Skip to main content
Log in

What Are We Missing? How Language Impacts Trauma Narratives

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The potential for the development of psychopathology in aolescent refugees and asylees is high due to the trauma inherent in their experience. Yet, psychopathology rooted in trauma has proven amenable to treatment. Nonetheless, as most clinicians are monolingual, the language difference between clinician and client may be a barrier of desensitization and processing typically characteristic of trauma therapy. Thus, this study aimed to describe qualitative differences in speech production among native and non-native narratives using Linguistic Inquiry and Word Count (LIWC) processing software (Pennebaker et al. 2015) to understand if the current best practice will function similarly in these populations. We compared 10 adolescent immigrants (50% male) who narrated events that provoked their migration to the U.S. in their second language (L2; i.e., English) to 10 age- and gender-matched adolescents narrating in their first language (L1; i.e., Spanish). Results revealed L1 narratives were significantly higher in their use of/talk about anger, cognitive processes, discrepancy, tentativeness, perceptual processes, ingestion, relativity, time, work, and home. L2 narratives were higher in their use of/talk about positive emotions, death, causation, health, motion, space, and fillers. Findings have implications for the efficacy of treatments using discourse to ameliorate symptoms related to trauma in non-native languages.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Altarriba, J. (2003). Does cariño equal “liking”? A theoretical approach to conceptual nonequivalence between languages. The International Journal of Bilingualism, 3, 305–322.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altarriba, J., & Bauer, L. M. (2004). The distinctiveness of emotion concepts: A comparison between emotion, abstract, and concrete words. The American Journal of Psychology, 117(3), 389–410. https://doi.org/10.2307/4149007.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayçiçeği, A., & Harris, C. L. (2004). Brief report: Bilinguals' recall and recognition of emotion words. Cognition and Emotion, 18(7), 977–987. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930341000301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balderas, C. N., Delgado-Romero, E. A., & Singh, A. A. (2016). Sin papeles: Latino parent–child conversations about undocumented legal status. Journal of Latina/o Psychology, 4(3), 158–172. https://doi.org/10.1037/lat0000060.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boals, A., & Klein, K. (2005). Word use in emotional narratives about failed romantic relationships and subsequent mental health. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 24, 252–268. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X05278386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bond, G. D., & Lee, A. Y. (2005). Language of lies in prison: Linguistic classification of prisoners’ truthful and deceptive natural language. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 19, 313–329.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buchanan, T. W. (2007). Retrieval of emotional memories. Psychological Bulletin, 133(5), 761–779. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.761.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cantin-Paquet, M. O. (2017). Syria: Examining the roots of the present civil war. Conflict Studies Quarterly, 19, 85–95.

  • Capecelatro, M. R., Sacchet, M. D., Hitchcock, P. F., Miller, S. M., & Britton, W. B. (2013). Major depression duration reduces appetitive word use: An elaborated verbal recall of emotional photographs. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 47(6), 809–815.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, C. K., Jones, C., Liu, A., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2008). Predicting success and failure in weight loss blogs through natural language use. In International Conference on Weblogs and Social Media (ICWSM), Seattle, Washington. Retreived from https://www.aaai.org/Papers/ICWSM/2008/ICWSM08-033.pdf.

  • Cohn, D. (2015). Future immigration will change the face of America by 2065. Retreived from, http://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2015/10/05/future-immigration-will-change-the-face-of-america-by-2065/. Accessed Dec 2016.

  • Davis, L., Fabbri, P., & Alphonse, M. (2014). Gender country profile DRC. Retrieved from https://www.lauradavis.eu/wp-content/uploads/2014/07/Gender-Country-Profile-DRC-2014.pdf. Accessed Dec 2016.

  • Dekraker, J., & Köhler, S. (2015). Psychophysiological evidence for the role of emotion in adaptive memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(5), 925.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewaele, J. (2008). The emotional weight of I love you in multilinguals' languages. Journal of Pragmatics, 40(10), 1753–1780. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2008.03.002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foa, E. B., & Kozak, M. J. (1986). Emotional processing of fear: Exposure to corrective information. Psychological Bulletin, 99(1), 20–35.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gusmano, M. K. (2012). Undocumented immigrants in the United States: Use of healthcare. Retrieved from http://www.undocumentedpatients.org/issuebrief/health-care-use/. Accessed Dec 2016.

  • Halligan, S. L., Clark, D. M., & Ehlers, A. (2002). Cognitive processing, memory, and the development of PTSD symptoms: Two experimental analogue studies. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33(2), 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1016/S00057916(02)00014-9.

  • Hofstede, G. H. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, D., Alpers, G. W., Ismailji, T., Classen, C., Wales, T., Cheasty, V., Miller, A., & Koopman, C. (2007). Cognitive and emotional processing in narratives of women abused by intimate partners. Violence Against Women, 13, 1192–1205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, C., Jacobs, A. M., & Conrad, M. (2015). Can Harry potter still put a spell on us in a second language? An fMRI study on reading emotion-laden literature in late bilinguals. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior., 63, 282–295.

    Google Scholar 

  • Javier, R. A., Barroso, F., & Muñoz, M. A. (1993). Autobiographical memory in bilinguals. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 22(3), 319–338. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01068015.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kalist, D. E. (2005). Registered nurses and the value of bilingualism. Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 59(1), 101–118.

  • Kim-Prieto, C., & Eid, M. (2004). Norms for experiencing emotions in sub-saharan Africa. Journal of Happiness Studies, 5(3), 241–268. https://link.springer.com/journal/volumesAndIssues/10902. Accessed Dec 2016.

  • Kowalski, R. M. (2000). “I was only kidding!” victims’ and perpetrators’ perceptions of teasing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 231–241.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kupzyk, S. S., Banks, B. M., & Chadwell, M. R. (2016). Collaborating with refugee families to increase early literacy opportunities: A pilot investigation. Contemporary School Psychology, 20(3), 205–217.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marmolejo, G., Diliberto-Macaluso, K. A., & Altarriba, J. (2009). False memory in bilinguals: Does switching languages increase false memories? The American Journal of Psychology, 122(1), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, K., Venta, A., Henderson, C., Barker, M., & Sharp, C. (2017). Linguistic analysis as a method for assessing symptoms after sexual trauma among female adolescent psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Child Sexual Abuse, 1–17.

  • Matsumoto, D., Yoo, S. H., Fontaine, J., Anguas-Wong, A. M., Arriola, M., Ataca, B., et al. (2008). Mapping expressive differences around the world: The relationship between emotional display rules and individualism versus collectivism. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 39(1), 55–74. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107311854.

    Google Scholar 

  • Migration Policy Institute. (2017). Central American immigrants in the United States. Retrieved from https://www.migrationpolicy.org/article/central-american-immigrants-united-states. Accessed March 2017.

  • Newman, M. L., Groom, C. J., Handelman, L. D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2008). Gender differences in language use: An analysis of 14,000 text samples. Discourse Processes, 45(3), 211–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norris, P. (2009). Why do Arab states lag the world in gender equality? HKS Working Paper No. RWP09–020. Retrieved from https://ssrn.com/abstract=1474820. Accessed Dec 2016.

  • Pair Project (n.d.). Appearing at a master calendar hearing in immigration court. Retrieved from http://www.pairproject.org/resources/MCH%20and%20Pleadings.pdf. Accessed Dec 2016.

  • Pasupathi, M. (2007). Telling and the remembered self: Linguistic differences in memories for previously disclosed and previously undisclosed events. Memory, 15(3), 258–270. https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210701256456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pavlenko, A. (2002). Bilingualism and emotions. Multilingual, 21, 45–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennebaker, J. W., Booth, R. J., Boyd, R. L., & Francis, M. E. (2015). Linguistic inquiry and word count: LIWC2015. Austin: Pennebaker Conglomerates. www.LIWC.net. Accessed Dec 2016.

  • Ramzan, N., & Amjad, N. (2017). Cross cultural variation in emotion regulation: A systematic review. Annals of King Edward Medical University, 23(1), 77–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schauer, M., Neuner, F., & Elbert, T. (2005). Narrative exposure therapy: A short-term intervention for traumatic stress disorders after war, terror, or torture. Cambridge: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schrauf, R. W., & Rubin, D. C. (1998). Bilingual autobiographical memory in older adult immigrants: A test of cognitive explanations of the reminiscence bump and the linguistic encoding of memories. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(3), 437–457. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sim, M. P., & Lamb, M. E. (2013). Children's disclosure of child sexual abuse: How motivational factors affect linguistic categories related to deception detection. Psychology, Crime & Law, 19(8), 649–660.

  • Tausczik, Y. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2010). The psychological meaning of words: LIWC and computerized text analysis methods. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 29(1), 24–54. https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X09351676.

    Google Scholar 

  • United Nations, Department of Economic and Social Affairs. (2015). [Table of total migrant stock at mid-year by origin and by major area, region, country or area of destination, 2015]. Trends in international migrant stock: The 2015 revision. Retrieved from http://www.un.org/en/development/desa/population/migration/data/estimates2/estimates15.shtml. Accessed Dec 2016.

  • Venta, A. (2016). Migration Experiences Interview. (unpublished manuscript), Sam Houston State University.

  • Venta, A., Bailey, C., Muñoz, C., & Shmueli-Goetz, Y. (2016). Entrevista sobre apego para la juventud. (unpublished manuscript), Sam Houston State University.

  • Venta, A., Muñoz, C., & Bailey, C. (2017). What language does your internal working model of attachment speak? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 48(6), 813–834. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117704053.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vrij, A., Mann, S., Kristen, S., & Fisher, R. P. (2007). Cues to deception and ability to detect lies as a function of police interview styles. Law and Human Behavior, 31, 499–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Winter, D. G. (1998). A motivational analysis of the Clinton first term and the 1996 presidential campaign. The Leadership Quarterly, 9, 367–376.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by the Sam Houston State University Enhancement Research Grant Amanda Venta, Principal Investigator. Special thank you to members of the Youth and Family Studies Lab who helped make this project possible.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Amanda Venta.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Ethical Standards and Informed Consent

All procedures followed were in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation [institutional and national] and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000. Informed consent was obtained from all patients for being included in the study.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic Supplementary Material

ESM 1

(DOCX 29 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bailey, C., McIntyre, E., Arreola, A. et al. What Are We Missing? How Language Impacts Trauma Narratives. Journ Child Adol Trauma 13, 153–161 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40653-019-00263-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40653-019-00263-3

Keywords

Navigation