Skip to main content
Log in

Multiple Exemplar Training: Some Strengths and Limitations

  • Original Research
  • Published:
The Behavior Analyst Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In successful multiple exemplar training, a set of exemplars that sample the range of stimulus and response topographies is trained, and the full range of performances results. Examples abound in experimental psychology and include abstraction and concept learning, responding to relations, identity matching, rule following, behavioral variability, responding to wh-questions, describing past events, learning sets, and continuous repertoires. Thus, behavior analysts often allude to a history of multiple exemplar training to account for different generalized performances. It is easy to see how a strategy of multiple exemplar training can work with many types of performances, even performances that involve relations between objects or events. However, there appear to be at least two exceptions, where direct multiple exemplar training does not work well: (1) when there are no physical dimensions at all along which generalized performances can emerge, and (2) when the relation between a stimulus and an effective response is complex. Interpretation of the latter type of cases in terms of mediated generalization is outlined and discussed. An experimental and conceptual research program should produce an account of the general limits of multiple exemplar training, and guidelines for the most effective training for generalized skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Baer, D. M. (1981). How to plan for generalization. Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1973). The concept of the operant in the analysis of behavior. Behaviorism, 1, 103–116.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (2013). Learning (5th ed.). Cornwall on Hudson: Sloan Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Engelmann, S., & Carnine, C. (1982). Theory of instruction: principles and applications. New York: Irvington Publishers, Inc.

  • Fields, L., Reeve, K. F., Matneja, P., Varelas, A., Belanich, J., Fitzer, A., et al. (2002). The formation of a generalized categorization repertoire: effects of training with multiple domains, samples, and comparisons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 291–313.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Ross, D. E. (2008). Verbal behavior analysis: inducing and expanding new verbal capabilities in children with language delays. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M., & Rivera-Valdes, C. (2005). The emergence of the listener to speaker component of naming in children as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 123–134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Stolfi, L., & Pistoljevic, N. (2007). Emergence of naming in preschoolers: a comparison of multiple and single exemplar instruction. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 8, 109–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttman, N., & Kalish, H. I. (1956). Discriminability and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51, 79–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, H. M. (1961). Stimulus generalization following three-stimulus discrimination training. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 54(2), 181–185.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harlow, H. F. (1949). The formation of learning sets. Psychological Review, 56(1), 51–65. doi:10.1037/h0062474.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: a précis. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: a post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 141–154). New York: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Fox, E., Gifford, E. V., Wilson, K. G., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Healy, O. (2001). Derived relational responding as learned behavior. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: a post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 21–49). New York: Kluwer.

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Loveland, D. H. (1964). Complex visual concept in the pigeon. Science, 146, 549–551.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., Loveland, D. H., & Cable, C. (1976). Natural concepts in pigeons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 4, 285–301.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holth, P. (2012). Variability as an operant? Behavior Analyst, 35(2), 243–248.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Holth, P. (2013). Different sciences as answers to different why questions. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 14, 165–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horne, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, R. H., Sprague, J., & Wilcox, B. (1982). General case programming for community activitie. In B. Wilcox & G. T. Bellamy (Eds.), Design of high school programs for severely handicapped students (pp. 61–98). Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., & Rusch, F. R. (1989). Teaching supported employees with severe mental retardation to solve problems. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 22, 365–372. doi:10.1901/jaba.1989.22-365.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C. L. (1920). Quantitative aspects of evolution of concepts: an experimental study. Psychological Monographs, 28, i–86. doi:10.1037/h0093130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jahr, E. (2001). Teaching children with autism to answer novel wh-questions by utilizing a multiple exemplar strategy. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 22, 407–423.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, J. M. (1979). On the relation between generalization and generality. Behavior Analyst, 2, 1–6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Kalish, H. I., & Guttman, N. (1959). Stimulus generalization after training on three stimuli: a test of the summation hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 268–272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kalish, H. I., & Haber, A. (1963). Generalization: I. Generalization gradients from single and multiple stlmulus points. II. Generalization of inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 176–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kifer, R. E., Lewis, M. A., Green, D. R., & Phillips, E. L. (1974). Training predelinquent youths and their parents to negotiate conflict situations. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 7, 357–364.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lovaas, O. I. (1981). Teaching developmentally disabled children: the me book. Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1991). Joint control and the generalization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–126.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1996). Joint control and word-object bidirectionality. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 252–255.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Luiselli, J. K., Russo, D. C., Christian, W. P., & Wilczynski, S. M. (Eds.). (2008). Effective practices for children with autism. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malott, R., & Siddall, J. W. (1972). Acquisition of the people concept in the pigeon. Psychological Reports, 31, 3–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maurice, C., Green, G., & Luce, S. (Eds.). (1996). Behavioral intervention for young children with autism: a manual for parents and professionals. Austin: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuringer, A. (2009). Operant variability and the power of reinforcement. The Behavior Analyst Today, 10(2), 319–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Neill, R. E. (1990). Establishing verbal repertiores: toward the application of general case analysis and programming. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 113–126.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Page, S., & Neuringer, A. (1985). Variability is an operant. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Processes, 11, 429–452.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, D. C. (1991). A behavioral interpretation of memory. In L. J. Hayes & P. N. Chase (Eds.), Dialogues on verbal behavior (pp. 261–279). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulson, C. L., & Kymissis, E. (1996). The operant language-acquisition psradigm and its empirical support. In S. W. Bijou & E. Ribes (Eds.), New directions in behavior development (pp. 73–89). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulson, C. L., Kyparissos, N., Andreatos, M., Kymissis, E., & Parnes, M. (2002). Generalized imitation within three response classes in typically developing infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 81, 341–357. doi:10.1006/jecp.2002.2661.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schlinger, H., & Blakely, E. (1987). Function-altering effects of contingency-specifying stimuli. The Behavior Analyst, 10(1), 41–45.

  • Sidman, M. (1979). Remarks. Behaviorism, 7(2), 123–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1935). The generic nature of the concepts of stimulus and response. The Journal of General Psychology, 12(1), 40–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: an experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillian.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1968). The technology of teaching. New York: Appleton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: a theoretical analysis. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprague, J. R., & Horner, R. H. (1984). The effects of single instance, multiple instance, and general case training on generalized vending machine use by moderately and severely handicapped students. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 17, 273–278.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (1977). An implicit technology of generalization. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 10, 349–367.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Stokes, T. F., & Baer, D. M. (2003). Mediated generalization: an unfinished portrait. In K. S. Budd & T. Stokes (Eds.), A small matter of proof: the legacy of Donald M. Baer (pp. 125–138). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1911/2000). Animal intelligence: experimental studies. New Brunswick: Transaction Publishers.

  • Wildemann, D. G., & Holland, J. G. (1972). Control of a continuous response dimension by a continuous stimulus dimension. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 18, 419–434.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Young, J. M., Krantz, P. J., McClannahan, L. E., & Poulson, C. L. (1994). Generalized imitation and response-class formation in children with autism. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 685–697.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Per Holth.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that he has no conflict of interest.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Holth, P. Multiple Exemplar Training: Some Strengths and Limitations. BEHAV ANALYST 40, 225–241 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-017-0083-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-017-0083-z

Keywords

Navigation