Skip to main content
Log in

Skeptic’s Corner: Punishment — Destructive Force or Valuable Social “Adhesive”?

  • Discussion and Review Paper
  • Published:
Behavior Analysis in Practice Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Skepticism that normally focuses on pseudoscientific claims can also be directed at “established” principles of behavior. After discussing some ways in which empirically-derived principles can potentially mislead, as an illustrative example I describe some reasons to wonder whether our understanding of punishment is as established as sometimes assumed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Yale University commencement speech, June 11, 1962; retrieved from http://millercenter.org/president/speeches/detail/3370.

  2. My comments also are selective in that I will steer clear of certain practical matters regarding punishment’s position in society. Examples include legal restrictions on clinical applications of punishment in many jurisdictions and the view of clinical punishment as unkind, abusive, or otherwise objectionable in many codes of clinical ethics. Lengthy treatises could be devoted to examining how punishment got to be so unpopular and how current legal and ethical stances square with the empirical evidence on punishment. In the latter case, interested readers may find that Cipani (2004) provides a good start.

  3. A common corollary to the First Core Truth is that efforts to harness punishment for therapeutic purposes are destined to fail (e.g., Vargas 2009). This is a complex topic that deserves more attention than be provided here, but I will mention two relevant issues before moving on. First, many interventions seek to build new repertoires and, unlike positive reinforcement, punishment does not teach what to do. I agree that under most circumstances positive reinforcement is preferred for constructing new repertoires. Second, for a variety of reasons, it is often said that punishment interventions do not actually eliminate unwanted behavior. This claim runs counter to a number of published reports of effective applications of punishment, both alone and in combination with positive reinforcement procedures (e.g., Lindsheid, Iwata, Rickets, Williams, & Griffiths, 1990; see also Lerman and Vorndran 2002).

  4. Some of my colleagues might discount this external evidence due to the fact that relevant studies were conducted using experimental methods in which they have limited faith. While part of skepticism does require considering the quality of empirical evidence, the wholesale dismissal of a literature rarely enhances a discipline’s credibility. I am reminded here of Hearst’s (1967) characterization of the typical basic behavior analyst as “a hard-nosed experimentalist who … attacks anything that sounds even mildly theoretical or physiological, ridicules anyone who has ever used statistics of the R.A. Fisher variety, and ignores the work of any psychologist who does not publish in the Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior” (unpaginated).

  5. Cooperation can be increased by making it the most individually profitable strategy, but under such circumstances individual self-interest aligns with the common good and the task no longer parallels the social dilemmas that cooperation games were intended to model.

  6. These results do not indicate whether the punishment game was preferred because penalties enforce cooperation, because it “feels good” to punish non-cooperation, or because of some combination of these factors. The noteworthy point, however, is that almost all players chose to place themselves in a game where they could be penalized. These findings have limited precedent in the behavior analysis literature. In a few cases, individuals have been shown to prefer contingencies involving aversive stimulation to contingencies involving only positive reinforcement (e.g., Hanley et al. 2005; Iwata and Bailey 1974). In these cases, however, the contingencies were strictly individual, and beneficial outcomes were measured at the individual level. The present focus is on preference for punishment-based social contingencies.

  7. The “better outcomes” are defined socially; that is, “greatest common good” means maximizing collective, not individual, benefits. In both laboratory and clinic, behavior analysts usually seek to identify functional relations governing individual behavior. Thus, the available evidence on not just punishment, but most behavioral phenomena, comes from this level of analysis. One wonders what other behavioral phenomena, besides punishment, might look different to us if viewed through the lens of interactive social relations.

  8. To those who find this conclusion about the potential social benefits of punishment unsettling, take heart: Research also indicates that when given the opportunity players in cooperation games will reward others’ cooperation, even when this costs them some of their own earnings. Such player-mediated rewards tend to increase the overall level of cooperation in the game (Balliet et al. 2011; Szolnoki and Perc 2010). As with the punishment contingencies I have described, however, these are “additional” contingencies, superimposed upon the primary contingencies embedded in the game, and thus do not change the general observation that cooperation is unreliable under typical payoff systems modeling social dilemmas. Additionally, cooperation is enhanced more by allowing players to penalize non-cooperation than by allowing them to reward cooperation (Baillet et al. 2011). Thus, my general point about the potential value of punishment appears to hold.

References

  • Ahearn, W. H. (2010). What every behavior analyst should know about the “MMR cases autism” hypothesis. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 3(1), 46–50.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adcock, J. (1995). The belief engine. Skeptical Inquirer, 19(3). Found online at http://www.csicop.org/si/show/belief_engine/.

  • Alenberg, J., Dreber, A., Apicella, C. L., & Rand, D. G. (2011). Third party reward and punishment: group size, efficiency, and public goods. In N. M. Palmetti & J. P. Russo (Eds.), Psychology of punishment (pp. 1–19). New York: Nova Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J. (1988). Why free ride? Strategies and learning in public goods experiments. Journal of Pubic Economics, 37, 291–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H., & Holz, W. C. (1966). Punishment. In W. K. Honig (Ed.), Operant behavior: areas of research and application (pp. 380–447). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balliet, D., Mulder, L. B., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2011). Reward, punishment, and cooperation: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 594–615.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowles, S., & Gintis, H. (2013). A cooperative species: human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizeable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13, 171–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (2007). Learning (interim 4th ed.). Cornwall-on-Hudson: Sloan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chesterton, G. K. (1923). Fancies and fads. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chok, J. T., Reed, D. D., Kennedy, A., & Byrd, F. L. (2010). A single-case experimental analysis of the effects of ambient prism lenses for an adolescent with developmental disabilities. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 3(2), 42–51.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cipani, E. (2004). Punishment on trial: a resource guide to child discipline. Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T. S. (2011). Interesting times: practice, science, and professional organizations in behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst, 34, 297–310.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T. S., Doepke, K. J., & Campbell, R. L. (in press). Origins of clinical innovations: Why practice needs science and how science reaches practice. To appear in F.D. DeGenarro Reed & D.D. Reed (Eds.), Bridging the gap between science and practice in autism service delivery. New York: Springer.

  • Critchfield, T. S., & Rassmussen, E. R. (2007). It’s aversive to have an incomplete science of behavior. Mexican Journal of Behavior Analysis, 33, 1–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T. S., & Reed, D. D. (2004). Conduits of translation in behavior-science bridge research. In J. E. Burgos & E. Ribes (Eds.), Theory, basic and applied research, and technological applications in behavior science: conceptual and methodological issues (pp. 45–84). Guadalajara: University of Guadalajara Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daniels, A. C., & Daniels, J. E. (2006). Performance management: changing behavior that drives organizational effectiveness. Atlanta: Performance Management Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawes, R. (1980). Social dilemmas. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 169–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deluty, M. Z. (1976). Choice and the rate of punishment in concurrent schedules. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 25, 75–80.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Quervain, D. J.-F., Fischbacher, U., Treyer, V., Schellhammer, M., Schnyder, U., Buck, A., & Fehr, E. (2004). The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science, 305, 1254–1258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, P. A. (1980). Toward a quantitative theory of punishment. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 33, 15–25.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1954). Punishment I: the avoidance hypothesis. Psychological Review, 61, 34–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1998). Punishment. In W. T. O’Donohue (Ed.), Learning and behavior therapy (pp. 188–204). Needham Heights: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Gatcher, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 405, 137–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gaiman, N. (1990). Dream of a thousand cats. Baldwin: DC Comics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gershoff, E. T. (2002). Corporal punishment by parents and associated child behaviors and experiences: a meta-analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 539–579.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis, H. (2008). Punishment and cooperation. Science, 319, 1345–1346.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gurek, O., Irlenbusch, B., & Rockenbach, B. (2006). The competitive advantage of sanctioning institutions. Science, 312, 108–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., Fisher, W. W., & Maglieri, K. A. (2005). On the effectiveness of and preference for punishment and extinction components of function-based interventions. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 51–65.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hearst, E. (1967). The behavior of Skinnerians. PsycCRITIQUES, 12(8) (electronic).

  • Heinrich, J., et al. (2006). Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312, 1767–1770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, B., Thoni, C., & Gatcher, S. (2008). Antisocial punishment across societies. Science, 319, 1362–1367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iwata, B. A., & Bailey, J. S. (1974). Reward versus cost token systems: an analysis of the effects on students and teacher. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 567–576.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Krantz, D. L. (1972). Schools and systems: the mutual isolation of operant and non-operant psychology as a case study. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 8, 86–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larzelere, R. E., & Kuhn, B. R. (2005). Comparing child outcomes of physical punishment and alternative disciplinary tactics: a meta-analysis. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 8, 1–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, D. C., Sansbury, T., Hovanetz, A., Wolever, E., Garcia, A., O’Brien, E., & Adepipe, H. (2008). Using behavior analysis to examine the outcomes of unproven therapies: an evaluation of hyperbaric oxygen therapy for children with autism. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(2), 50–58.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lerman, D. C., & Vorndran, C. M. (2002). On the status of knowledge for using punishment: Implications for treating behavior disorders. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 431–464.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lindsheid, T. R., Iwata, B. A., Ricketts, R. W., Williams, D. E., & Griffin, J. C. (1990). Clinical evaluation of the Self Injurious Behavior Inhibiting System (SIBIS). Journal of applied Behavior Analysis, 23, 53-78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucas, J. R. (2006). Broaden the vision. Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malott, R. M., Lyon, D., & Malott, M. E. (2002). A history of the association for behavior analysis. ABA Newsletter, 23(3), 5–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Weibe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and noninsight problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 15, 238–246. doi:10.3758/bf03197722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1940). An experimental analogue of “regression”, with incidental observations on “reaction formation”. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 35, 56–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Normand, M. P. (2008). Science, skepticism, and applied behavior analysis. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 1(2), 42–49.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paolucci, E. O., & Violato, C. (2004). A meta-analysis on the affective, cognitive, and behavioral effects of corporal punishment. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 138, 197–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pryor, K. (2002). Don’t shoot the dog! The new art of teaching and training. Reading: Ring Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rachlin, H., & Herrnstein, R. J. (1969). Hedonism revisited: on the negative law of effect. In B. A. Campbell & R. M. Church (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior (pp. 83–109). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russell, B. (1954/2009). Human society in ethics and politics. New York: Routledge.

  • Rutherford, A. (2009). Beyond the box: B.F. Skinner’s technology of behavior from laboratory to life, 1950s–1970s. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sarafino, E. P. (2012). Applied behavior analysis: principles and procedures for modifying behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (2000). Coercion and its fallout (revisedth ed.). Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: an experimental analysis. NY: Appleton-Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1948). Walden Two. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity. New York: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1993). A world of our own. Behaviorology, 1, 3–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Szolnoki, A., & Perc, M. (2010). Reward and cooperation in the spatial public goods game. Europhysics Letters, 92(3) 38003. Electronic: http://www.epl.journal.org

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: an experimental study of the associative processes in animals (Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements, No. 8). New York: Macmillan.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1913). Educational psychology (The fundamentals of learning, Vol. 2). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1932). The fundamentals of learning. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ulman, J. D. (1993). The Ulman–Skinner letters. Behaviorology, 1, 47–54.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Haaren, F. (2009). “Primum non nocere”: A review of Taking America off Drugs: Why Behavioral Therapy is more effective for treating ADHD, OCD, depression and other psychological problems by Stephen Ray Flora. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 2(2), 58–62.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vargas, J. S. (2009). Behavior analysis for effective teaching. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wheeler, J. J., & Richey, D. D. (2014). Behavior management: principles and practices of positive behavioral supports. Boston: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wyatt, W. J. (2009). Behavior analysis in the era of medicalization: the state of the science and recommendations for practitioners. Behavior Analysis in Practice, 2(2), 49–57.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas S. Critchfield.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Critchfield, T.S. Skeptic’s Corner: Punishment — Destructive Force or Valuable Social “Adhesive”?. Behav Analysis Practice 7, 36–44 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-014-0005-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40617-014-0005-4

Keywords

Navigation