Skip to main content
Log in

Knowing as Remembering: Methodological Experiments in Embodied Experiences of Number

  • Published:
Digital Experiences in Mathematics Education Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A premise of this article is that the current methods used in mathematics education research may be preventing researchers from adequately addressing the body and, in particular, the alignment of acting and knowing. Pursuing a non-dualistic and non-hierarchical approach to learning and knowing, I experiment with new methods that aim to increase situated and embodied validity. I do so through a short video clip of a four-year-old child interacting with TouchCounts, which is a multi-touch application designed to support early number sense. I work through the many arm, hand and finger actions made by the child, both manually on the screen and digitally in the air, focusing on the translations of these actions across contexts, which I understand as learning through remembering. I then discuss some consequences of these methods, which involve narrative and re-enactment, on knowledge production, both for learners and for researchers, specifically when digital technologies are involved.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The video clip is not available for general viewing or use.

Notes

  1. Lather is interested in transgressing traditional forms of scientific validity that constitute representational and postpositivist assumptions. She proposes four forms of validity that are in better alignment with poststructural research paradigms. I note that the embodied one was deployed long before mathematics educators began focusing on theories of embodiment.

  2. Like many people, I find transcripts hard to ‘read’. I get lost in the symbols used to indicate non-linguistic aspects of the event and often find the descriptions of gestures and actions hard to imagine. A few years ago, in an article written for a special issue on language and mathematics for a French language journal, my co-author and I (Forte and Sinclair, 2019) decided to provide a more narrative description of a classroom video. Jérôme Proulx contacted me to tell me how much that narrative description was engaging and convincing, a comment that surprised me at the time, but it has remained with me ever since, no doubt contributing to the present writing.

  3. Given the positioning of the video camera, my own movements are harder to see. A better re-enactment would involve (at least) two people, which would bring in eye contact, timing of movement and speech and positioning. I think all this is potentially quite relevant, but for this first attempt at image re-enactment, I restrict the focus to Ruby.

  4. The word remembering is etymologically associated with the mind, as retaining in the mind, from the Latin memor (mindful). Given Ruby’s specific form of remembering, I prefer to associate the word ‘member’ to the meaning of body part or limb rather than just to the mind. Remembering thus becomes a fully integrated mind–body process.

  5. There are some connections with the notion of concept image, but significant differences as well: for Barsalou, the experience is full (including social, emotional) and not just cognitive or strictly mathematical; also, while concept image is often assumed to be some coherent set of ideas that make up one’s understanding, Barsalou is explicit about the layers of experience, which may have overlaps, but are not parsed into categories such as pictures, properties and associated problems.

  6. There is a large body of research around the ideas of quantum cognition (see Wendt, 2015), though Barsalou’s connects specifically to theories of embodiment. See also de Freitas and Sinclair (2017) for an interpretation of the quantum perspective in the context of mathematics education. See also Forde (2021) for a quantum-informed theorising of mathematics education.

  7. Here, the apparatus of quantum mechanics becomes the situation, and that situation produces the concept (of addition, in this case) in the same way that the two-slit apparatus is a measurement that produces the concept (of particle) (Barad, 2007).

References 

  • Arzarello, F., Robutti, O., & Thomas, M. (2015). Growth point and gestures: Looking inside mathematical meanings. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 90(1), 19–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barad, K. (2007). Meeting the universe hallway: Quantum physics and the entanglements of matter and meaning. Duke University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. (2020). Challenges and opportunities for grounding cognition. Journal of Cognition, 3(1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Freitas, E., & Sinclair, N. (2014). Mathematics and the body: Material entanglements in the classroom. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. Perigree.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, L. (2009). Gesture and conceptual integration in mathematical talk. Education Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 127–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, L., Ferrara, F. & Moore-Russo, D. (2017). Emerging Perspectives on gesture and embodiment in mathematics. Charlotte, SC: Information Age Publishing.

  • Ferrara, F., & Ferrari, G. (2017). Agency and assemblage in pattern generalisation: A materialist approach to learning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 94(1), 21–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forde, J. (2021). Mathematics as the science of material assemblage: Enactivist, quantum theoretical, and educational perspectives. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Burnaby, BC: Simon Fraser University.

  • Forte, M. & Sinclair, N. (2019). Les intra-actions sociomatérielles au service de l’apprentissage mathématique. Education et francophonie, XLVII(3). https://www.acelf.ca/c/revue/pdf/EF-47-3-083_FORTE.pdf.

  • Gerofsky, S. (2011). Seeing the graph vs. being the graph: Gesture, engagement and awareness in school mathematics. In G. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 245–257). John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gunes, C., Paton, S. & Sinclair, N. (in review) n.d..The sensory politics of mathematics: Aestheticizing multiplication.

  • Jackiw, N., & Sinclair, N. (2014). TouchCounts. Software Application for the iPad. Simon Fraser University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackiw, N., & Sinclair, N. (2017). TouchCounts and gesture design. In T. Hammond, A. Adler, & M. Prasad (Eds.), Frontiers in Pen and Touch (pp. 51–64). Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2000). Where mathematics come from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lather, P. (1993). Fertile obsession: Validity after poststructuralism. Sociological Quarterly, 34(4), 673–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lavie, I., Steiner, A., & Sfard, A. (2019). Routines we live by: From ritual to exploration. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 101(2), 153–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. University of Chicago Press; Chicago.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nasheeda, A., Abdullah, H. B., Krauss, S. E., & Ahmed, N. B. (2019). Transforming transcripts into stories: A multimethod approach to narrative analysis. International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 18. https://doi.org/10.1177/1609406919856797

  • Nemirovsky, R. (2011). Episodic feelings and transfer of learning. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 20(2), 308–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R., & Duprez, D. (2023). Tessellation, shamanism, and being alive to things. Oxford Review of Education, 49(4), 496–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky, R., Kelton, M. L., & Rhodehamel, B. (2013). Playing mathematical instruments: Emerging perceptuomotor integration with an interactive mathematics exhibit. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 44(2), 372–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nemirovsky , R., Lara-Meloy, T., Earnest, D., & Ribeiro, B. T. (2001). Videopapers: Investigating new multimedia genres to foster the interweaving of research and teaching. Paper presented at the 25th Meeting of the International group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Utrecht University. The Netherlands.

  • Parikka, J. (2023). Operational Images: From the visual to the invisual. University of Minnesota Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2011). Geometry as objective science in elementary classrooms: Mathematics in the flesh. Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shapin, S. (1984). Pump and circumstance: Robert Boyle’s literary technology. Social Studies of Science, 14(4), 481–520. https://doi.org/10.1177/030631284014004001

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, N., & de Freitas, E. (2014). The haptic nature of gesture: Rethinking gesture with new multitouch digital technologies. Gesture, 14(3), 351–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinclair, N., & Heyd-Metzuyanim, E. (2014). Learning number with TouchCounts: The role of emotions and the body in mathematical communication. Technology, Knowledge and Learning, 19(1–2), 81–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staats, S. (2008). Poetic lines in mathematics discourse: A method from linguistic anthropology. For the Learning of Mathematics, 8(2), 26–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Streeck, J. (2009). Gesturecraft: The manu-facture of meaning. John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vogelstein, L., Brady, C., & Hall, R. (2019). Reenacting mathematical concepts found in large-scale dance performance can provide both material and method for ensemble learning. ZDM Mathematics Education, 51(2), 331–346.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wendt, A. (2015). Quantum mind and social science: Unifying physical and social ontology (3rd ed.). Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Deep appreciation to Ricardo Nemirovsky, Sean Chorney, Sheena Tan and Anna Baccaglini-Frank for their perceptive comments, which led to many improvements, and to David Pimm for always being a dependable and generous first reader and inveterate editor.

Funding

This research was funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada. Grant number: 435–2018-0433.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Since this is a solo-authored article, it goes without saying that I am fully responsible for all parts of the article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nathalie Sinclair.

Ethics declarations

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Conflict of Interest

There are not conflicts of interest to declare.   

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Sinclair, N. Knowing as Remembering: Methodological Experiments in Embodied Experiences of Number. Digit Exp Math Educ 10, 29–46 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40751-023-00132-7

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40751-023-00132-7

Keywords

Navigation