Abstract
Purpose
Body weight dissatisfaction (BWD) and visual body perception are specific aspects that can influence the own body image, and that can concur with the development or the maintenance of specific psychopathological dimensions of different psychiatric disorders. The sexual orientation is a fundamental but understudied aspect in this field, and, for this reason, the purpose of this study is to improve knowledge about the relationships among BWD, visual body size-perception, and sexual orientation.
Methods
A total of 1033 individuals participated in an online survey. Physical comparison, depression, and self-esteem was evaluated, as well as sexual orientation and the presence of an eating disorder. A Figure Rating Scale was used to assess different valences of body weight, and mediation analyses were performed to investigated specific relationships between psychological aspects.
Results
Bisexual women and gay men reported significantly higher BWD than other groups (p < 0.001); instead, higher body misperception was present in gay men (p = 0.001). Physical appearance comparison mediated the effect of sexual orientation in both BWD and perceptual distortion. No difference emerged between women with a history of eating disorders and without, as regards the value of body weight attributed to attractiveness, health, and presence on social media.
Conclusion
This study contributes to understanding the relationship between sexual orientations and body image representation and evaluation. Physical appearance comparisons should be considered as critical psychological factors that can improve and affect well-being. The impact on subjects with high levels of eating concerns is also discussed.
Level of evidence
Level III: case–control analytic study.
Introduction
The way people perceive their bodies is an essential aspect of daily intrapersonal and interpersonal interactions; indeed, a negative body image is associated with eating disorders (ED), adverse sexual experiences, low self-esteem, depression, and anxiety [1,2,3]. Body image is influenced by cultural factors, interpersonal skills, physical changes over time, personality characteristics, self-esteem, and quality of life [4,5,6,7]. Recently, the literature has considered genders and sexual orientations as significant factors that might have a significant effect on one’s body evaluation, but bisexual women and men are often misrepresented in the literature and included with other sexual minorities [8,9,10,11,12].
The presence of a discrepancy between the body’s perception and the ideal body that people struggle to achieve can be defined as body image disturbance [13]. Body image disturbance is related to the body dissatisfaction construct, even if it is less linked to the affective elements and more to the perception feature [14, 15]. However, this construct seems more complicated than just the discrepancy between two body sizes [16]. Different elements could be included in the definition of body image disturbance, starting from body perception [17]. Indeed, the body size estimation accuracy is linked to top/down cognitive mechanisms and cognitive schemas that could bias the recall from memory of one’s own body [18,19,20]. Another element that could be considered in the body image disturbance is body weight dissatisfaction, which refers only to the body’s weight-related visual appearance, showing the evaluation discrepancy reported by subjects [19]. Studies on these elements are growing in the last decades [21] but results are still preliminary in the sexual minority population.
There is a broad consensus that heterosexual women experience normative pressure to achieve specific ideal body sizes [22]. However, lesbian and bisexual women remain underrepresented in the literature, with unclear results due to opposing evidence and mixed sampling methodologies [22,23,24]. Data have recently suggested that bisexual women are more likely than women with other sexual orientations to internalize shape/weight overvaluation and body dissatisfaction [25]. However, more research is needed to confirm these points. The data in the literature seem to be more consistent when it comes to the higher prevalence of body dissatisfaction in gay than in heterosexual men, which is probably due to the emphasis placed on physical appearance in the gay culture and because of pressure from social and interpersonal factors [9, 26,27,28]. For example, it has been suggested that—due to different beauty ideals and their minority status—gay men are slightly more vulnerable to body dissatisfaction than heterosexual men [29,30,31,32].
The evaluation of one’s body image includes the concerns about body weight and shape, the perception thereof, and the ideal body appearance, linked to a specific mood, self-esteem, and cultural environment [33, 34]. Differences in visual body perception have been shown between genders in heterosexual cisgender, with different brain activation linked to their own body and opposite-sex bodies, but no study is available for sexual minorities [35]. The influence of socio-cultural aspects linked to body appearance could bring gay men and women to show a higher misperception of their body, but also for this aspect, few studies are available, and bisexual persons are misrepresented in the existing literature [28]. However, this is a relevant clinical aspect that is crucial in the treatment of body image distortion in eating disorder patients, and more studies are needed in this field [36].
However, in the literature, the psychometric instruments used have not been sufficiently sensitive to differences between subgroups with different socio-cultural pressures about body evaluation, and this could explain the mixed results obtained in the literature [10, 25]. Body dissatisfaction tasks are devised using assumptions about what shape aspects might be relevant for the specific target group (e.g., focusing on muscularity, thinness, or specific body regions). For this reason, body weight dissatisfaction is typically evaluated with Figure Rating Scales (FRS), which are composed of pictures, drawings, or human models displaying a specific weight range and can separate visual distortion components from distorted attitudes [37]. Unfortunately, body weight dissatisfaction among individuals with different sexual orientations is a neglected topic, even though body evaluation plays a well-known role in how people evaluate their quality of life [38, 39].
Our goal is to study different facets of body weight and body image perception in a significant sample of cisgender women and men with different sexual orientations. Recent evidence from the literature has shown psychological differences between cisgender and transgender people regarding body image and body experiences [40, 41]. Moreover, a recent neuroimaging study has suggested that there could be different network connectivity in the gender dysphoria population as regard own body perception [42]. Thus, only cisgender participants’ inclusion allowed to focus on sexual orientations’ role in body weight dissatisfaction and perception with more robust results, advocating for future research about the relationship between cisgender, transgender, and queer population. Our first hypothesis is that heterosexual women and sexual minority men are more likely than sexual minority women and heterosexual men to have high levels of body dissatisfaction or to be influenced more by social norms regarding their bodies. Second, we want to test if there are differences in body image constructs between different sexual orientations by studying a sample of patients with and without ED symptomatology; our aims are to evaluate the clinical impact of sexual orientation in this specific health field. Third, due to the evidence that physical comparison, eating psychopathology, depression, and self-esteem may influence individuals’ body image, different correlation and mediation analyses were performed. However, due to the fewer evidence from literature, the analyses performed have to be considered exploratory.
Methods
The data were collected with an anonymous online survey (SurveyMonkey.com) completed by Italian speakers, 18 years old or older. The participants were recruited via online invitations through social media (i.e., Italian Facebook groups related to gender, physical activities, and cultural associations linked to civil rights; both open and close groups) and LGBTQ + group mailing lists from the area of the Veneto Region (Italy), through those responsible for managing personal data, without the involvement of researchers. The invitation consisted of a request to complete voluntarily and spontaneously in questionnaires on body image and body experiences to indicate that the questionnaires would be used for research purposes. The online survey was devised in such a way as to prevent multiple responses from the same IP addresses, but the IP addresses were hidden from investigators. The data were collected between February 2019 and October 2019. Each participant provided informed consent before receiving the survey. The research was in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and an ethical evaluation of the study was obtained by the local research ethics committee. The anonymous nature of the data acquired does not require the application of European legislation about privacy. The duration of the survey was about 30 min.
Sociodemographic characteristics
As suggested by the international literature, the gender of each participant was determined by asking them whether they identify as cisgender, transgender, or queer with forced choices about the gender they had at birth and about their current gender [43]. Sexual orientation was assessed asking about their sexual orientation with four possible answers: heterosexual, bisexual, gay/lesbian, asexual (“Which of the following best describes you? (1) heterosexual; (2) bisexual; (3) gay or lesbian; (4) asexual”) [44]. Lifetime diagnosis of any ED was determined with an item about a current or past diagnosis of any ED. Besides, demographic information was collected.
Self-esteem
The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) is a widely-used tool that measures positive and negative feelings about oneself [45]. It is a 10-item Likert scale with four-point scale answers on a continuum ranging from “strongly agree” to “strongly disagree.” The total score indicates how high or low a subject’s self-esteem is. The reliability of the scale in this study was high (Cronbach’s α = 0.815).
Physical comparisons at social events
The Physical Appearance Comparison Scale (PACS) [46] was used to measure the subjects’ tendency to compare their physical appearances with others in various social situations. The Italian version was already validated in a previous study [47]. The PACS is a 5-item Likert scale with five possible answers, ranging from “never” to “always.” The total score indicates the extent of an individual’s appearance-related social comparison tendencies. The reliability of the scale in this study was sufficient (Cronbach’s α = 0.752).
Psychological well-being
The Patient Health Questionnaire-9 (PHQ-9) is a screening tool for depression [48]. It is a 9-item Likert scale, and each item evaluates the presence of one of the DSM criteria for a depressive episode in the prior 2 weeks. There are four answer categories: 0 (“not at all”), 1 (“few days”), 2 (“more than half the days”), and 3 (“almost every day”). The total score is obtained by adding up the scores for each item, and higher scores indicate greater depression symptomatology, with a score between 8 and 11 denoting major depression. The reliability of the scale in this study was sufficient (Cronbach’s α = 0.788).
Eating concerns
The Eating Attitudes Test (EAT-26) is a well-established self-report questionnaire measuring eating symptoms and concerns characteristic of individuals with and without ED [49]. It includes 26 items that are rated on a 6-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (“neve”) to 6 (“always”), and it comprises three subscales (dieting, bulimia and food preoccupation, and oral control), which can be combined to yield a total EAT-26 score. The higher is the score, the higher is the number of ED symptoms present in an individual; a value of 20 points or higher indicates a probable ED diagnosis [50, 51]. The reliability of the scale in this study was sufficient (Cronbach’s α = 0.898).
Figure Rating Scales
Biometric Figure Rating Scales (FRSs) are reliable and efficient tools for assessing body image disturbances as long as the scale is representative of the weight range and centers roughly around the average weight of the target population [33]. Similarly to the procedure described in the literature [33], two different 12-point biometric FRS were obtained from a statistical model of average shape variation [52] (i.e., one scale for men and one for women). These FRSs cover a BMI spectrum from 14 to 36 kg/m2 with an increase of 2 kg/m2 and a center of 25 kg/m2. The participants were asked to choose which figure best represented their body size and represented an ideal body; these questions were asked to identify possible biased representations and body weight dissatisfaction. Body weight dissatisfaction was evaluated by subtracting the selected ideal figure score from the estimated current figure score. Perceptual distortion was assessed by subtracting the current figure score (i.e., the individual’s body’s score, which was closest to self-reported BMI) from the estimated current figure score [33]. Participants were also asked to choose a human model that represented the body shapes they consider to be the healthiest and most attractive. For each question, the participants could choose only one human model from the male FRS and one from the female FRS. This part of the study’s results were obtained by analyzing the body mass indices of the average human model selected. Previous studies have demonstrated the validity of using a visual rating scale to assess different constructs linked to body sizes and weight, such as attractiveness or cognitive representation [53].
Data analysis
The entire analysis was conducted with IBM SPSS Statistics 23.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). Differences between subgroups in terms of body dissatisfaction and the perceived pressure from social norms were explored using MANOVAs and Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests. To evaluate the clinical impact of sexual orientation on the prevalence and extent of EDs, Chi-square tests, and MANOVAs were used to compare participants with and without ED symptoms. Since the groups differed significantly in age, we conducted analyses of covariance by applying general linear models (GLMs) with age and BMI as covariates. Pearson correlations between variables were calculated to evaluate the contribution of other variables to the relationship between sexual orientation and body weight dissatisfaction.
Several mediation analyses were performed with the SPSS PROCESS vers. 3.4 macro-extension [54], applying Model 4. We used eating concerns, social physical comparison, depression, and self-esteem as possible mediators, to determine if these psychological constructs can mediate the relationship between sexual orientation (independent variable) and body weight dissatisfaction (dependent variable). This model was chosen in order to evaluate each construct independently as a mediator. According to the recent literature, sexual orientation was considered a continuous variable from heterosexual orientation to gay/lesbian, but it has been collapsed into a discrete variable for statistical analysis purposes [55,56,57]. This procedure estimates the total, direct, and indirect effects of multiple predictors on a dependent variable via the mediator while controlling for covariates. The PROCESS SPSS macro procedure was selected because of its better performance and statistical power than other mediation approaches (such as the Sobel test) [58]. The bootstrapping sampling distributions of the indirect effects were set to 5000, and they were produced by selecting a sample of cases from the complete data set and calculating the indirect effects in the resamples. Point estimates and confidence intervals were estimated for the indirect effects, and the bias level was set to 95%. The bootstrapping method enables to compare the relative strength of the single indirect effects involved in the multiple mediation model, estimating the statistical significance of the point estimate for the difference between mediators. For this specific study, the procedure showed the direct effect of the independent categorical variable (sexual orientation) on the dependent variable, and the indirect effect through variables. The alpha was set at p < 0.05 for all analyses.
Results
Sociodemographic and anthropometric characteristics
A total of 1389 subjects provided informed consent and participated in the survey, and 1112 participants completed the survey (80.1%). Furthermore, 1065 subjects (96%) identified themselves as cisgender and were thus included in the analysis. Data from participants who did not complete the survey were excluded from the study, and none of the participants provided gender-mixed answers. Only 12 women identified themselves as asexual and were excluded from the analysis as these individuals’ sample was too small. Moreover, no men identified as asexual. Only five men reported a lifetime diagnosis of an ED and were excluded from the analysis because of the small number of individuals. The demographic characteristics of the 375 men and 574 women without a lifetime diagnosis of any ED are presented in Table 1. The sexual orientation groups differed in terms of age, BMI, and education levels; however, BMI differences across sexual orientation groups were only present in the female samples.
No difference was found for the marriage status; single women: 72.3% of heterosexual, 74.3% of bisexual, 83.9% of lesbian (χ2 = 4.214, p = 0.378 for women); single men: 80.15 of heterosexual, 88.0% of bisexual, 82.14% of gay men (χ2 = 2.873, p = 0.579 for men).
Differences in body comparison, self-esteem, general health and eating concerns
The following abbreviations were used for sexual orientation subgroups: HEW (heterosexual women), BIW (bisexual women), HEM (heterosexual men), and BIM (bisexual men). Table 2 presents the means, standard deviations, and other statistics illustrating group differences. Gay men (5.3% of the sample) and BIW (7.3% of the sample) were more likely than their heterosexual peers to demonstrate eating concerns. The PACS scores indicate that gay men and HEM were less likely than their peers to engage in physical comparison, with significant differences between sexual orientations. Furthermore, gay men and BIW had lower self-esteem than HEM and HEW, and BIW had higher depression levels than HEW and gay men.
Different ANOVA analyses were performed both for the FRS PD (F (999, 5) = 3.259, p = 0.006, ηp2 = 0.016) and the FRS BWD (F (1027, 5) = 29.002, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.124), considering all the subgroups together. Table 2 shows the post hoc comparison between the women and men subgroups. On the other hand, as regards the FRS PD results, the post hoc analyses between the two different genders showed a significant difference only between HEW and gay men (p = 0.002). As regards the FRS BWD, HEW showed significantly higher dissatisfaction than HEM (p < 0.001) and BIM (p < 0.001, p < 0.001); BIW showed higher dissatisfaction than HEM (p < 0.001), BIM (p < 0.001), and gay men (p = 0.001); and lesbian women showed higher body weight dissatisfaction than BIM (p = 0.002).
Significant differences were also confirmed with the GLM analyses using age and BMI as covariates, which demonstrates that age and BMI do not affect the differences mentioned above.
The respondents’ visual perception, body weight dissatisfaction, and specific mentalized body representations are summarized in Table 2. Women did not exhibit differences in perceptual distortions and chose human models that are close to their actual BMIs; gay men, however, significantly overestimated their body sizes. Body weight dissatisfaction was higher in BIW and gay men than their peers.
As regards the mentalized ideal body weight, BIW were more likely than HEW to find male and female bodies with higher BMIs more attractive, and BIM were more likely than HEM to find these bodies more attractive. Figure 1 provides a graphic representation of the participants’ views on the attractiveness of body shapes. No differences were found in the GLM analyses with age and BMI as covariates. Comparisons between sexual orientation subgroups without gender separation: gay men are the subgroup with the highest perceptual distortion, which was calculated based on individuals' ability to choose their corresponding human model (p = 0.006). Regarding body weight dissatisfaction, BIW had the highest average among all subgroups, but it is not different from the scores for lesbians and gay men.
Attractive body shape selection. This figure is the graphic representation of data from Table 2. For each sexual orientation group, the violin plot showed the distribution of the FRS responses for attractive and healthy female and male bodies. The light blue area showed a normal BMI (> 18.5 kg/m2 and < 25.0 kg/m2). Significant differences are showed with continuous lines between subgroups. From the figure, it is possible to appreciate how most of the female bodies' responses are in the underweight area, which does not happen in males. Moreover, the different distributions of the responses could be appreciated thanks to the graphs’ shapes, even without different means. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.001
Different correlation analyses have been performed between variables, showing different relationships between dependent constructs in different sexual orientations. See Table 3 for the data.
Comparisons based on lifetime diagnosis of EDs
Eighty-one women reported a lifetime diagnosis of an ED and scored higher than the group without an ED on all the psychological evaluation scales. There were no significant differences between the demographic profile of this group and that of other women. Still, significant differences between the psychopathological profiles were found; women with an ED diagnosis in all the subgroups had significantly higher scores for PHQ-9, EAT26, and PACS and lower self-esteem scores. Regarding perceptual distortion, women with an ED diagnosis did not show any significant difference in the subgroup analysis, but they had a different profile than women without an ED. Indeed, in the HEW samples, women with an ED had a lower own-body misperception (i.e., they were more accurate in the selection of their human model). However, sexual minority women with an ED were more likely to exhibit perceptual distortions than sexual minority women without an ED. All the subgroups with an ED were more dissatisfied than women without an ED. In terms of the selection of healthy bodies, there were few differences between women with and without an ED; only the attractive female body had a significantly higher BMI in HEW and BIW without ED. See Table 4 for more details.
Mediation analyses
The first hypothesis tested with mediation analysis was that psychological constructs could mediate the relationship between body weight dissatisfaction and sexual orientation. Looking at women without an ED, the effect of sexual orientation on body weight dissatisfaction was mediated only via the physical appearance comparison (PACS, β = − 0.150, SE = 0.044, 95% CI [− 0.244, − 0.072]). Looking at men, the effect of sexual orientation on body weight dissatisfaction was significantly mediated only via eating concerns (EAT26, β = 0.102, SE = 0.044, 95% CI [0.028, 0.196]). Looking at women with an ED, no mediation effects were found.
The second hypothesis was about the distorted perception of the body. Women without ED as well as men, showed that the effect of sexual orientation on perceptual body distortion was mediated physical comparison (PACS, women: β = − 0.092, SE = 0.030, 95% CI [− 0.158, − 0.039]; men: β = 0.196, SE = 0.044, 95% CI [0.116, 0.290]). No significant mediation effect was found in the ED subgroup. See Fig. 2 for details.
Mediation analysis model. This figure shows the mediation model used. The independent variable used was the sexual orientation, which is a categorical variable and produces mediation results as comparisons between subgroups. The mediators were the psychological data collected: PACS, RSES, PHQ9, and EAT26. Only significant mediation analyses are visualized, *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01
Discussion
The purpose of this study is to provide a comprehensive characterization of body image among cisgender individuals of different sexual orientations. Body image is a crucial aspect of psychological well-being, but the existing literature does not provide a clear picture of the relationship between body image, sexual orientation, and gender. Our data confirmed the literature finding that body image concerns were different across genders and that these concerns have a significant relationship with sexual orientation [59]. Specifically, our data demonstrated that sexual orientation plays a significant role in body weight dissatisfaction in sexual minority groups (especially gay men and BIW) and that different socio-cultural and psychological factors must be considered to understand how people perceive their bodies.
Our primary research goal was to explore the role of gender and sexual orientation in body image. Our first hypothesis was that gay men and HEW were more likely than individuals with other sexual orientations to have body concerns. Our results only partially confirmed our hypothesis, but they are in line with data from the literature that indicate that BIW and gay men are more likely to have body concerns than their heterosexual peers [24, 60, 61]. Indeed, data from recent studies suggest that BIW feel pressured to integrate physical norms from both the heterosexual and lesbian cultures and are significantly vulnerable to body dissatisfaction, even if results are not unique [8, 25]. According to our results, sexual minority men are more prone than the other cisgender groups to internalize social pressure about body evaluation. Depression was significantly correlated with body weight dissatisfaction and other psychological factors, confirming that the link between mental wellness and body image is crucial [39].
This study used a visual approach to identify body weight dissatisfaction and ideal body sizes. As for visual body perception distortions, we found no significant differences between BIW, BIM, HEW, and HEM. Gay men were the only group that overestimated their body sizes; the finding is in accordance with other studies that hypothesized a significant impact of socio-cultural standards in the gay culture with a misperception of their real body shapes [62, 63]. Gay men often exhibit a significant drive to become muscular and fear becoming fat, and this could be the foundation of the misperception of their bodies [64]. This misperception could lead them to overestimate their body sizes, which could also lead them to be more likely to have body dissatisfaction and eating concerns. This result is in accordance with the literature suggesting that there is distorted body size cognition among gay men, who exhibit a strong drive to alter their bodies, even when their bodies reflect their ideal body sizes [63]. The literature has demonstrated that among gay men, there is a disconnect between their ideal and actual BMI; these men cognitively misrepresent their body sizes and overestimate their shapes, which leads to unhealthy eating behaviors to achieve a muscular yet thin body [63, 65].
Similarly, according to our results, women with an ED are significantly more likely than other women to overestimate their body size and weight, as reported in the literature [19]. Indeed, this is a core symptom of EDs, and it is also a well-established outcome predictor [66,67,68,69]. This similarity between gay men and women with an ED could be interpreted as a result of the struggle of gay men with their body images and how they are exposed to external and internal pressure; these forms of pressure could lead to unhealthy eating behaviors even in the absence of an ED. Moreover, even BIW showed high levels of body weight dissatisfaction. Our findings are in accordance with the literature on the possible conflict between the internalized Western body standards during childhood and the personal acceptance process linked to being part of a non-heterosexual women group [70]. This conflict between two different ideal body shapes could be at the root of the higher vulnerability of BIW to body-related psychopathology [24].
The attractiveness of the human model was significantly different across subgroups, and BIW and BIM chose a figure with a higher BMI than other subgroups. Women with EDs chose thinner human models than those without EDs, and this finding supports the presence of a possible internalized cultural thinness standard hypothesis as a maintaining factor in EDs [13]. In addition, for the ideal male sizes, attractiveness was the only factor that exhibited significant differences. The male sexual minorities were more likely than HEM to prefer higher BMI bodies, confirming the social drive to achieve muscularity/higher weight [71]. Furthermore, BIW and HEW chose heavier models than lesbian women, demonstrating that sexual attractiveness is a significant factor that has to be taken seriously by both women and men.
The hypothesis motivating our second research goal concerns the role of sexual orientation in the body image of people with an ED. This is a neglected topic in the literature, particularly its relevance for women. Our data are consistent with the idea that there is a relationship between sexual orientation and body dissatisfaction in non-clinical women, but sexual orientation plays no specific role in body image when a woman has an ED [8, 72, 73]. Indeed, no significant differences in perceptual distortion and body weight dissatisfaction were found between subgroups. Women with an ED had higher psychopathology levels than non-clinical subgroups, which is in line with recent reviews of the literature [24, 74]. However, we observed no differences in body concerns between different sexual orientations. A potential explanation could be that our data do not take masculinity/femininity into consideration, which is influences body dissatisfaction in sexual minority women [75], and sexuality, which studies have shown to be highly correlated with body dissatisfaction [76].
Our third hypothesis was about the relationship between physical comparison, self-esteem, eating concerns, and body weight dissatisfaction across the sexual orientation spectrum. Our observations show that body weight dissatisfaction was most common among BIW and gay men. As suggested by previous studies, the cultural background could significantly impact body weight dissatisfaction [77], but the instrument used could also make a difference. Indeed, a study using silhouettes found the persistence of a discrepancy between the actual silhouette and the one chosen by the participants [78]. In the subgroup correlations analysis, this relationship was not valid for BIW and gay men; for these groups, there was no relationship between these factors. These results are in line with previous research that showed how gay men are exposed to body weight dissatisfaction, regardless of age. The results also demonstrate that BIW have to be considered as a vulnerable group; this group has been understudied [24, 25, 79, 80]. The mediation analyses demonstrated how important the comparison of physical appearance is. The physical appearance comparison directly and indirectly affects body weight dissatisfaction in women and a mediator role for the body misperception across all the cisgender population. Moreover, these data confirmed how meaningful physical comparison is for cognitive body bias and body dissatisfaction [81,82,83]. Thus, physical comparisons should be considered as an essential target for the reduction of body misperception and the reduction of body weight dissatisfaction in women. Indeed, attentional bias studies have demonstrated that there may be a judgment and memory bias about body shapes and satisfaction [20], with a possible role of cognitive biases in body weight dissatisfaction. More studies are needed to identify effective methods that can change the way physical comparison influences body dissatisfaction, or how cognitive bias can be improved [84,85,86]. For example, the “exposure with response prevention” is a cognitive-behavioral technique that has already demonstrated preliminary efficacy in modifying body evaluation with targeting interventions on ritualistic behaviors and thoughts due to physical comparison. However, the results are still preliminary [87, 88]. In the male population, the mediation analysis showed that eating concerns has a mediator role between sexual orientation and bodyweight dissatisfaction, corroborating the role of eating concerns in the evaluation of own bodyweight in gay men and showing a possible target for interventions focused on the improvement of their weight dissatisfaction that could bring to unhealthy behaviors [65].
Limitations
This study has some limitations that must be considered. First, the data were collected from a large sample and with a variety of methods to assess body image. Quantitative analyses have been applied with a possible reduction of salient aspects that could be evaluated. Unfortunately, we could not recruit enough sexual minority men with an ED to allow for sound statistical analyses. Second, this study was not able to compare all sexual orientations in men and women; doing so could enable us to infer relationships among different subgroups. Third, while online surveys have demonstrated sufficient reliability [89], they rely on collecting self-reported responses, which should be viewed with caution. Fourth, the decision to include only cisgender responders has reduced the relationship between sexual orientation and their own body. Still, this decision helps simplify the analysis of the results regarding the role of sexual orientations. Finally, the exclusive inclusion of participants who have a computer and internet access should be considered a limitation. Therefore, this study’s results should be considered exploratory, but they advocated for more research in the relationship between sexual orientation, body weight dissatisfaction, and ED psychopathology.
Conclusion
Our data support the hypothesis that gender (female/male) and sexual orientations have an influence on body weight dissatisfaction and body perception/representation in the cisgender population. This study found significant relationships between the sexual orientations of cisgender individuals and body weight dissatisfaction and body size perception. Bisexual women and gay men seem to be the most vulnerable to body weight dissatisfaction, which is a significant risk factor for psychiatric disorders with severe impacts on patients’ lives. However, the data collected should be increase with more studies with integrated methodology (i.e., with qualitative data) that allowed a deeper inside into the effect of body weight dissatisfaction or body perception. For this reason, sexual orientation should be systematically taken into consideration in the assessment and treatment of EDs or other body image disturbances (e.g., dysmorphophobic disorder).
What is already known on this subject?
The relationship between sexual orientation and body weight dissatisfaction and body representation is a neglected topic, especially in women and sexual minorities. It has a role in the development and in the maintenance of body image disturbance, but there is a lack of research about the presence of any moderator constructs that could contribute to its modification.
What does this study add?
A large sample of cisgender women and men was included in the study, showing how that bisexual women and gay men are exposed to higher body weight dissatisfaction and distorted body shapes perception. Attractiveness analysis highlighted the relevance of internalized normative pressure for thinner female bodies in all subgroups, even though bisexual women and men prefer healthy weight bodies. Physical appearance comparison is a crucial psychological element in the assessment of body comparison and evaluation, and this should be taken into consideration in mental health prevention programs.
Data availability
The datasets generated and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
References
Cash TF (2012) Cognitive-behavioral perspectives on body image. Encyclopedia of body image and human appearance. Elsevier Inc., Amsterdam, pp 334–342
Cooley E, Toray T (2001) Body image and personality predictors of eating disorder symptoms during the college years. Int J Eat Disord 30:28–36
Stice E, Hayward C, Cameron RP et al (2000) Body-image and eating disturbances predict onset of depression among female adolescents: a longitudinal study. J Abnorm Psychol 109:438–444
Cash TF (2004) Body image: past, present, and future. Body Image 1:1–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1740-1445(03)00011-1
Grossbard JR, Lee CM, Neighbors C, Larimer ME (2009) Body image concerns and contingent self-esteem in male and female college students. Sex Roles 60:198–207. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9535-y
McCabe MP, Ricciardelli LA (2004) Body image dissatisfaction among males across the lifespan: a review of past literature. J Psychosom Res 56:675–685
Lampis J, Cataudella S, Busonera A et al (2019) The moderating effect of gender role on the relationships between gender and attitudes about body and eating in a sample of Italian adolescents. Eat Weight Disord 24:3–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-017-0372-2
Henn AT, Taube CO, Vocks S, Hartmann AS (2019) Body image as well as eating disorder and body dysmorphic disorder symptoms in heterosexual, homosexual, and bisexual women. Front Psychiatry 10:1–11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00531
Basabas MC, Greaves L, Barlow FK, Sibley CG (2019) Sexual orientation moderates the effect of gender on body satisfaction: results from a national probability sample. J Sex Res 56:1–10. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2019.1667947
Kling J, Kwakkenbos L, Diedrichs PC et al (2019) Systematic review of body image measures. Body Image 30:170–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.06.006
Zullig KJ, Matthews-Ewald MR, Valois RF (2019) Relationship between disordered eating and self-identified sexual minority youth in a sample of public high school adolescents. Eat Weight Disord 24:565–573. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-017-0389-6
Dotan A, Bachner-Melman R, Dahlenburg SC (2019) Sexual orientation and disordered eating in women: a meta-analysis. Eat Weight Disord. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-019-00824-3
Stice E, Shaw HE (2002) Role of body dissatisfaction in the onset and maintenance of eating pathology: a synthesis of research findings. J Psychosom Res 53:985–993. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00488-9
Artoni P, Chierici ML, Arnone F et al (2020) Body perception treatment, a possible way to treat body image disturbance in eating disorders: a case–control efficacy study. Eat Weight Disord. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-020-00875-x
Grogan S (2016) Body image: understanding body dissatisfaction in men, women and children. Taylor & Francis, Philadelphia
Glashouwer KA, Bennik EC, de Jong PJ, Spruyt A (2018) Implicit measures of actual versus ideal body image: relations with self-reported body dissatisfaction and dieting behaviors. Cognit Ther Res 42:622–635. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-018-9917-6
Moussally JM, Grynberg D, Goffinet S et al (2017) Novel assessment of own and ideal body perception among women: validation of the computer-generated figure rating scale. Cognit Ther Res 41:632–644. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-016-9827-4
D’Amour S, Harris LR (2019) The representation of body size: variations with viewpoint and sex. Front Psychol 10:1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02805
Mölbert SC, Thaler A, Mohler BJ et al (2018) Assessing body image in anorexia nervosa using biometric self-avatars in virtual reality : attitudinal components rather than visual body size estimation are distorted. Psychol Med 48:642–653. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717002008
Rodgers RF, DuBois RH (2016) Cognitive biases to appearance-related stimuli in body dissatisfaction: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Rev 46:1–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.04.006
Feusner J, Deshpande R, Strober M (2017) A translational neuroscience approach to body image disturbance and its remediation in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 50:1014–1017. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22742
Moreno-Domínguez S, Raposo T, Elipe P (2019) Body image and sexual satisfaction: differences among heterosexual, bisexual and lesbian women. Front Psychol 10:1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00903
Graham R, Berkowitz B, Blum R et al (2011) The health of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender people: building a foundation for better understanding. Wash DC Inst Med 10:13128
Meneguzzo P, Collantoni E, Gallicchio D et al (2018) Eating disorders symptoms in sexual minority women: a systematic review. Eur Eat Disord Rev 26:275–292. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2601
Hazzard VM, Schaefer LM, Schaumberg K et al (2019) Testing the tripartite influence model among heterosexual, bisexual, and lesbian women. Body Image 30:145–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.07.001
Gigi I, Bachner-Melman R, Lev-Ari L (2016) The association between sexual orientation, susceptibility to social messages and disordered eating in men. Appetite 99:25–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.12.027
Matera C, Nerini A, Stefanile C (2019) Sexual orientation, peer influence, body dissatisfaction, and eudaimonic well-being in italian men. Front Psychol 10:1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01843
Filice E, Raffoul A, Meyer SB, Neiterman E (2020) The impact of social media on body image perceptions and bodily practices among gay, bisexual, and other men who have sex with men: a critical review of the literature and extension of theory. Sex Roles 82:387–410. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01063-7
Frederick DA, Essayli JH (2016) Male body image: the roles of sexual orientation and body mass index across five national U.S. studies. Psychol Men Masc 17:336–351. https://doi.org/10.1037/men0000031
Watson LB, Grotewiel M, Farrell M et al (2015) Experiences of sexual objectification, minority stress, and disordered eating among sexual minority women. Psychol Women Q 39:458–470. https://doi.org/10.1177/0361684315575024
Heimerdinger-Edwards SR, Vogel DL, Hammer JH (2011) Extending sexual objectification theory and research to minority populations, couples, and men. Couns Psychol 39:140–152. https://doi.org/10.1177/0011000010383894
Balsam KF, Beauchaine TP, Mickey RM, Rothblum ED (2005) Mental health of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and heterosexual siblings: effects of gender, sexual orientation, and family. J Abnorm Psychol 114:471–476. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.114.3.471
Claire S, Klein L, Thaler A et al (2017) Depictive and metric body size estimation in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev 57:21–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2017.08.005
Thompson JK, Burke NL, Krawczyk R (2012) Measurement of body image in adolescence and adulthood. Elsevier Inc, Amsterdam
Burke SM, Majid DSA, Manzouri AH et al (2019) Sex differences in own and other body perception. Hum Brain Mapp 40:474–488. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24388
Urgesi C, Fornasari L, Perini L et al (2004) Visual body perception in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 45:207–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.01.003
Mölbert SC, Thaler A, Streuber S et al (2017) Investigating body image disturbance in anorexia nervosa using novel biometric figure rating scales: a pilot study. Eur Eat Disord Rev 25:607–612. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2559
Nayir T, Uskun E, Yürekli MV et al (2016) Does body image affect quality of life? A population based study. PLoS ONE 11:1–13. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0163290
Richard A, Rohrmann S, Lohse T, Eichholzer M (2016) Is body weight dissatisfaction a predictor of depression independent of body mass index, sex and age? Results of a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health 16:1–8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3497-8
Strübel J, Sabik N, Tylka T (2020) Body image and depressive symptoms among transgender and cisgender adults: examining a model integrating the tripartite influence model and objectification theory. Body Image 35:53–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2020.08.004
McClain Z, Peebles R (2016) Body image and eating disorders among lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender youth. Pediatr Clin N Am 63:1079–1090. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pcl.2016.07.008
Feusner JD, Lidström A, Moody TD et al (2017) Intrinsic network connectivity and own body perception in gender dysphoria. Brain Imaging Behav 11:964–976. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-016-9578-6
Salk RH, Thoma BC, Choukas-Bradley S (2020) The gender minority youth study: overview of methods and social media recruitment of a nationwide sample of U.S. cisgender and transgender adolescents. Arch Sex Behav 49:2601–2610. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-020-01695-x
Sell RL (2007) Defining and measuring sexual orientation for research. Health Sex Minor. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-31334-4_14
Rosenberg M (1965) Rosenberg self-esteem scale (RSE). Acceptance and commitment therapy. Meas Package 61(52):18
Fisher E, Dunn M, Thompson JK (2002) Social comparison and body image: an investigation of body comparison processes using multidimensional scaling. J Soc Clin Psychol 21:566–579. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.21.5.566.22618
Meneguzzo P, Collantoni E, Bonello E et al (2020) The predictive value of the early maladaptive schemas in social situations in anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2724
Kroenke K, Spitzer RL (2002) The PHQ-9: a new depression diagnostic and severity measure. Psychiatr Ann 32:509–515
Garfinkel PE, Newman A (2001) The eating attitudes test: twenty-five years later. Eat Weight Disord 6:1–21. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf03339747
Dotti A, Lazzari R (1998) Validation and reliability of the Italian EAT-26. Eat Weight Disord 3:188–194. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03340009
Mintz LB, O’Halloran MS (2000) The eating attitudes test: validation with DSM-IV eating disorder criteria. J Pers Assess 74:489–503. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7403_11
Anguelov D, Srinivasan P, Koller D et al (2005) SCAPE: shape completion and animation of people dragomir. ACM SIGGRAPH. https://doi.org/10.2307/375706
Fingeret MC, Gleaves DH, Pearson CA (2004) On the methology of body image assessment: the use of figural rating scales to evaluate body dissatisfaction and the ideal body standards of women. Body Image 1:207–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.01.003
Hayes AF (2017) Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: a regression-based approach. Guilford Publications
Savin-Williams RC (2014) An exploratory study of the categorical versus spectrum nature of sexual orientation. J Sex Res 51:446–453. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2013.871691
Bailey JM, Vasey PL, Diamond LM et al (2016) Sexual orientation, controversy, and science. Psychol Sci Public Interest 17:45–101. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616637616
Savin-Williams RC (2016) Sexual orientation: categories or continuum? Commentary on Bailey et al. (2016). Psychol Sci Public Interest 17:37–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616637618
Williams J, MacKinnon DP (2008) Resampling and distribution of the product methods for testing indirect effects in complex models. Struct Equ Model 15:23–51. https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701758166
Smolak L, Stein JA (2006) The relationship of drive for muscularity to socio-cultural factors, self-esteem, physical attributes gender role, and social comparison in middle school boys. Body Image 3:121–129. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2006.03.002
Cuzzolaro M, Fassino S (2018) Body image, eating, and weight. Springer, New York. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-90817-5
Dakanalis A, Di Mattei VE, Bagliacca EP et al (2012) Disordered eating behaviors among Italian men: objectifying media and sexual orientation differences. Eat Disord 20:356–367. https://doi.org/10.1080/10640266.2012.715514
Dunlop K, Woodside B, Olmsted M et al (2016) Reductions in cortico-striatal hyperconnectivity accompany successful treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder with dorsomedial prefrontal rTMS. Neuropsychopharmacology 41:1395–1403. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2015.292
Kaminski PL, Chapman BP, Haynes SD, Own L (2005) Body image, eating behaviors, and attitudes toward exercise among gay and straight men. Eat Behav 6:179–187. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.11.003
Conner M, Grogan S, Johnson C, Grogan S (2004) Body image and eating behaviours. J Health Psychol 9:505–515. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105304044034
Hadland SE, Austin SB, Goodenow CS, Calzo JP (2014) Weight misperception and unhealthy weight control behaviors among sexual minorities in the general adolescent population. J Adolesc Health 54:296–303. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.08.021
Danielsen M, Øyvind R (2012) Changes in body image during inpatient treatment for eating disorders predict outcome. Eat Disord 20:261–275. https://doi.org/10.1080/10640266.2012.689205
Farrell C, Lee M, Shafran R (2005) Assessment of body size estimation: a review. Eur Eat Disord Rev 13:75–88. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.622
Solmi M, Collantoni E, Meneguzzo P et al (2018) Network analysis of specific psychopathology and psychiatric symptoms in patients with eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 51:680–692. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22884
Solmi M, Collantoni E, Meneguzzo P et al (2019) Network analysis of specific psychopathology and psychiatric symptoms in patients with anorexia nervosa. Eur Eat Disord Rev 27:24–33. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2633
Kelly L (2007) Lesbian body image perceptions: the context of body silence. Qual Health Res 17:873–883. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049732307306172
Brennan DJ, Craig SL, Thompson DEA (2012) Factors associated with a drive for muscularity among gay and bisexual men. Cult Health Sex 14:1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/13691058.2011.619578
Shaw H, Rohde P, Desjardins CD, Stice E (2020) Sexual orientation correlates with baseline characteristics but shows no moderating effects of dissonance-based eating disorder prevention programs for women. Body Image 32:94–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bodyim.2019.11.006
Mason TB, Lewis RJ, Heron KE (2018) Disordered eating and body image concerns among sexual minority women: a systematic review and testable model. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers 5:397–422. https://doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000293
Calzo JP, Blashill AJ, Brown TA, Argenal RL (2017) Eating disorders and disordered weight and shape control behaviors in sexual minority populations. Curr Psychiatry Rep. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-017-0801-y
Henrichs-Beck CL, Szymanski DM (2017) Gender expression, body–gender identity incongruence, thin ideal internalization, and lesbian body dissatisfaction. Psychol Sex Orientat Gend Divers 4:23–33. https://doi.org/10.1037/sgd0000214
Pinheiro AP, Raney TJ, Thornton LM et al (2010) Sexual functioning in women with eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 43:123–129. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.20671
Bibiloni MDM, Coll JL, Pich J et al (2017) Body image satisfaction and weight concerns among a Mediterranean adult population. BMC Public Health 17:1–11. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-016-3919-7
Runfola CD, Von HA, Trace SE et al (2013) Body dissatisfaction in women across the lifespan: results of the UNC-SELF and gender and body image (GABI) studies. Eur Eat Disord Rev 21:52–59. https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.2201.Body
Peplau LA, Frederick DA, Yee C et al (2009) Body image satisfaction in heterosexual, gay, and lesbian adults. Arch Sex Behav 38:713–725. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-008-9378-1
Robbins AR, Reissing ED (2018) Appearance dissatisfaction, body appreciation, and sexual health in women across adulthood. Arch Sex Behav 47:703–714. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-017-0982-9
Buscemi S, Marventano S, Castellano S et al (2018) Role of anthropometric factors, self-perception, and diet on weight misperception among young adolescents: a cross-sectional study. Eat Weight Disord 23:107–115. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-016-0306-4
Challinor KL, Mond J, Stephen ID et al (2017) Body size and shape misperception and visual adaptation: an overview of an emerging research paradigm. J Int Med Res 45:2001–2008. https://doi.org/10.1177/0300060517726440
Brooks KR, Mond J, Mitchison D et al (2020) Looking at the figures: visual adaptation as a mechanism for body-size and -shape misperception. Perspect Psychol Sci 15:133–149. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619869331
Ridolfi DR, Myers TA, Crowther JH, Ciesla JA (2011) Do appearance focused cognitive distortions moderate the relationship between social comparisons to peers and media images and body image disturbance? Sex Roles 65:491–505. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-9961-0
Want SC, Vickers K, Amos J (2009) The influence of television programs on appearance satisfaction: making and mitigating social comparisons to friends. Sex Roles 60:642–655. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9563-7
Rothwell CJ, Desmond D (2018) The effect of viewing fitness imagery on body dissatisfaction: sex and physical activity differences. Psychol Health Med 23:980–986. https://doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2018.1442009
Mac Neil BA, Leung P, Montemarano V (2018) Exposure with response prevention (ERP) for body dissatisfaction in a group therapy format: an exploratory study. Eat Weight Disord 23:225–232. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-016-0340-2
Alleva JM, Sheeran P, Webb TL et al (2015) A meta-analytic review of stand-alone interventions to improve body image. PLoS ONE 10:1–32. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0139177
Ramo DE, Hall SM, Prochaska JJ (2011) Reliability and validity of self-reported smoking in an anonymous online survey with young adults. Health Psychol 30:693–701. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023443
Funding
Open access funding provided by Università degli Studi di Padova within the CRUI-CARE Agreement. PM was supported by Fondazione Emanuela, Luigi e Maria Dalla Vecchia Onlus.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
PM: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, software, writing—original draft; EC: data curation, methodology, writing—original draft; EB: data curation, investigation, writing—original draft; MV: formal analysis, investigation, writing—original draft; SCB: conceptualization, writing—review and editing; ET: conceptualization, resources, supervision, validation, writing—review and editing; AF: conceptualization, methodology, resources, supervision, validation, writing—review and editing.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.
Ethical approval
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee, the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments, and comparable ethical standards. This is an observational study, and the Research Ethics Committee of the Padua University Hospital has confirmed that no ethical approval is required.
Informed consent
Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.
Additional information
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Males and Eating and Weight Disorders.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Meneguzzo, P., Collantoni, E., Bonello, E. et al. The role of sexual orientation in the relationships between body perception, body weight dissatisfaction, physical comparison, and eating psychopathology in the cisgender population. Eat Weight Disord 26, 1985–2000 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-020-01047-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40519-020-01047-7