Skip to main content
Log in

Reflecting on RFT and the Reticulating Strategy: a Response to Villatte, Villatte, and Hayes

  • Book Review
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Notes

  1. In their response, Villatte et al. (in press) point out that the number of RFT-based publications has been growing, citing examples and a recent review article by O'Connor, Farrell, Munnelly, and McHugh (2017). A growing number of publications do not, however, constitute genuine conceptual development, nor reflect the health of RFT. Consider, for example, that approximately one third of the published empirical RFT articles cited by O’Connor et al. were co-authored by a single individual. To compound matters, approximately one third of the empirical articles were focused on ‘implicit cognition’ (i.e., most were papers on the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure, the IRAP). This relatively narrow focus and heavy dependence on a single researcher are in our view cause for concern, rather than celebration. Of course, we are not arguing that no progress has been made, simply that the progress seems relatively slow when the authorship and content of the papers are considered.

  2. Indeed, the MDML appears to render the whole debate around middle-level versus technical terms redundant. Specifically, the precision of scientific terms is not dichotomous (i.e., middle-level versus technical), but is a matter of degree. For example, the first level of the MDML is mutually entailing, which is widely accepted as a technical term in RFT, but remains relatively imprecise until you specify a particular relation and only gains increasing precision as you specify the dimensions along which it can be measured. Precision is not, therefore, just a matter of debate or argument, but is defined, in part, through a particular experimental analysis.

References

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Hussey, I., & Luciano, C. (2016). Relational frame theory: Finding its historical and philosophical roots and reflecting upon its future development: An introduction to part II. In R. Zettle, S. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & A. Biglan (Eds.), The handbook of contextual behavioral science. West Sussex: Wiley Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Luciano. C., & McEnteggart, C. (2017). From the IRAP and REC model to a multi-dimensional multi-level framework for analyzing the dynamics of arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science 66(4), 434–445. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.08.001.

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Finn, M., McEnteggart, C., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (in press). Derived stimulus relations and their role in a behavior-analytic account of human language and cognition. Perspectives in behavioral science (formerly the behavior analyst), Special Issue on Derived Relations.

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., McEnteggart, C., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (in press). Symbolic thought and communication: An RFT perspective. In S. Hayes & D. S. Wilson (Eds.), Evolution and contextual behavioral science: A reunification. Oakland: Context Press, New Harbinger.

  • Barnes-Holmes, Y., Kavanagh, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Finn, M., Harte, C., …, & McEnteggart, C. (in press). Review: Mastering the clinical conversation: Language as intervention. In M. Villatte, J. L. Villatte, & S. C. Hayes (Eds.), The Psychological Record. New York, NY: Guilford Press. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0229-0.

  • Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & McEnteggart, C. (2017). Persistent rule-following in the face of reversed reinforcement contingencies: The differential impact of direct versus derived rules. Behavior Modification, 41(6), 743–763.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Connor, M., Farrell, L., Munnelly, A., & McHugh, L. (2017). Citation analysis of relational frame theory: 2009-2016. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6, 152–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.04.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Villatte, M., Villatte, J. L., & Hayes, S. C. (in press). A reticulated and progressive strategy for developing clinical applications of RFT. The Psychological Record. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0251-2.

Download references

Funding

This book review was prepared with the funding from the FWO Type I Odysseus Programme at Gent University, Belgium, awarded to Dermot Barnes-Holmes.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ciara McEnteggart.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

This article does not contain any studies with human participants or animals performed by any of the authors.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Barnes-Holmes, Y., Kavanagh, D., Barnes-Holmes, D. et al. Reflecting on RFT and the Reticulating Strategy: a Response to Villatte, Villatte, and Hayes. Psychol Rec 68, 119–121 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0272-5

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0272-5

Navigation