1 Introduction

Just recently, 181 members of the Business Roundtable, a nonprofit association whose members are chief executive officers of major U.S. companies, signed a “Statement on the Purpose of a Corporation”, which represents a shift from a strong shareholder orientation to a more modern stakeholder capitalism (Business Roundtable 2019; Gartenberg and Serafeim 2019). Two of the five pillars described in the statement are a commitment to socially oriented and sustainable business practices. The statement follows on peaked public concern about climate change and its effects on society and increased pressure of shareholders and governments demanding firms to be socially responsible and to lower their ecological footprint (Basu and Palazzo 2008; Lin-Hi and Müller 2013; Luo et al. 2017; Henderson 2018).

While management is generally responsible for the implementation of a firm’s CSR activities (Petrenko et al. 2016), the board also has a fundamental role in serving as a link to the external environment by representing a broad range of stakeholders and considering CSR issues in a firm’s strategic agenda (Hillman et al. 2000; Wang and Dewhirst 1992). In this context, a substantial body of archival research suggests that female board members and female managers are more willing to encourage firms’ stakeholder orientation and CSR performance than their male counterparts (Francoeur et al. 2019, 2008; Glass and Cook 2018; Glass et al. 2016; Hafsi and Turgut 2013; Harjoto et al. 2015).Footnote 1 However, CSR research has not yet delivered conclusive evidence for a causal inference between female board and management representation and CSR performance (Smith 2018). Also, there is a lack of insights into which character traits and factors influence executives’ CSR decision-making and why archival research finds differences between female and male executives (Wang et al. 2016). While some studies suggest that executives’ incentive programs and the level of shareholder pressure are relevant determinants of firms’ CSR activities (e.g. Beddewela and Fairbrass 2016; Dam and Scholtens 2013; Flammer et al. 2019), their effect on executives’ individual CSR decision-making has not yet been examined by empirical studies.

Thus, we create an experimental investing scenario in which we observe executives’ CSR decision-making on short-term profit maximization versus improvement of social/ecological standards under a restrained budget. We naturally measure participants’ gender, record participants’ real-world incentive structure, and manipulate shareholder pressure. Additionally, we capture participants’ risk propensity, attitude towards sustainability, and empathy as these character traits are used as an explanation in many archival studies that find positive effects of female executives’ on CSR performance. Our resulting experimental design enables us to examine gender differences in CSR decision-making while observing the role of incentives, shareholder pressure, and relevant character traits.

We draw upon psychological concepts and, in line with the large body of archival research, predict that female executives are generally more willing to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards when making investment decisions than male executives (Hypothesis 1a). We find weak statistical evidence that this relationship holds. We complement our analyses with a structural equation model (SEM) explicitly considering individual character traits (risk propensity, attitude towards sustainability, and empathy) as potential mediators, which allows a more nuanced analysis (Hypotheses 1b1–1b3). We find a strong effect of gender on the considered character traits, but no statistically significant effect of these character traits on CSR decision-making. Furthermore, as we capture participants’ real-world incentive programs, we are able to analyze how this affects their CSR decision-making in our experiment. We find strong statistical evidence that, as the relevance of achieving financial goals in participants’ real-world incentive program increases, participants focus more on short-term profit maximization in our experimental setting (Hypothesis 2). Therefore, we conclude that a certain type of behavior, which is promoted through participants’ real-world incentive program, also transfers to the decisions made by participants in our experiment.

Additionally, since there is an ongoing debate in the empirical research about whether a firm’s financial performance determines its CSR efforts (Arora and Dharwadkar 2011; Flammer and Ioannou 2021; Nelling and Webb 2009), we manipulate shareholder pressure (high vs. low) through shareholder satisfaction and the relative financial performance of our hypothetical firm in our experiment and observe if this affects executives’ CSR decision-making (Hypothesis 3). We find that our manipulation does not affect executives’ CSR decision-making. Finally, in an additional exploratory analysis, we find that risk propensity in connection with our shareholder pressure manipulation affects executives’ CSR decision-making (Research Question 1).

Our study makes important contributions to the CSR literature and the literature on executives’ decision-making by advancing our understanding of gender effects in managerial decision-making in the CSR context. With our experimental analysis, we can generally confirm a positive impact of female executives on firms’ CSR performance. Although we find that the character traits, which are used extensively in archival studies to explain positive effects of female executives on firms’ CSR performances, are gender-dependent, we do not find any effects of these character traits on executives’ CSR investment decisions. Our analyses provide further interesting insights that might help to explain inconclusive findings of prior archival studies. As we find that executives’ real-world incentive program plays a key role in explaining executives’ CSR decision-making, we contribute to a growing stream of literature on CSR decision-making (Wang et al. 2016) by outlining that it is essential to control for executives’ incentive programs to derive valid inferences. Additionally, our study is of practical importance. By emphasizing the positive impact of female executives on firms’ CSR performance, our study also offers implications for policymakers and for firms themselves, which will continue to face public and investor pressure to expand their social and ecological activities and, therefore, must adapt their corporate decision-making to address all stakeholder concerns. The results of our study imply that firms that intend to transform their business to adapt to the increased challenges of sustainability and social compliance need to make sure that their incentive programs sufficiently emphasize the achievement of sustainability goals.

Our paper proceeds as follows. In Sect. 2 we discuss related research and develop our hypotheses. Sect. 3 describes our methodology, while Sect. 4 discusses our results. Sect. 5 concludes our paper.

2 Related Research and Hypotheses Development

Over the past two decades, there has been a vivid debate about women’s representation in top executive teams and its effect on a firm’s decision-making (Dezsö and Ross 2012; Terjesen et al. 2009). Research has examined the impact of female executives on corporate decisions, such as financing, investment, reporting, and mergers and acquisitions (e.g., Francis et al. 2015; Huang and Kisgen 2013; Ibrahim et al. 2009; Levi et al. 2008; Mohan and Chen 2004). Evidence shows that decisions made by female executives can differ from those made by male executives.

Due to high levels of investor, public, and media attention, a firm’s decision regarding its involvement in socially and environmentally responsible business practices is currently one of the most critical it must make and introduces new challenges to its leadership (Haney et al. 2020). Studies find that addressing social issues and the improvement of corporate social performance entails positive investor reactions and is perceived as a competitive advantage (Cordeiro and Tewari 2015; Flammer 2018; Kunz 2020; McWilliams et al. 2006). As CSR is therefore a highly relevant topic for almost all firms, research has also examined the role of gender diversity on CSR performance. Archival research linking female board and/or top management representation and CSR performance generally finds a positive association (e.g. Francoeur et al. 2019, 2008; Glass and Cook 2018; Glass et al. 2016; Hafsi and Turgut 2013; Harjoto et al. 2015).

However, archival studies on the relationship between female board and management representation and CSR are subject to some methodological challenges. Kato and Kodama (2018) find a delayed but positive effect of firms’ CSR performance on workplace gender diversity, which indicates a possible reverse causality in the connection between gender diversity and CSR. It seems plausible that firms with a strong CSR performance also focus more on their public reputation and thus foster a higher female representation on management and oversight board (Brieger et al. 2019). Also, both the trend for more women in executive positions and the increased awareness of firms’ social and ecological responsibility arose simultaneously. Therefore, archival studies might also just capture this simultaneous and continuous increase of female executives and CSR awareness. Consequently, archival CSR research has not yet delivered conclusive evidence for a causal inference between female board and management representation and CSR performance (Smith 2018). Above that, research has not yet empirically examined which specific character traits are responsible for the gender differences when it comes to CSR decision-making.

Prior studies show that executives’ personal characteristics and beliefs affect their decision-making, also with respect to CSR activities (e.g. Agle et al. 1999; Chin et al. 2013; Hoffmann and Meusburger 2018; Wally and Baum 1994). Research focusing on gender differences often draws on the work of Eagly (1987) and Eagly and Wood (1991) to explain differences between men and women and their respective roles in a corporate environment. According to their social role theory, women, through their role induced by society, develop more communal qualities (they are generous, social-oriented, concerned with others, etc.). This means that they are more socially skilled, emotionally sensitive, value personal relationships more, and are more oriented to others’ welfare. In contrast, men display more agentic qualities (they are ambitious, self-directed, aimed at personal development, etc.) and focus on their own rewards, as they are assigned to social roles that demand these character traits. Consequently, through this societal selection, women’s and men’s social behaviors differ.

Also, Gilligan (1977, 1982) argues that based on their early childhood experiences, women and men utilize different procedures for making moral decisions and that women have a higher care orientation, which is characterized by a focus on long-term relations and responsiveness to the needs of others. However, empirical evidence of the differences between women and men regarding moral reasoning is ambiguous. Jaffee and Hyde (2000) find only sparse evidence in their meta-analysis that men and women differ in terms of care reasoning or justice reasoning, a finding supported by Forte (2004). In contrast, there is empirical evidence that women have a more long-term orientation (Fetchenhauer and Rohde 2002), which is supported by Embrey and Fox (1997) and Stewart (2018) in an investment context, and a higher social orientation (Croson and Buchan 1999).

Hence, as executives’ personal characteristics and beliefs affect their decision-making, it may not be mere gender affiliation that explains gender differences in CSR decision making, but rather specific character traits. Inter alia, Glass and Cook (2018) use risk aversion to explain gender differences in business practices related to CSR. There is a large body of prior research that finds women to be more risk-averse than men (e.g. Croson and Gneezy 2009; Eckel and Grossman 2002), which also holds for financial decision-making and for C‑level executives (Faccio et al. 2016; Huang and Kisgen 2013; Powell and Ansic 1997). In contrast, Adams and Funk (2012) document that female directors are more risk-loving than their male counterparts and highlight that this finding differs from studies of the general population. We analyze executives’ decision-making in an investment context. Therefore, we identify risk propensity as one of our gender-dependent character traits possibly affecting executives’ decision-making in terms of CSR. More specifically, related insights from prior research would imply that individuals with lower (higher) risk propensity are more (less) willing to invest in CSR. This is because CSR may function as reputation insurance against management lapses (e.g., Werther and Chandler 2005; Minor and Morgan 2011) and can lead to greater organizational resilience to systematic shocks (e.g., Sajko et al. 2021; Shiu and Yang 2017). However, explicit theory or empirical evidence regarding the association between risk propensity and executives’ decision-making in terms of CSR is largely missing.

Archival research on female board and/or top management representation and CSR performance often draws on the works of Eagly (1987), Eagly and Wood (1991), and Gilligan (1977, 1982) to motivate gender differences in terms of CSR, highlighting that women are more long-term and socially orientated than men (see for example Boulouta 2013; Francis et al. 2015; Francoeur et al. 2019; Glass and Cook 2018). From this line of research, we derive empathy and the attitude towards sustainability as two possible mediators of a gender effect on CSR decision-making. Eagly and Wood (1991) and Gilligan (1977, 1982) describe that, based on their childhood experiences and social roles, women are more concerned with the welfare of others and have a more long-term orientation than men (see also Croson and Gneezy 2009; Fetchenhauer and Rohde 2002). Investments in sustainability are generally considered long-term (Porter and Miles 2013), and the attitude towards sustainability explains executives’ CSR decision-making (Adomako et al. 2021). As long-term orientation seems to be more prominent in women, the attitude towards sustainability might also be gender-dependent. Thus, we examine if the attitude towards sustainability affects executives’ CSR decision-making. Additionally, CSR investments result in welfare for others (Dickson and Chang 2015). As empathy constitutes the concern for others’ welfare and, according to the literature, seems to be gender-dependent, we also capture executives’ empathy and examine the impact of empathy on executives’ CSR decision-making.

Consequently, we expect that the three outlined character traits rather than mere gender affiliation influence executives’ CSR decision-making. However, these character traits might only partially capture the effects of gender affiliation on CSR decision making, as there might be other character traits that are gender-dependent and related to CSR decision making. Hence, to begin with, we formulate the following baseline Hypothesis 1a (H1a), which is derived from the archival evidence of a positive association between gender and CSR performance, and reads as follows:

Hypothesis 1a

Female executives are more willing to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards when making investment decisions than male executives.


Furthermore, as already argued, we expect that risk propensity, attitude towards sustainability, and empathy are gender-dependent and related to CSR decision-making because specific character traits rather than mere gender affiliation should affect executives’ CSR decision-making. More specifically, based on the above-mentioned literature, we predict that female executives are more risk-averse, have a stronger attitude towards sustainability, and are more empathetic compared to male executives. We also expect that more empathy and a higher attitude towards sustainability lead to executives spending more on CSR. Yet, as we are not able to formulate a directed hypothesis for risk propensity based on the literature, we formally state our three Hypotheses 1b1–1b3 (H1b1–H1b3) for risk propensity, attitude towards sustainability, and empathy uniformly undirected, as follows:

Hypothesis 1b1

Risk propensity mediates the effect of gender on the investment decision to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards.

Hypothesis 1b2

Attitude towards sustainability mediates the effect of gender on the investment decision to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards.

Hypothesis 1b3

Empathy mediates the effect of gender on the investment decision to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards.


Incentive programs play a pivotal role in today’s business world (Antón et al. 2020). Bonner et al. (2000) underline the prevalent importance of incentives not only for management accounting in general but also for experimental research. Furthermore, incentives are widely used to direct management’s efforts to comply with stakeholders’ interests and the inclusion of CSR criteria in executives’ compensation schemes has recently become more prevalent (Flammer et al. 2019). Research has found various effects of incentives on executives’ decision-making, for example with regard to innovation, performance, and earnings management (Bergstresser and Philippon 2006; Bonner et al. 2000; Makri et al. 2006). In a CSR context, Fabrizi et al. (2014) find that the split between monetary and non-monetary incentives of CEOs affects a firm’s CSR efforts, while Boone et al. (2020), within their theoretical framework, explain that CEOs social values and the incentivizing context play an important role when explaining CSR decision-making. Hence, incentive programs should have a significant influence on executives’ CSR decision-making. Specifically, we expect that programs with a stronger focus on financial goal achievement lead to executives focusing on short-term financial performance, while programs with a stronger focus on sustainable goal achievement encourage executives’ CSR spending (Flammer et al. 2019). Furthermore, we argue that, due to spillover effects, executives’ real-life incentive program affects their general CSR decision-making, which then also translates to our experiment (Huang and Murad 2020; Pierce et al. 2016). Applied to our experiment, this would mean that greater relevance of financial goals achievement in executives’ incentive program leads executives’ to focus more on short-term financial return when making the experimental CSR investment decisions. Consequently, we formally state the following Hypothesis 2 (H2):

Hypothesis 2

Greater relevance of financial goals achievement in executives’ incentive program reduces executives’ willingness to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards when making investment decisions.


In recent years, public attention to firms’ involvement in socially and environmentally responsible business practices has risen, while investors with a focus on socially and ecologically responsible investments have gained prominence and increased their activism (Boiral et al. 2017; Michelon and Rodrigue 2015). Research shows that external pressure, such as shareholder concentration or regulatory and institutional pressure, can influence a firm’s CSR activities (Beddewela and Fairbrass 2016; Bryant et al. 2020; Dam and Scholtens 2013). More specifically, studies suggest that firms tend to respond to shareholder activism on sustainability issues with an increase in sustainability performance (Grewal et al. 2016; Harvey and Pearson 2018); this association is, however, conditional on the shareholder activists’ status and their reputation to threaten the firm (Perrault and Clark 2015).

In our study, we want to investigate a different type of shareholder pressure where a firm’s response in terms of executive CSR decision-making seems less intuitive: shareholder pressure that originates from a firms’ financial performance. While it may seem reasonable to assume that a firms’ financial performance has an influence on its ability and willingness to engage in (potentially costly) CSR, evidence in this regard is inconclusive. On the one hand side, Arora and Dharwadkar (2011) document that firms that exceed their aspired performance targets show increased CSR efforts. In this regard, Nelling and Webb (2009) find some evidence for a positive association between a firm’s stock returns and specific aspects of CSR performance, such as employee relations. On the other hand side, Flammer and Ioannou (2021) report that firms that were adversely affected by the sharp increase in the cost of credit during the financial crisis maintained their CSR investments. Thus, they do not find an effect of decreased financial performance on CSR efforts.

In order to contribute to the debate about the effect of financial performance on CSR efforts, we manipulate shareholder pressure (high vs. low) through shareholder satisfaction and the relative financial performance of our hypothetical firm (see following sections for details). While we recognize the findings of Flammer and Ioannou (2021), we use the findings of Nelling and Webb (2009) and in particular Arora and Dharwadkar (2011) to posit a positive association between financial performance and CSR performance, leading to the following Hypothesis 3 (H3):

Hypothesis 3

Under high shareholder pressure executives are less willing to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards when making investment decisions.


Fig. 1 illustratively summarizes our hypotheses.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Structure of hypotheses

3 Method

3.1 Participants

We analyze the decision-making of executives in an experimental setting recruiting participants from a private survey company.Footnote 2 Survey companies have been used to recruit high-level business managers in a number of prior studies (e.g. Arnold et al. 2012; Hannah et al. 2014; Johnson et al. 2020a; see also Brandon et al. (2014) for a comparison of participant recruitment services) and Walter et al. (2019) find that results drawn from commercial online panel data are generally suitable for applied psychological research.

We specified to the survey company that we wanted to recruit experienced executives residing and working for companies in the United States (U.S.). To ensure that participants met our criteria, we implemented several screening questions in our survey. We asked participants whether they work for a for-profit organization and only participants that affirm continued with the survey. Also, we asked participants in which industry they work. Participants who selected “Finance (Banks, Insurances, etc.)” automatically exited the survey. We also asked participants in which domain they primarily work and accepted only participants with the following backgrounds: Governance, Strategy, Finance, Accounting, Operations/Production.Footnote 3 We also asked participants if they are a member of the board of directors, executive board, senior management, mid- or lower-level management of their organization. Without further screening, we only accepted participants that belong to either board of directors, executive board, or senior management. Participants that belong to mid- or lower-level management were additionally asked how frequently they are involved in significant investment decisions (e.g. about opening a new factory, production line, office, store or alike) and only passed this screening questions if they answered “sometimes”, “often” or “very often”. Additionally, to ensure that participants who generally met our criteria paid attention to our survey questions, we included one attention check later in the experiment and asked participants to select the response option “strongly disagree” if they pay attention.Footnote 4 Participants who failed to answer the attention check correctly were also screened out, automatically exited the survey, and did not receive compensation. In total, 533 participants entered our survey and 123 passed all our screening questions.Footnote 5 Thus, our final sample consists of responses from 123 participants.Footnote 6 These participants were randomly assigned to one of our experimental conditions.

We paid $ 25 per completed survey to the survey company, of which participants earned a fixed portion based on what they agreed on with the survey company. Based on their decisions during our experiment, participants could earn up to $ 3.00 as markup. Table 1 summarizes relevant statistics about our participants and their backgrounds. While 4.07% of our participants have a doctoral degree and 8.94% have a professional degree, 34.96% have a master’s and 38.21% have a bachelor’s degree. Thus, 13.82% of our participants have less than a bachelor’s degree. Concerning education, there is no statistically significant difference between female and male participants. However, our female participants work less often for listed companies compared to our male participants. Consequently, our male participants work for employers with higher revenues and more employees compared to our female participants (untabulated). In addition, for our female participants, financial targets account for a higher portion of their incentive programs, while sustainability goals account for a smaller portion of their incentive programs compared to our male participants. Also, our female participants have held their current professional roles longer than our male participants. However, there are no statistically significant differences between our low and high shareholder pressure conditions in terms of participants’ gender, education, salary, and incentive program and in terms of employer characteristics such as revenue and number of employees (untabulated).

Table 1 Descriptive statistics for our sample of 123 participants

Table 1 and Fig. 2a–c show relevant participant and firm characteristics. On average, our participants have been with their respective companies for approximately nine years and 76% of our participants earn an annual fixed salary above $ 100,000. Additionally, 71% of our participants’ employers generate more than $ 11 million per year in revenues and have more than 100 employees. Therefore, we are confident that our participants’ decisions can be generalized and represent executive decision-making adequately.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Participant statistics. Panel a displays the annual revenues of participants’ employers. Panel b shows the number of employees for participants’ employers and Panel c presents participants’ annual fixed salary

3.2 Design and Procedure

Participants assume the role of an executive belonging to the management board of fictitious Firm Y. Participants view the background information and financial information of Firm Y, which is a hypothetical global manufacturer based in the United States. The financial information is adapted from a real-world example and comprises income statements for the years 2018 and 2019. Our experiment observes participants’ allocation of money to three attributes of an investment—markup in short-term-profit/upcoming year dividend per share, improvement in ecological standards and/or improvement in social standards. We test our hypotheses using an experimental design. We manipulate shareholder pressure by varying Firm Y’s relative financial performance to its peers as well as resulting shareholder satisfaction with Firm Y’s performance (high vs. low shareholder pressure; similar to Espahbodi et al. 2019). In the low shareholder pressure condition, Firm Y’s sales and earnings increase by 10% from 2018 to 2019, while those of industry peers only grow by 2% on average, and shareholders are satisfied with Firm Y’s performance. In the high shareholder pressure condition, Firm Y’s sales and earnings decrease by 10% from 2018 to 2019, while those of industry peers only experience an average decrease of 2%, and Firm Y’s shareholders are unsatisfied with Firm Y’s performance. Nevertheless, in both conditions, Firm Y is profitable and generates over $ 4 billion in revenues, with a return on sales of approximately 4.7%.Footnote 7

Our study begins by asking participants to assume the role of an executive on Firm Y’s management board. Next, participants view background information on Firm Y and either the low or high shareholder pressure income statement. Information is adapted from a real manufacturing firm and comprises a comparative income statement for the years 2018 and 2019. Additionally, participants learn that they, in their role as executives, are personally invested in Firm Y and participate in its dividend payout. We then explain that the variable compensation of participants depends on the upcoming dividend per share of Firm Y. Subsequently, participants learn that Firm Y plans to invest $ 100 million in a new production facility to increase growth and profitability. We also explain that if Firm Y chooses to maximize profits and the upcoming dividend per share, participants will earn $ 3 as variable pay (10 × their allocation to dividend per share), but social and ecological standards at the new production facility will then not exceed minimum legal requirements. However, Firm Y can also improve social (e.g., working conditions and the benefit to the local community) and/or ecological standards (e.g., greenhouse gas emissions and waste reduction), which will reduce the markup in the upcoming dividend per share. Next, we explain that the management board of Firm Y leaves it up to the participant acting as a member of management to decide if Firm Y focuses on maximizing the upcoming dividend per share or improves social and/or ecological standards at the new production facility. Using sliders, participants allocate 30 cents of the earnings per share to the three attributes of the project. To not only incentivize participants with their personal compensation, which is linked to the upcoming dividend per share of Firm Y, we also explain to them that we donate money allocated to improving ecological and social standards to respective U.S. charities after our experiment is finished.Footnote 8 After deciding on the allocation of money to the specific features of the project, participants answer post-experimental questions. To measure participants’ risk propensity, we employ the 8‑item gambling and investing risk-taking propensity subscale of the domain-specific risk-taking (DOSPERT) scale (Markiewicz and Weber 2013; Weber et al. 2002), which captures the specific risk propensity of individuals in the investment decision-making domain. The DOSPERT scale is a proven and reliable scale used for measuring risk propensity in specific domains (Breuer et al. 2016; Wu and Cheung 2014) and Highhouse et al. (2017) highlight the scale’s usefulness for predicting real-world outcomes. A Cronbach’s alpha of 0.643 indicates acceptable internal validity. We use the 17-item Environmental Attitudes Scale (EAS) (Ebenbach et al. 1998; Kortenkamp and Moore 2001) to measure participants’ attitude towards sustainability. A Cronbach’s alpha of 0.873 indicates strong internal validity. In addition, the EAS has a high correlation with other environmental attitude scales (e.g. Dunlap and Van Liere 1978). Therefore, we are convinced that we adequately capture participants’ attitude towards sustainability. To measure participants’ empathy, we use two 7‑item empathy subscales developed by Davis (1980), the perspective-taking scale and the empathic-concern scale, which assess participants’ ability to “adopt the perspective […] of other people” and the “ability to experience feelings of […] compassion and concern for others undergoing negative experiences” (Dietz and Kleinlogel 2014). A Cronbach’s alpha of 0.643 indicates acceptable internal validity. We also include further post-experimental questions capturing participants’ real-life salary structure. Specifically, we ask participants what proportion of their salary is variable and what percentage of their total compensation is attributable to the achievement of financial goals, sustainability goals, personal development goals, or other goals. We capture participants’ real-life incentive programs because we expect that participants’ decisions in our experiment are influenced by how participants are incentivized in their everyday business environment. A more detailed description of our experimental material can be found in the Appendix.

3.3 Dependent and Explanatory Variables

We ask participants to allocate 30 cents to either the markup in short-term profits, i.e., the markup in the upcoming year dividend per share, the improvement in social standards and/or the improvement in ecological standards. The distribution must add up to 30 cents, but any allocation to the three attributes is possible. Our main dependent variable, hereafter labeled CSR investment, is the sum of participants’ allocation of money to improve social standards and ecological standards, as both attributes are considered beneficial for society, are subsumed under the term CSR and represent a voluntary investment of Firm Y (Carroll 1999). As shown in Fig. 3, participants’ allocation to the improvement of social and ecological standards is rather similar. Additionally, we focus on executives’ general investment behavior regarding investments in CSR and do not differentiate between the ecological and social dimensions when deriving our hypotheses. Therefore, from a theoretical standpoint, it is also consistent to sum up participants’ investment in improving ecological and social standards.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Distribution of participants’ allocation to dividend (short-term profit), social standards, and ecological standards in our experiment

In our experiment, a participant’s decision only affects dividends per share for the upcoming year, as we do not address years beyond that point. However, we explain to participants that improving social and ecological standards is generally beneficial for workers of Firm Y and the local community and helps in protecting the environment. Definitions of CSR usually emphasize firms’ contributions to society and a cleaner environment and underline the inherent long-term relationship between business and society (Ackerman 1975; Bowen 2013). Generally, CSR activities focus on long-term success rather than short-term profit (Hang et al. 2019; Porter and Miles 2013). Both the improvement in working conditions and the protection of the environment are sustainable actions that can be seen as prudent and responsible management (Carroll 1999; Dickson and Chang 2015). Therefore, with our CSR investment variable, we capture participants’ long-term orientation and their focus on the improvement of sustainable business practices and future earnings, while the counterpart, participants’ investment in the profitability and dividend per share of Firm Y in the upcoming year, displays their short-term profit orientation.

We capture our first explanatory variable “gender” by asking participants to indicate their gender during our post-experimental questions. We also measure participants’ incentive programs and capture what percentage of participants’ salary is attributable to financial goals.Footnote 9 Thus, our second explanatory variable “financial goals” can range from 0 to 100. The third explanatory variable “shareholder pressure” captures participants’ random distribution to the low or high shareholder pressure condition.

4 Results

4.1 Manipulation Checks

To assess the effectiveness of our shareholder pressure manipulation, we ask participants to agree (disagree) with the following statement: “The case description said that Firm Y’s shareholders were very satisfied with Firm Y’s performance”. Participants answered on a five-point scale ranging from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 5 = “strongly agree”. Participants in the low shareholder pressure condition perceived a significantly lower shareholder pressure than participants in the high shareholder pressure condition (means 4.178 and 3.164, respectively; t123 = 4.50, p < 0.001, two-tailed).Footnote 10 Thus, we conclude that our manipulation works.

4.2 Analysis and Test of Hypotheses

4.2.1 Regression Results

To test our hypotheses, we use a regression model with shareholder pressure as our manipulated variable, participants’ gender, financial goals, and the interaction term of gender and financial goals. We include the interaction term of gender and financial goals, as we have an uneven distribution of financial goals over shareholder pressure and gender (see Table 2) and the effect of financial goals on CSR investment is stronger for female executives (see Fig. 4).

Table 2 Mean distribution of financial goals (% of salary attributable to achieving financial goals) over gender and shareholder pressure conditions
Fig. 4
figure 4

Linear prediction of CSR investment and the portion of financial goals in participants’ incentive program for female and male executives

In Hypothesis 1a, we predict that female executives are more willing to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards when making investment decisions than male executives. As indicated in Table 3, we find a significant and positive association between gender and CSR investment (3.394; p = 0.047, one-tailed).Footnote 11 Thus, we can provide some evidence that female executives are more oriented towards social and sustainable practices per se. This finding is in line with many archival studies that find a positive association between board gender diversity and CSR performance. However, the structural equation model described later on will add nuances to this conclusion with interesting further implications.

Table 3 Regression-Model of CSR Investment

In Hypothesis 2, we predict that the orientation towards financial goals in executives’ incentive programs negatively affects their CSR investment. Table 3 shows our financial goals variable, which indicates what percentage of participants’ salary is attributable to financial goals, is statistically significant and negatively associated with CSR investment (−0.060; p = 0.032, one-tailed). Also, Fig. 4 emphasizes that, for both genders, executives’ CSR investment decreases with increasing relevance of financial goals in executives’ incentive programs. Therefore, we find evidence that greater relevance of financial goals achievement in executives’ incentive programs reduces their willingness to invest in CSR. Consequently, we find support for Hypothesis 2.Footnote 12

In Hypothesis 3, we predict that shareholder pressure affects executives’ willingness to sacrifice financial return to improve social/ecological standards when making investment decisions. Table 3 shows that our binary shareholder pressure variable, which indicates whether participants were assigned to the low (value of variable = 0) or high (value of variable = 1) shareholder pressure condition, is not statistically significant (0.236; p = 0.401, one-tailed).

Thus, we do not find support for Hypothesis 3. Therefore, we show that, in our case, a firm’s relative performance to the industry average and resulting shareholder (dis)satisfaction do not affect executives’ CSR decision-making. This finding is consistent with Nelling and Webb (2009) who do not find a direct causal link between a firm’s performance and aspects of CSR related to the community or environment. Our results continue to hold when we control for specific participant- or employer characteristics that significantly differ between our male and female participants, such as tenure in their current corporate role, revenue or number of employees of the employer or the variable that indicates if participants’ employer is a listed firm.

4.2.2 Structural Equation Model

Our main analysis provides some evidence for a positive association between participants’ gender and CSR investment. However, prior studies show that executives’ personal characteristics and beliefs affect their investments in CSR activities (Agle et al. 1999; Chin et al. 2013; Hafenbrädl and Waeger 2017). Gender as a variable captures different character traits, which in turn might influence CSR investment. Based on the research focusing on gender differences, including archival research analyzing the effect of gender diversity in governance and management boards on CSR performance, we identify three possible character traits that might drive CSR investment: risk propensity, attitude towards sustainability, and empathy.

Fig. 5 shows the design and corresponding results of our maximum likelihood structural equation model. The traditional Chi-square test indicates a good fit of our model (χ2 = 4.491; p = 0.611), which is supported by the Comparative Fit Index (1.000; above the generally accepted minimum of 0.95; Byrne 2013). Also, the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (0.000; below the 0.05 rule of thumb) indicates a good fit (MacCallum et al. 1996).Footnote 13

Fig. 5
figure 5

Structural equation model (total effects) of gender specific character traits, external factors and CSR investment. P-values based on one-tailed tests

In Hypothesis 1a, we predict that female executives are more willing to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards when making investment decisions than male executives. We find only minimal support for Hypothesis 1a in our regression. Our SEM, as displayed in Fig. 5, shows that gender is significantly associated with the character traits risk propensity, attitude towards sustainability, and empathy. According to our SEM, female participants are risk-averse (−0.924; p < 0.001), have a stronger attitude towards sustainability (0.311; p < 0.001) and are more empathetic (0.187; p = 0.009) compared to male participants. However, we do not find that these character traits have a statistically significant association with CSR investment (−0.506; p = 0.188, 1.414; p = 0.126 and −0.339; p = 0.375 respectively), and hence do not find evidence for a mediating effect as proposed in Hypotheses 1b1–1b3. This is surprising as studies on gender differences often use these character traits as an explanation for differences between females and males (e.g. Cabeza-García et al. 2018; Francis et al. 2015; Francoeur et al. 2019; Glass and Cook 2018). In our context, however, it seems that participants’ incentive program overrides the relation between their character traits and CSR investment, as the ratio of financial goals within participants’ incentive programs is strongly associated with participants’ CSR investment (−0.099; p < 0.001), which further supports Hypothesis 2.Footnote 14 This finding is highly interesting as it adds further insights to findings of prior studies investigating the association between board gender diversity and CSR performance. While our results confirm gender differences regarding character traits which are seen as important drivers of CSR investments, our findings suggest that it is essential to control for executives’ incentive program to arrive at valid conclusions.Footnote 15 In line with the results from our regression, we do not find a significant effect of our shareholder pressure variable on executives’ CSR investment (0.280; p = 0.759) in the SEM.

In our SEM, we also capture a direct effect of gender on CSR investment, which corresponds to our Hypothesis 1a. As displayed in Fig. 5, this effect is not significant (0.61; p = 0.536), as some part of a potential direct effect of gender on CSR investment is likely captured by the three distinguished character traits we included in our SEM. Thus, it is not surprising that the statistically weak significant effect of gender on CSR investment vanishes in our SEM, as other gender-dependent character traits are included and may mediate a direct effect to some degree.

4.2.3 Additional Exploratory Analysis

Bryant et al. (2020) show that external pressure (in form of direct regulatory and stakeholder pressure on firms that primarily contribute to greenhouse gas emissions) influences how firms engage in environmental actions to reduce climate change. However, they also find that this relationship is positively moderated by a firm’s attitude towards risk. We use these insights as the departure point for additionally investigating the following Research Question 1 (RQ1):

Research Question 1

Does shareholder pressure moderate the effect of risk propensity on the investment decision to sacrifice short-term financial return to improve social/ecological standards?

For this rather exploratory analysis, we expand our SEM by also including the interaction between risk propensity and shareholder pressure.Footnote 16 We find that our risk propensity variable then has a negative and marginally significant association with CSR investment (−1.41; p = 0.056, two tailed, untabulated), just as our shareholder pressure variable (−5.34; p = 0.093, two tailed, untabulated). In contrast, the interaction term of risk propensity and shareholder pressure is positively associated with CSR investment and marginally significant (1.75; p = 0.065, two tailed, untabulated). Fig. 6 displays the linear predictions of participants’ spending for our low and high shareholder pressure conditions and the risk propensity scale. In our high shareholder pressure condition, in which Firm Y’s performance is below the industry average and shareholders express that they are unsatisfied with the performance, participants with a higher risk propensity invest more in CSR.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Linear prediction of CSR investment and risk propensity for high and low shareholder pressure

While the specific type of pressure analyzed in our experiment differs from Bryant et al. (2020), our findings are still in line with that study. Bryant et al. (2020) find that firms with a high risk propensity are more likely to engage in beyond-compliance environmental actions under high external pressure. They conclude that those firms are more willing to hedge risks arising from climate change, as they view climate change risk differently than financial risk. In our high shareholder pressure condition, shareholders are unsatisfied as Firm Y’s financial performance is below the industry average. Consequently, risk-averse participants invest more into short-term profitability. In contrast, participants with a high risk propensity invest more into CSR in our experiment, so that the social and ecological standards exceed minimum legal requirements.

In our low shareholder pressure condition, in which Firm Y performs above the industry average and shareholders are satisfied, the response pattern is opposite. Participants with a high risk-propensity invest less in CSR (more in short term-profit) and increase their own profit. As indicated by the slope of the two functions in Fig. 6, the difference in CSR investment between high and low risk propensity participants is higher for the low shareholder pressure condition. In this condition, Firm Y performs above the industry average and shareholders are satisfied and thus, managers should have greater discretion in allocating resources than in the high shareholder pressure condition (Arora and Dharwadkar 2011). Some studies find that risk propensity is connected to greed in specific settings (Li et al. 2019), which corroborates our finding that, in our low shareholder pressure condition, participants with a high risk propensity invest more in short term-profit and increase their own profit. This is in line with Sajko et al. (2021) who find that greedy CEOs invest less in CSR. Interestingly, Bryant et al. (2020) also show that firms that are not subject to regulatory pressure are less likely to engage in beyond-compliance environmental actions if they have a high risk propensity. Again, this matches our findings and underlines that executives with a high risk propensity cut CSR investments to increase their own profits when having discretion in allocating resources.

5 Conclusion

The public demand for sustainable corporate behavior is omnipresent and firms intensively invest in CSR activities (Johnson et al. 2020b; Moser and Martin 2012). Archival research linking female board and management representation with CSR performance generally finds positive associations (e.g. Francoeur et al. 2019; Glass and Cook 2018; Hafsi and Turgut 2013; Harjoto et al. 2015) but does not provide conclusive evidence for why gender impacts CSR performance.

We experimentally examine executives’ consideration of sustainable and social attributes in investment decisions. In line with social theory (Eagly 1987; Eagly and Wood 1991) and based on the positive evidence of most archival studies on the association between female board representation and CSR performance, we predict that female executives are more willing to sacrifice financial return to improve social/ecological standards when making investment decisions than male executives (H1a). We find weak statistical support for this prediction.

However, we observe participants’ incentive programs and, consistent with our prediction, find that the relevance of achieving financial goals in participants’ real-world incentive programs significantly affects their CSR investment (H2).

Additionally, we manipulate shareholder pressure and examine how executives’ CSR investments differ between a high and low shareholder pressure condition (H3). We do not find significant evidence for an effect of shareholder pressure on executives’ CSR investment.

We also employ a structural equation model and find that gender is significantly associated with the character traits risk propensity, attitude towards sustainability, and empathy. However, we do not find that these character traits have a statistically significant association with the CSR investment (H1b1–1b3). This is surprising as studies on gender differences often use these character traits as an explanation for differences between females and males (e.g. Cabeza-García et al. 2018; Francis et al. 2015; Francoeur et al. 2019; Glass and Cook 2018). However, in our additional exploratory analysis in relation to RQ1, we find that risk propensity in connection with our shareholder pressure manipulation affects executives’ CSR decision-making.

Our study makes important contributions to the CSR literature and the literature on executives’ decision-making. To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to explicitly, experimentally analyze gender differences in executives’ CSR investment behavior. We expand the literature on the impact of female executives on corporate decision-making (e.g. Francis et al. 2015; Huang and Kisgen 2013; Levi et al. 2008; Mohan and Chen 2004) and on the association between female executives and CSR performance (e.g., Francoeur et al. 2019; Glass and Cook 2018; Harjoto et al. 2015). With our experimental analysis, we can generally confirm a positive impact of female executives on firms’ CSR performance. However, although we do find that the character traits often used in archival are gender-dependent, we do not find evidence that these character traits affect executives’ CSR decision-making. Additionally, in line with Fabrizi et al. (2014) and Flammer et al. (2019), we find that executives’ real-world incentive programs play a key role in explaining executives’ CSR decision-making. Thus, we contribute to a growing stream of literature on CSR decision-making (Wang et al. 2016). By outlining that it is essential to control for executives’ real-world incentive programs to derive valid inferences when examining executives’ decision-making. Finally, our study offers important practical implications. As we find that executives’ incentive program strongly affects their CSR decision-making, we encourage firms that want to increase their CSR activities to consider redesigning their executives’ incentive programs. By emphasizing the positive impact of female executives on firms’ CSR performance, our study also offers implications for policymakers and for firms themselves, which will continue to face public and investor pressure to expand their social and ecological activities and, therefore, must adapt their corporate decision-making to address all stakeholder concerns.

While our study offers important contributions, it also has its limitations and provides avenues for future research. First, we manipulate shareholder pressure with the relative performance of a company to its peer group as well as shareholder satisfaction and do not find an impact on executives’ decision-making. Retrospectively, our manipulation might have been insufficiently strong to trigger different decisions of executives. Second, with sample size requirements being an important question in studies using SEM, we acknowledge that we had to satisfice with a quite small sample of 123 observations, as recruiting executives for experimental research is rather costly. In addition, the small sample size may reduce the generalizability of our results. Third, we are naturally not able to manipulate participants’ real-life incentive programs. Thus, we cannot completely rule out that a portion of our results with respect to participants’ incentive programs can be attributed to more altruistic participants choosing jobs with lower proportions of financial performance targets (Buurman et al. 2012).

Future research should further examine if shareholder pressure, induced by a firm’s financial performance, affects executives’ CSR decision-making. Also, there are other reasons why shareholders might be dissatisfied with a company’s activities or performance, which might affect executives’ behavior differently. Consequently, future research could explore how executives react to different types of shareholder pressure and how this affects their investment decisions. Furthermore, although we do not find significant effects for our three character traits on CSR investment, we encourage future experimental research to further examine specific character traits and their effects on executives’ decision-making, in terms of gender differences, CSR, and in other contexts.