and 21. We excluded all OED entries that were either marked <Obs.> or <†>, i.e. as obsolete or for which the OED did not provide any quotations dating after 1990.

  6. Also cf. Viana (2006) on the same observation for Brazilian students.

  7. The sentences were obtained from the Cambridge Learner Corpus First Certificate in English (FCE) dataset, which contains 1244 exam scripts written by candidates who sat the Cambridge ESOL First Certificate in English examination in either 2000 or 2001 (cf. Berzak 2016: 738; Yannakoudakis et al. 2011).

  8. All in all, ICNALE contains 1.8-million-words of controlled L2 English speeches and essays by over 3500 college undergraduate and graduate students in ten countries and areas in Asia as well as L1 productions by 350 English native speakers.

  9. Brown was compiled by W. Nelson Francis and Henry Kučera at Brown University and contains more than 1 million words of running text of edited English prose printed in the United States during the calendar year 1961.

  10. A crosscheck with ICNALE via #LancsBox has revealed that out of 237 <should have> patterns in the AEL ICNALE dataset only 6 instances are <should + have + past participle> constructions.

  11. Note that the AntConc token counts also include those tokens that we manually excluded during the syntactic and semantic annotation. Since the discrepancies are minor and also since we are going to discuss random samples individually, we do not expect them to affect the integrity of our argumentation.

  12. Log likelihood considers the number of words in the focus corpus in relation to a reference corpus (here: Brown), and uses the following formula: G2[LL] = 2*((a*ln (a/E1)) + (b*ln (b/E2))), with the variables a and b representing the number of tokens in either corpus.

  13. COCA provides a list of the 5000 most frequent words in the corpus on: http://www.wordfrequency.info/free.asp?s=y. Accessed 20 Nov 2016.

  14. 45,558 hits for have to + 21,752 hits for had to + another 8224 hits for has to.

  15. I.e. <modal auxiliary + I/we + lexical verb…?> and <how/whether/what/which etc. I/we + modal auxiliary + lexical verb>.

  16. Random checks revealed that, if we had considered the collocates of e.g. a span of 5 words left and 5 words right (e.g. we—should with a span of 5< >5), we would have seen numbers about a third higher than the ones we received from the immediate collocates (e.g. 6530 collocates of we—should with a span of 5< >5).

References

  • Aijmer, K. (2002). Modality in advanced Swedish learners’ written interlanguage. In S. Granger, J. Hung, & S. Petch-Tyson (Eds.), Computer learner corpora, second language acquisition and foreign language teaching (pp. 55–76). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Bartley, L., & Hidalgo-Tenorio, E. (2016). “Well, I think that my argument is...,” or modality in a learner corpus of English. Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada, 29(1), 1–29.

  • Bensaid, M. (2015). Arab ESL learners and modals. Arab World English Journal (AWEJ), 6(4), 90–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berber-Sardinha, T. (2000). Comparing corpora with WordSmith Tools: How large must the reference corpus be? In Proceedings of the workshop on comparing corpora (Vol. 9, pp. 7–13). Association for Computational Linguistics.

  • Berzak, Y., Kenney, J., Spadine, C., Wang, J. X., Lam, L., Mori, K. S., Garza, S., & Katz, B. (2016). Universal dependencies for learner English. In Proceedings of the 54th annual meeting of the association for computational linguistics, Berlin, Germany, August 712, 2016, (pp. 737–746). Association for Computational Linguistics.

  • Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., & Finegan, E. (1999). Longman grammar of spoken and written English. Harlow: Pearson Education Limited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bies, A., Mott, J., Warner, C. & Kulick S. (2012). English Web Treebank corpus (EWTC). Accessed via: https://catalog.ldc.upenn.edu/ldc2012t13

  • Bose, A. C. (2005). The problems in learning modal auxiliary verbs in English at high school level. Language in India. Accessed 10 March 2017. http://www.languageinindia.com/nov2005/chandrabose1.html

  • Brezina, V. (2014). Statistics in Corpus Linguistics: Web resource. Available from http://corpora.lancs.ac.uk/stats

  • Brown University Standard Corpus of Present-Day American English (Brown). Accessed via: http://www.essex.ac.uk/linguistics/external/clmt/w3c/corpus_ling/content/corpora/list/private/brown/brown.html

  • Cambridge Learner Corpus First Certificate in English (FCE). Accessed via: http://ilexir.co.uk/applications/clc-fce-dataset/

  • Celce-Murcia, M., & Larsen-Freeman, D. (1999). The grammar book: An ESL/EFL teacher’s course. Boston, MA: Heinle & Heinle.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choi, S. (1995). The development of epistemic sentence-ending modal forms and functions in Korean children. In J. Bybee & S. Fleischman (Eds.), Modality in grammar and discourse (pp. 165–204). Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Coates, J. (1983). The semantics of the modal auxiliaries. London: Croom Helm.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collins, C. (1991). The modals of obligation and necessity in Australian English. In K. Aijmer & B. Alternberg (Eds.), English corpus linguistics: Studies in honour of Jan Svartvik (pp. 145–165). London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corpus of Contemporary American English (henceforth COCA, UR. http://corpus.byu.edu/coca/ ).

  • Debbie, G. E. (2009). Exponents of politeness in Brunei English. World Englishes, 28(1), 35–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Desai, J. (2007). Intergenerational conflict within Asian American families: The role of acculturation, ethnic identity, individualism, and collectivism. Dissertation Abstracts International, 67, 7369.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dutra, D. P. (1998). The Acquisition of English root modality by non-native speakers. [unpublished doctoral dissertation presented to the graduate school of The University of Florida]. Retrieved from http://www.leffa.pro.br/tela4/Textos/Textos/Teses/Deise_Prina.pdf

  • Elenizi, S. D. (2004). Modality and the conditionals in English and French: A national comparative study. Journal of King Saud University, 17, 19–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fu, R. (2016). Comparing modal patterns in Chinese–English interpreted and translated discourses in diplomatic setting. A systemic functional approach. Babel, 62(1), 104–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gablasova, D. V., Brezina, T., McEnery, T., & Boyd, E. (2015). Epistemic stance in spoken L2 english: The effect of task and speaker style. Applied Linguistics Advance Access. doi:10.1093/applin/amv055.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granger, S. (2002). A Bird’s-eye view of computer learner corpus research. In S. Granger, J. Hung & S. Petch-Tyson (Eds), Computer learner corpora, second language acquisition and foreign language teaching. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (2004). An introduction to functional grammar (3rd ed.). London: Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hinkel, E. (1995). The use of modal verbs as a reflection of cultural values. TESOL Quarterly, 29(2), 325–343. doi:10.2307/3587627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hopper, P. J., & Traugott, E. C. (2003). Grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge textbooks in linguistics. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Horty, J. (2014). Deontic modals: Why abandon the classical Semantics? Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 95, 424–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • House, J. (2002). Developing pragmatic competence in English as a lingua franca. In K. Knapp & C. Meierkord (Eds.), Lingua Franca Communication (pp. 245–267). Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoye, L. (1997). Adverbs and modality in English. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kecskes, I. (2007). Formulaic language in English lingua franca. In I. Kecskes & L. R. Horn (Eds.), Explorations in pragmatics: Linguistic, cognitive and intercultural aspects (pp. 191–219). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kecskes, I. (2015). Is the Idiom Principle blocked in bilingual L2 production? In R. Heredia & A. Cieslicka (Eds.), Bilingual Figurative Language Processing (pp. 28–53). Cambridge: CUP.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Khojasteh, L., & Reinders, H. (2013). How textbooks (and learners) get it wrong: A corpus study of modal auxiliary verbs. Applied Research on English Language, 2(1), 33–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krug, M. G. (2000). Emerging English modals: A corpus-based study of grammaticalization. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Leech, G., & Coates, J. (1980). Semantic indeterminacy and the modals. In S. Greenbaum, G. Leech, & J. Svartvik (Eds.), Studies in English linguistics (pp. 79–90). London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, J. (1977). Semantics (Vol. 2). Cambridge: CUP.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, M. R. (2007). The development of narrative skills and the evaluative use of modal verbs in the narratives of young non-native speakers of English. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, A(1), 79–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDouall, A. (2012). A corpus based investigation into the use of English modal auxiliaries by adult korean L2-learners. KLING, 6, 33–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • McEnery, T., Xiao, R., & Tono, Y. (2006). Corpus-based language studies: An advanced resource book. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montero, B., Watts, F., & Garcia-Carbonell, A. (2007). Discussion forum interactions: Text and context. System, 35(4), 566–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Myhill, J. (1995). Change and continuity in the functions of the American English modals. Linguistics, 33, 157–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nakayasu, M. (2013). Modals, speech acts and (im)politeness: interactions in Shakespeare’s plays. Studia Anglica Posnaniensia: International Review of English Studies, 48(4), 5–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, K., & Fivush, R. (2004). The emergence of autobiographical memory: A social cultural developmental theory. Psychological Review, 111, 486–511.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obara, H. (2013). The modals as pragmatic softening devices: Evidence from late Modern English Grammar and manner books. Journal of Literature and Art Studies, 3(8), 518–524.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, F. R. (1979). Modality and the English Modals. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, F. R. (1986). Mood and Modality. Cambridge: CUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, F. R. (1990). Modality and the English modals (2nd ed.). London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papafragou, A. (1998). Inference and word meaning: The case of modal auxiliaries. Lingua, 105, 1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, M. R. (1983). Modal expressions in English. London: Frances Pinter Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quirk, R., Greenbaum, S., Leech, G., & Svartvik, J. (1985). A comprehensive grammar of the English language. Essex: Pearson Education Limited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romero-Trillo, J. (Ed.). (2008). Pragmatics and corpus linguistics: a mutualistic entente. Berlin/New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rühlemann, C., & Aijmer, K. (Eds.). (2015). Corpus Pragmatics: A Handbook. Cambridge: CUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Salazar, D., & Verdaguer, I. (2009). Polysemous verbs and modality in native and non-native argumentative writing: A corpus-based study. International Journal of English Studies., 9(3), 209–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanada, K. (2013). A closer look at subjectification in the grammaticalization of English modals. From the main verb mo(o)t to the root modal must. In R. Kikusawa & L. A. Reid. Historical Linguistics 2011: Selected papers from the 20th international conference on historical linguistics, Osaka, 2530 July 2011. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

  • Swan, M. (1998). Practical English usage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweetser, E. E. (1988). Grammaticalization and semantic bleaching. In S. Axmaker, A. Jaisser & H. Singmaster (Eds.), Berkeley linguistics society: general session and parasession on grammaticalization. Berkeley: Berkeley Linguistics Society.

  • Sweetser, E. E. (1990). From etymology to pragmatics: metaphorical and cultural aspects of semantic structure. Cambridge: CUP.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Taavitsainen, I., Jucker, A. H., & Tuominen, J. (Eds.). (2014). Diachronic corpus pragmatics. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thornbury, S. (1999). How to teach grammar. Harlow: Pearson Education Limited.

    Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. (1993). Collectivism and individualism as cultural syndromes. Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Comparative Social Science, 27, 155–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • The International Corpus Network of Asian Learners of English (ICNALE online). http://language.sakura.ne.jp/icnale/icnale_online.html

  • Treebank of Learner English (TLE). Accessed via: http://esltreebank.org/

  • Verhulst, A., Depraetere, I., & Heyvaert, L. (2013). Source and strength of modality: An empirical study of root should, ought to and be supposed to in Present-Day English. Journal of Pragmatics, 55, 210–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Viana, V. (2006). Modals in Brazilian advanced EFL learners’ compositions: A corpus-based investigation. Profile Issues in Teachers’ Professional Development, 7, 77–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, Z. (2004). Power in cross-examination: A modality analysis of a segment of courtroom discourse of O. J. Simpson case. Foreign Language Research, 3, 51–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, H., & Chen, C. (2007). A contrastive study of modality in english political and academic speeches. Foreign Languages and Their Teaching, 5, 21–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yannakoudakis, H., Briscoe, T., & Medlock, B. (2011). A new dataset and method for automatically grading ESOL texts. In ACL, (pp. 180–189).

  • Zalaltdinova, L. (forthcoming). Modality as a reflection of pragmatic competence.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Istvan Kecskes.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kecskes, I., Kirner-Ludwig, M. “It Would Never Happen in My Country I Must Say”: A Corpus-Pragmatic Study on Asian English Learners’ Preferred Uses of Must and Should . Corpus Pragmatics 1, 91–134 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s41701-017-0007-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s41701-017-0007-x

Keywords

Navigation