FormalPara Key Points

Physical dependence to antidepressants may occur in some patients, caused by adaptation of the brain to long-term use of the medication. As pharmacologically defined, this physical dependence is a distinct phenomenon from addiction, and is manifested by a drug withdrawal syndrome.

Longer duration of treatment with antidepressants may increase the incidence and severity, and perhaps duration, of antidepressant withdrawal.

Serotonin and noradrenaline reuptake inhibitors and paroxetine are associated with a higher risk of withdrawal effects compared with other antidepressants.

Based on characteristics of antidepressant use, such as type, duration, dose and past experience, we have developed a preliminary tool to aid estimation of the risk of withdrawal effects on stopping. Further research is required to validate this tool.

1 Introduction

In 2019/2020, one in six adults in England were given at least one prescription for an antidepressant, representing 7.8 million people [1], with approximately half of those on antidepressants estimated to be taking them for more than 2 years [2], and at least 930,000 people taking them for at least 3 years [3].

Although there is uncertainty about the precise number, perhaps up to one-half of those taking antidepressants will experience withdrawal symptoms when they stop them, with some of those effects being severe and long lasting [4,5,6,7]. Severe withdrawal effects can lead to misdiagnosis of other medical conditions or a misdiagnosis of relapse [8, 9], presentations to the emergency department [10] and suicide attempts [11]. Some people will find withdrawal effects so aversive that they will recommence their antidepressant, leading to long-term unwarranted use and unnecessary exposure to adverse effects [10, 12]. The estimated cost of antidepressant withdrawal syndrome has not yet been evaluated, but costs to the health system and social costs may be substantial [8, 13].

There has been widespread debate on how commonly withdrawal symptoms from antidepressants occur, as well as their severity and duration [4, 5, 14]. It has also been suggested that various aspects of antidepressant use are likely to affect the risk of withdrawal, including dosage, duration of use and characteristics of the antidepressant [10, 15, 16]. In this paper, we briefly review the neurobiological causes of withdrawal symptoms before examining what is known about the determinants of antidepressant withdrawal from the existing literature on the subject. From this review, we develop a preliminary risk calculator to estimate the risk of withdrawal symptoms in a given patient.

1.1 Neurobiology of Physical Dependence and Withdrawal Symptoms from Antidepressants

1.1.1 Physiological Dependence Distinguished from Addiction

The term ‘dependence’ has recently come to be used interchangeably with ‘addiction’ (to mean uncontrolled drug-seeking behaviour), for example in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fifth Edition) [17]. This choice was made in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Third Edition) because the term ‘addiction’ was thought to be pejorative while the word ‘dependence’ was thought more neutral [17]. However, the original usage of the word ‘dependence’ referred to “physiological adaptation that occurs when medications acting on the central nervous system are ingested with rebound when the medication is abruptly discontinued” [17]. This usage is referred to by many expert groups: according to a consensus statement from the American Academy of Pain Medicine, the American Pain Society and the American Society of Addiction Medicine, “Physical dependence is a state of adaptation that is manifested by a drug class specific withdrawal syndrome that can be produced by abrupt cessation, rapid dose reduction, decreasing blood level of the drug, and/or administration of an antagonist” [18]. Similarly, the National Institute on Drug Abuse states “Dependence means that when a person stops using a drug, their body goes through ‘withdrawal’: a group of physical and mental symptoms that can range from mild (if the drug is caffeine) to life threatening (such as alcohol or opioids, including heroin and prescription pain relievers). Many people who take a prescription medicine every day over a long period of time can become dependent; when they go off the drug, they need to do it gradually, to avoid withdrawal discomfort. But people who are dependent on a drug or medicine aren’t necessarily addicted” [19]. Indeed, Goodman and Gilman’s textbook of pharmacology points out “The appearance of a withdrawal syndrome when administration of the drug is terminated is the only actual evidence of physical dependence” [20].

Addiction “is characterized by behaviours that include one or more of the following: impaired control over drug use, compulsive use, continued use despite harm, and craving” [18]. Therefore, “For drugs not associated with abuse potential, an individual may still develop dependence; but again, this would not be classified as an addiction” [21]. Additionally, according to the National Institute on Drug Abuse, “a person can be dependent on a drug, or have a high tolerance to it, without being addicted to it” (emphasis in original) [19]. Physical dependence is therefore an important concept to retain as without this term it becomes difficult to communicate how a non-addictive substance like caffeine can cause withdrawal effects, equally applicable to antidepressants (for which there is no evidence of addiction). The process of physical dependence also allows us to understand why certain characteristics of medication (half-life, receptor targets, dose, duration of use) might predict an increased risk of withdrawal as all these characteristics may affect the degree of adaptation to the drugs.

1.1.2 Antidepressants and Physiological Dependence

All major classes of antidepressants [selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), serotonin and norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, monoamine oxidase inhibitors, tricyclic antidepressants, noradrenaline and specific serotonergic antidepressants] can be associated with withdrawal symptoms on cessation or a dose reduction in a substantial proportion of patients, likely as a result of physical dependence (a normal neurobiological response to drugs that act on the central nervous system) in these patients [3, 10, 17, 21,22,23]. Physical dependence arises because the body and brain undergo adaptations to the presence of a drug, countering its effect in order to maintain homeostasis [17, 24, 25]. The only evidence of a state of physical dependence is the appearance of withdrawal symptoms on reducing or stopping the drug [20]. It is also clear that vast majority of antidepressants—with the exception of tranylcypromine and amineptine—do not cause addiction, as they do not induce compulsion, craving and other symptoms of addiction [4, 26].

During ongoing administration of antidepressants, as for other drugs acting on the central nervous system, neuroadaptation establishes a new homeostatic equilibrium, in which the system accommodates to alterations produced by the drug. When the medication is reduced or stopped, the homeostasis is perturbed, resulting in withdrawal symptoms [24, 25, 27]. Adaptations to the presence of the drug predict withdrawal effects because these adaptations do not resolve instantaneously upon stopping the drugs but will persist for some period: the ‘mis-match’ between the level of drug action to which the body has been adapted to and the lesser amount of drug action it receives (upon dose reduction or cessation) gives rise to withdrawal effects [27]. As a Professor of Pharmacology, Prof. Reidenberg puts it “For drugs given chronically, one can look for laboratory evidence of adaptation to the drug. If such adaptation occurs it is likely that, after discontinuation, the body will eliminate the drug more rapidly than the adaptation will subside. Any laboratory evidence of adaptation … should be interpreted as presenting a potential discontinuation syndrome” [27]. Further, Reidenberg said that “drug discontinuation effects are part of the pharmacology of a drug” when the body eliminates a drug faster than adaptations to the presence of the drug can subside (Fig. 1) [27].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Adapted, with permission, from [77]

Conceptual model of the neurobiology of antidepressant withdrawal. In this diagram, the homeostatic ‘set-point’ is shown in black and antidepressant (AD) drug concentrations are shown in blue dotted lines [77]. (1) The system is at baseline. At the blue arrow, an antidepressant is administered; drug plasma concentrations increase. Physiological adaptations of the system to the presence of the drug begin (which may be the period for which ‘start-up side effects’ are most pronounced). (2) At the plateau, drug plasma concentrations (and target receptor activation) have reached a steady state with a new homeostatic set-point of the system established (‘start-up side effects’ may reduce). (3) The antidepressant is abruptly ceased and plasma drug concentrations drop to zero (exponentially, according to the elimination half-life of the drug). This difference between the homeostatic set point (the ‘expectations’ of the system) and the concentration of drug in the system (dotted blue line) is experienced as withdrawal symptoms. The duration of withdrawal symptoms is largely determined by the time required for adaptations to the drug to resolve. Hence, withdrawal symptoms may worsen or peak even long after the drug has been eliminated from the system. The shaded area under the curve, representing the difference between the homeostatic set-point and the concentration of the drug, indicates the degree of risk of withdrawal symptoms: the larger the area the greater the risk. The greater the departure of drug concentration from the homeostatic set-point, the greater the risk.

The exact nature of neurobiological adaptation to antidepressants has received relatively little study, but it is thought to involve down-regulation of serotonergic receptors in response to higher levels of synaptic serotonin arising as a consequence of serotonin transporter (SERT) antagonism, the primary target of antidepressants [16, 28]. There is evidence that adaptation to antidepressants occurs in humans as measured by positron emission tomography binding studies: short-term SSRI use reduces the sensitivity of cortical 5-HT2A receptors [29] in patients with depression, 5-HT4 receptor in healthy controls [30] as well as down-regulates 5-HT1A receptors in patients with depression [31]. Additionally, there is evidence that 5-HT1A down-regulation can persist for months and years after antidepressants are ceased [31]. In one neuroimaging study, patients who had been previously treated with antidepressants showed 5-HT1A down-regulation in 38 out of 40 brain regions analysed a mean of 29 months after antidepressants were ceased (range 8–60 months) [32]. The relationship of these persistent receptor changes and withdrawal symptoms has not been investigated empirically, but suggests a mechanism whereby a ‘mis-match’ between the brain’s expectations and inputs may give rise to (potentially long-lasting) withdrawal symptoms [27]. Indeed, there is some indirect evidence connecting this receptor to antidepressant withdrawal symptoms, in that patients with the -1019C allele of the 5-HT1A receptor gene more commonly experienced withdrawal symptoms than those with the -1019G homozygote [33].

Animal studies of antidepressant discontinuation have found a number of alterations to the serotonergic system that persist after SSRI cessation (resolving over varying time periods from 7 to 60 days) following weeks of treatment with antidepressants, in conjunction with the detection of withdrawal effects [16]. The following changes have been detected after antidepressants were stopped: lowered serotonin and serotonin metabolites in the hippocampus and frontal cortex, reduced SERT binding, reduced SERT mRNA in the raphe nucleus, reduced 5-HT1B mRNA in the raphe nucleus and reduced 5-HT2C mRNA in the frontal cortex, reduced oxytocin response (in response to a 5-HT1A receptor agonist) and reduced 5-HT1A sensitivity [16]. Notably, many of these changes persisted for up to 2 weeks in rodent models [16], with 17 days in rats equivalent to a human year [34]. Consistent with this, in animals, long-term treatment with antidepressants produces a reduction in endogenously synthesised levels of serotonin detected [35] after an initial increase [36], although this phenomenon has not been studied in humans. In one study that measured changes for longer, 14 days of fluoxetine treatment in rats produced a reduced oxytocin response that was still present 60 days after drug cessation—four times longer than the period of treatment [16, 37]. Using a widely cited means of drawing equivalencies between rat time and human time, 60 days is equivalent to 3 human years, although this equivalence has not been specifically verified in regard to the duration of adaptations to antidepressant treatment [34, 37]. This is consistent with the time period demonstrated in neuro-imaging of patients who had previously used antidepressants.

Several authors have argued withdrawal symptoms are unlikely to be mediated simply through serotonergic receptors as there are many other downstream effects including effects on norepinephrine, dopamine, glutamate and GABA-ergic pathways, which may also adapt to long-term administration of antidepressants [16, 25]. For example, some authors suggest that the rapid reversal of the inhibition of noradrenergic function in the locus coeruleus caused by the inhibitory influence of increased serotonin levels during antidepressant treatment observed in animal studies when antidepressants are stopped may explain some symptoms of antidepressant withdrawal [38]. The anti-cholinergic effects of paroxetine may lead to cholinergic rebound on cessation, partly accounting for the severity of its withdrawal syndrome [38]. There has been no empirical study of these effects in human subjects.

The pathophysiological principle of adaptation makes it clear why the major determinant of how long withdrawal symptoms persist is not a drug characteristic such as a half-life, but how long it takes neurobiological adaptations to the drug to resolve to a pre-drug state [27]. An analogy might be made to the experience of walking out of a loud concert (more signal as with the increased synaptic serotonin during antidepressant treatment) into a quiet street (physiological levels of serotonin after drug removal), where sounds appear muted because of adaptation of tympanic sensitivity (as with serotonergic sensitivity) for a few minutes while your tympanic membrane re-accommodates to a different average amount of sound (an analogous delay for the serotonergic system to re-adapt to less signal, although it seems to take much longer than a few minutes). The time for the sound to dissipate (or for synaptic serotonin levels to normalise on removal of the drug) is trivial; it is the time taken for tympanic re-accommodation that determines the duration of the withdrawal effects (as for re-accommodation of the processes affected by long-term antidepressant use). Because of wide-ranging adaptations in the brain and body to antidepressants, withdrawal symptoms can manifest in both physical and psychological symptoms [10, 39].

Notably, the understanding that the degree of adaptation to the drug predicts withdrawal effects suggests lines of research that could be undertaken to further explore this phenomenon. The degree of adaptation to the drug for an individual could be quantified to predict the risk of withdrawal. Drawing from animal studies and human neuro-imaging studies, adaptation may be found in the reduced sensitivity and number of serotonergic receptors [29, 30], lowered serotonin and serotonin metabolites, reduced SERT binding, reduced oxytocin response, or reduced 5-HT1A sensitivity [16]. For example, further studies evaluating the hypothesis that patients with a greater degree of 5-HT1A down-regulation caused by antidepressant administration are more likely to have withdrawal symptoms on cessation would be informative. Currently, the adaptations to antidepressants demonstrated in neuro-imaging of clinical subjects and in animal studies provide a plausible mechanism for withdrawal but we cannot draw definitive conclusions that this represents the underlying pathophysiology of withdrawal effects.

2 Literature Search

In order to investigate the aspects of antidepressant use that influence withdrawal incidence, severity and duration, we performed a narrative review following principles outlined in the Scale for the Assessment of Narrative Review Articles (SANRA) [40].

A search on PubMed for systematic reviews published from 2000 until 13 May, 2022 was performed using the search string “antidepressants” AND (“withdrawal OR discontinuation”), restricting the search to systematic reviews. This yielded 325 results. Further analyses were added as suggested by experts in the field, reviewers of this article or by a search of the references of included articles, including an examination performed by the Committee on the Safety of Medicines (CSM) on antidepressant withdrawal, bringing the total to 331 results. We selected studies that were in English, dealt with human adults and which included some examination of a hypothesised determinant of withdrawal risk such as varying dose of antidepressant, different types of antidepressants or varying duration of antidepressant treatment. The full text of 11 studies was screened and after screening, nine reviews or other analyses were included. The intention of this review was to glean evidence about the determinants of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms from these existing reviews or analyses.

3 Antidepressant Withdrawal Symptoms

3.1 Incidence of Antidepressant Withdrawal Symptoms

A 2019 systematic review identified 14 relevant studies from which to calculate the incidence of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms [5]. The incidence rates ranged from 27 to 86%, with a median of 55% and a weighted average of 56.4% [5]. Restricting the analysis only to double-blind randomised controlled trials (RCTs) from this review, the incidence of withdrawal syndrome was 53.9% (six RCTs, 731 participants) [Table 1] (where the majority of studies used an increase in discontinuation emergent signs and symptoms [DESS] [41] of ≥ 4 to define a withdrawal syndrome).

Table 1 Incidence of withdrawal in double-blind RCTs captured in Davies and Read [5]

A potential limitation of this review was that, in addition to RCTs and observational studies, it included three online surveys; critics point out it is possible that surveys may capture a skewed sample of patients motivated to answer the survey because of their experience with more severe withdrawal symptoms than average, and withdrawal symptoms were reported by patients rather than using objective withdrawal questionnaires [4, 14]. However, the weighted average incidence of withdrawal symptoms was similar in the six RCTs (53.9%) to the five observational studies (52.5%) and the three online surveys (57.1%) [5]. Restricting analysis to studies of SSRIs, the most widely used class of antidepressants, discontinuation syndromes occurred with a median rate of 53.6%, and a weighted average of 50.5% [5]. The double-blind, placebo-controlled, staggered discontinuation studies are the most reliable of the studies conducted and are highlighted in Table 1.

3.1.1 Nocebo/Psychosomatic Effects

Another limitation to the Davies and Read review is reporting of single-arm frequencies, that is, withdrawal effects from stopping antidepressants are not compared to withdrawal effects from stopping placebo or continuing antidepressants as a control. Some have suggested that withdrawal symptoms may be a psychosomatic response rather than genuine physiological symptoms [4]. These authors have hypothesised that patients have negative expectations of the consequence of stopping their antidepressants, leading to nocebo withdrawal effects (the opposite of the placebo effect) [4]. The presence of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms in both animals [16] and neonates of antidepressant-using mothers [42] suggests that the process is primarily physiological rather than psychosomatic. Randomised controlled trials conducted to detect withdrawal symptoms used double-blind placebo-controlled designs so that the patient and doctor were unaware whether the patient was receiving a continuation of their antidepressant or identical placebo pills for several days [41, 43]. This design minimises the role of psychological expectation or nocebo effects and therefore suggests that withdrawal effects are physiological consequences of stopping the medication [41]. In one carefully conducted study, the average number of new symptoms recorded on the DESS scale was 5.7 (standard deviation 6.96) for sertraline patients and 7.8 (standard deviation 8.55) for paroxetine-treated patients, suggesting a large number of symptoms, including physical symptoms (such as dizziness, and headache) with onset at the same time, and resolution upon re-commencing the antidepressants (unbeknownst to the participants) consistent with a physiological syndrome [41]. Furthermore, cessation of fluoxetine (whose half-life of 7–15 days makes withdrawal symptoms unlikely in the 5–8 days of the study) produced a non-significant increase of 0.2 symptoms, serving as a useful negative control group [41].

The rate of such nocebo withdrawal effects has been explored in six double-blind RCTs in two ways: either by cessation of placebo or continuation of antidepressants unbeknownst to the patient. These six studies were reviewed previously [44] and results are presented in Table 2 [44]. Excluding the outlier of 75% (defined as experiencing any symptom of withdrawal) [45] yields a weighted average of nocebo withdrawal effects of 11.8%. A limitation to this analysis is that the severity of withdrawal effects is not measured in these studies, and it is not clear that ‘dizziness’ or ‘nausea’ reported in the nocebo group is the same as such symptoms in the withdrawal group (there are case reports of such symptoms being severe enough to prompt an investigation of stroke) [46]. Furthermore, withdrawal effects from placebo show no relationship with duration of treatment, in the same way that withdrawal from paroxetine does, with marked divergence over time (Fig. 2, with Fisher’s exact tests showing significant differences between the two groups after 2 months [p = 0.005], 3 months [p < 0.001] and more than 4 months of treatment [p < 0.001]) [47]. Therefore, as with all health symptoms, there may be a psychological component to withdrawal effects but this is likely to be minor. For example, the data in Fig. 2 appear to be consistent with a ceiling value similar to that calculated above for nocebo withdrawal. Adjusting the reported single-arm frequencies for antidepressant withdrawal for the rate of withdrawal in the ‘nocebo’ condition yields an incidence of 42.1% (53.9–11.8%) for antidepressants in double-blind RCTs and 38.7% (50.5–11.8%) for trials looking specifically at SSRIs [44].

Table 2 Summary of studies that have estimated the role of nocebo effects in the incidence of withdrawal syndrome
Fig. 2
figure 2

Relationship between duration of treatment (before stopping) and proportion of patients who experienced withdrawal effects on stopping either paroxetine or placebo (overall trend p value <0.001) [47]. Significance for Fisher exact tests for by-month group comparisons. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001

3.2 Severity of Antidepressant Withdrawal Symptoms

The severity of the withdrawal syndrome from SSRIs varies widely, with a range from mild short-lasting cases that can be managed with education and reassurance, to severe cases that cause significant disruptions to normal functioning [5, 10]. This variability presumably relates to differing degrees of neurobiological adaptation to antidepressants amongst individuals. In its severe form, the SSRI withdrawal syndrome has been reported to be associated with ataxia leading to falls, electric shock sensations that impair walking and driving [10], and urgent consultations at emergency departments [46, 48], in published case studies. The discontinuation period is also associated with a 60% relative increase in suicide attempts, compared with previous users of antidepressants [11]. It is improbable that these suicide attempts arose as a consequence of relapse, as relapse is generally thought to be delayed in onset for more than 2 weeks after stopping antidepressants [49].

The systematic review also identified five studies that evaluated the severity of withdrawal effects [5], with nearly half of participants who had experienced withdrawal effects choosing the most extreme option in the scale offered to them to describe the severity of those effects [5]. For example, in response to a question ‘How severely do you feel withdrawal has affected your life?’ on a scale of 0–10 given to 580 people who had attempted withdrawal from antidepressants, mostly SSRIs, 43% (249) of participants chose 10, the highest level of the scale [50]. As above, it is possible that the online survey method employed by four of these studies may be biased by patients with more negative experiences; however, it is notable that somewhat more than half of the participants surveyed in these studies had used antidepressants for more than 3 years [51], similar to the wider English population (where about half of antidepressant users have been taking them for more than 2 years) [2]. However, the self-selected nature of this population of respondents limits the ability to extrapolate to the wider population of people taking antidepressants.

The remaining study, conducted by Pfizer, found that 34.3% of patients treated with sertraline for 8 weeks experienced moderately severe symptoms (as rated by an investigator on a global assessment), 23.9% of them experienced a mild withdrawal reaction, while 23.9% reported a minimal reaction [52]. For venlafaxine, after only 8 weeks of use, 38.7% of patients were rated by study researchers as experiencing moderately severe withdrawal symptoms, with 3.2% as ‘severe’ and 1.6% as ‘very severe’ [52]. As a longer duration of treatment appears to be associated with a greater incidence and severity of withdrawal symptoms (see below) [47, 51], patients who are taking antidepressants for longer than 8 weeks are more likely to experience more severe withdrawal symptoms.

Other double-blind staggered RCTs of discontinuation of antidepressants do not measure the severity of withdrawal per se but the mean number of symptoms recorded on the DESS, for each antidepressant provides a proxy measure of severity, albeit imperfect, because a patient could have many minimal symptoms or one very severe symptom-aspects that would not be captured by such a method. Nevertheless, as shown in Table 3, withdrawal symptoms from paroxetine are numerically greater than for citalopram and sertraline, which are in turn numerically greater than fluoxetine (albeit measured only over short periods of time), consistent with their risk of withdrawal (see below).

Table 3 Incidence of withdrawal and measure of severity (by numerical count of DESS or investigator global assessment) for specific antidepressants in double-blind randomised controlled trials captured in Davies and Read [5]

3.3 Duration of Withdrawal Symptoms

There is significant evidence that withdrawal symptoms can last for weeks, months or even years in some cases [13, 50], but a weighted average of the ten studies included in the recent systematic review was not possible, owing to methodological heterogeneity [5]. One study examining reports from doctors to the Medicines and Healthcare products Regulatory Agency in the UK described a duration of withdrawal symptoms from 1 to 52 days, with an average of 10.5 days, although this is likely to represent an underestimate as a number of patients taking paroxetine had to be re-started on the drug because their withdrawal symptoms were too severe [53]. A Royal College of Psychiatrists online survey found that for the 512 users who experienced withdrawal, the symptoms lasted for up to 6 weeks, and a quarter of the group reported anxiety lasting more than 12 weeks [5]. This is consistent with an earlier study that found withdrawal symptoms lasted at least 6 weeks in 40% of people [45]. In another online study of 580 people who had withdrawn from antidepressant medication, 86.7% responded that the syndrome had lasted at least 2 months, 58.6% at least 1 year and 16.2% for more than 3 years [50], although this study may have surveyed a population with a more severe experience of withdrawal than average. Other studies also report longer durations of withdrawal symptoms, in at least some cases, symptoms can persist for years [13, 50, 54]. It is difficult to establish to what extent these very long-lasting syndromes represent outliers, as it is not possible to establish that these are representative populations of antidepressant users, but withdrawal symptoms likely persist significantly longer than the 1-week or 2-week periods that have been previously ascribed to them, albeit in an unknown proportion of patients [55].

3.4 Determinants of Antidepressant Withdrawal Symptoms

As adaptations to the presence of the drug are thought to underlie withdrawal symptoms, a longer duration of use and a higher dosage would be expected to contribute to the degree of adaptation and thus to the incidence, severity and duration of withdrawal symptoms. Drugs with shorter half-lives may induce more severe withdrawal effects with an earlier onset than those drugs with longer half-lives because the mis-match between what the brain has accommodated to and what is provided is greater (Fig. 1). Alternatively, different antidepressants may cause greater effects on the brain, perhaps related to their different receptor targets and binding properties. Individual physiological differences [10, 27] may also affect the degree of adaptation to the drug and thus the risk of withdrawal symptoms. We explore evidence for all of these determinants.

3.4.1 Effect of Duration of Use on Incidence, Severity and Duration of Withdrawal Symptoms

Although the primary data are not publicly available, the CSM was granted access to unpublished manufacturer’s data for antidepressant withdrawal effects for a variety of antidepressants [47]. The Committee was provided with all clinical trial data (placebo-controlled or active-controlled studies) from which the manufacturers had evaluated the incidence and severity of withdrawal effects (calculated according to the manner that they had been evaluated in the trials) and their relationship to duration of use and tapering.

Duration of use of paroxetine was found to be related to the incidence of withdrawal symptoms on stopping, when adult clinical trial data were obtained from the manufacturer (Fig. 2) [47]. Although there was not enough information for the CSM to make a determination of the role of duration of treatment in citalopram withdrawal effects, there was some indication that a longer duration of treatment with escitalopram increased the risk of a withdrawal reaction [47]. There was limited evidence of an effect related to the duration of use for fluoxetine, fluvoxamine (although there was confusion between adverse effects that led to a drop out from trials and adverse effects arising from the withdrawal process itself), mirtazapine (pooled analyses not appropriate to evaluate this question) or sertraline. Venlafaxine demonstrated some evidence of a duration of treatment effect: 14% of patients after 8 weeks of treatment experienced dizziness, while this rose to 29% after 24 weeks of treatment [47].

Although no RCTs examined the severity of withdrawal symptoms in association with treatment duration (rather, they only counted the number of symptoms), online surveys of patients did so [56, 57]. Although these surveys may have captured skewed samples, the line of best fit suggests a clear gradient between the duration of use and severity (as well as the incidence) of withdrawal syndrome (Fig. 3) [57]. These surveys included patients who stopped taking various antidepressants including paroxetine, venlafaxine, citalopram, fluoxetine, escitalopram, sertraline and tricyclic antidepressants [57]. Duration of antidepressant use is likely to lead to greater physiological adaptations increasing the risk and severity of withdrawal symptoms [57]. The relationship between duration of use and the risk of withdrawal effects also appears to have a second-order relationship (the gradient between the risk of withdrawal effects become less steep over time) perhaps consistent with ceiling effects for adaptation.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Relationship between duration of treatment and severity and duration of withdrawal symptoms from surveys of antidepressant users and observational studies. The relationship between duration of treatment of antidepressants and incidence of moderate or severe withdrawal symptoms. Graph is derived from data in Read et al. [57]

3.4.2 Effect of Dose on Withdrawal Symptoms

The CSM also examined the relationship between the dose of medication and the risk of withdrawal, and though it was not explicit about the nature of these studies, the implication is that these data were derived from studies mostly abruptly stopping the medications [47]. Although it is difficult to make comparisons between such studies analysed by the CSM, and other studies that looked at dose-dependent effects [58] because such data are susceptible to the ecological fallacy, there is evidence of dose-dependent effects within individual trials (Table 4). There was a higher incidence of withdrawal effects for higher dosages of paroxetine in the analysis by the CSM [47], although the effect reached a threshold at 20 mg (Table 4), probably because of the hyperbolic relationship between antidepressant dosage and effect on its target receptors [59,60,61,62]. There was a more pronounced dose-dependent relationship for venlafaxine withdrawal effects (Table 4) [47], with an increased incidence at higher dosages possibly related to greater noradrenergic effects at these dosages [63, 64]. Fluvoxamine and mirtazapine did not demonstrate clear dose-dependent effects; however, the CSM cautioned that the pooled analysis applied may not have been appropriate to detect these effects [47]. Overall, dosage does appear to have some relationship to the risk of withdrawal symptoms (where higher doses were associated with an increased risk of withdrawal), but its influence may not be as strong as the duration of use, perhaps because higher dosages have only small additional pharmacological effects over minimum clinically employed dosages because of the hyperbolic shape of their dose-response curves [60, 62, 65,66,67].

Table 4 Relationship between dosage of antidepressants and incidence of withdrawal effects

3.4.3 Effect of Drug Type on Withdrawal Symptoms

It has been suggested that the risk of withdrawal symptoms varies between different antidepressants. This could be due to differing half-lives, with drugs with shorter half-lives being eliminated more quickly and therefore producing more precipitous drops in inputs ‘expected’ by the system (Fig. 1) [15]. This is supported by the finding that percentage reductions in plasma concentrations of fluoxetine, sertraline and paroxetine, following cessation, showed a significant correlation with the appearance of withdrawal symptoms [68]. Cessation of paroxetine for several days causes withdrawal symptoms in 66–100% of patients [41, 43], cessation of sertraline in 59–60% of patients [41, 43] and fluoxetine in 14–77% of patients [41, 43], (double-blind RCTs examining drugs are summarised in Table 3). In surveys, which may include a self-selected population, these differences among common SSRIs are roughly preserved: 69, 62 and 44% of patients stopping paroxetine, sertraline, and fluoxetine, respectively, report withdrawal symptoms [5].

However, withdrawal symptoms following the cessation of fluoxetine, the SSRI with the longest half-life (7–15 days for its active metabolite, nor-fluoxetine), has been observed to occur with a delay of onset of 4–6 weeks after discontinuation in one study [45], and 2 weeks after discontinuation in another [69]. In two studies that have examined withdrawal effects for fluoxetine for several days after stopping, one found a minor increase on average of 1.3 points (95% confidence interval − 1.8 to 4.3) on the DESS [43] and the other found a barely detectable mean increase of 0.2 (standard deviation 5.22) points on the DESS [41], which seems to suggest that the 77% incidence in Hindmarch et al. [43] may provide an exaggerated view of the likelihood of withdrawal symptoms from this medication in the short term. However, given the delayed onset of withdrawal effects predicted by the long elimination half-life of fluoxetine, it is unclear whether there would be a greater incidence of withdrawal effects with a longer follow-up; withdrawal may be rarer than for other antidepressants.

Interestingly, in the CSM analysis, mirtazapine appeared to cause less withdrawal effects than placebo [47], although reports to a helpline concerning withdrawal effects suggest that mirtazapine is associated with withdrawal problems as, or more, commonly as SSRIs [23]. Similarly, patients treated with fluvoxamine in drug company trials showed a similar rate of adverse effects on stopping as those patients treated with placebo (for some durations of treatment, withdrawal incidence was higher for the placebo group), although the CSM commented that the non-systematic manner in which these data were collected may underestimate the withdrawal effects [47]. This finding is in contrast to the data presented by an analysis of calls to a medication helpline in England for withdrawal effects (normalised to national prescription rates), which found that fluvoxamine was markedly over-represented with more calls than any other SSRIs, except paroxetine, and more calls than for venlafaxine [23], perhaps reflecting differences for the longer term use seen in clinical practice than in short-term manufacturer studies.

In general, this is a major limitation with studies examining withdrawal effects from antidepressants—most are conducted in patients enrolled in acute efficacy trials who are exposed to several weeks (or sometimes a short number of months) of treatment [4, 58]. As half of the people taking antidepressants in England are taking these medications for more than 2 years [2], and a longer duration predicts greater withdrawal effects, such studies are likely to under-estimate the withdrawal effects of medication, despite being otherwise well conducted. For example, in Rosenbaum et al. [41], 66% of patients treated for 11.6 months on average with paroxetine met the threshold for a withdrawal syndrome (four or more new-onset symptoms, as measured by the DESS), but in the analysis by Baldwin et al. [58] after 12 weeks of paroxetine treatment, only 28.4–31.5% of patients met the same criteria for a withdrawal syndrome (with 32.7% after 27 weeks of treatment).

Paroxetine and fluoxetine are both metabolised by cytochrome P450 2D6 (while the active metabolite of fluoxetine, norfluoxetine, is metabolised by cytochrome P450 3A4) and inhibit their own metabolism, resulting in non-linear kinetics [70]. This predicts disproportionate declines in plasma concentrations during dose reduction. While this effect may not be clinically significant for fluoxetine because of its long half-life, it is likely to be significant for paroxetine [28]. In addition, paroxetine may produce a more severe withdrawal syndrome than other SSRIs because it exhibits the highest known binding affinity for the central site of SERT [71], and demonstrates muscarinic antagonist effects and moderate norepinephrine transporter-inhibiting effects as well [16, 28].

One study each has examined withdrawal effects for agomelatine and vortioxetine. No withdrawal effects were detected for agomelatine after 12 weeks of treatment [72], consistent with the finding that agomelatine was the antidepressant least likely to be reported to the World Health Organization pharmacovigilance service [73]. No significant increase in withdrawal symptoms after 8 weeks of vortioxetine treatment was detected when compared to placebo, although the relatively long elimination half-life of vortioxetine (66 h) limits the conclusions that can be drawn from this study [74]. Henssler and colleagues designated vortioxetine a ‘moderate risk’ of withdrawal based on a systematic appraisal of studies, not restricted to RCTs.

3.4.3.1 Tiers of Risk Based on Drug Type

A recent structured analysis by Henssler et al. [15] attempted to quantify the relative risks of different antidepressants based on controlled trials, cohort studies, retrospective analyses and case reports [15]. We have supplemented this review with an analysis of calls to an English medication helpline for issues related to withdrawal, normalised to prescription numbers [23], as well as a comparative study of withdrawal effects from the World Health Organization pharmacovigilance database to provide a summary table of levels of risk for antidepressants (Table 5). We categorised antidepressants based on the metric of calls to a helpline for withdrawal, normalised to prescription numbers according to the following categories: high (≥ 15), moderate (6–14) and low (≤ 5) [23]. We also incorporated information from the review by Henssler et al., compressing ‘very high risk’ and ‘high risk’ into ‘high risk’. Last, we utilised the World Health Organization pharmacovigilance database to designate four antidepressants as ‘high risk’ based on a reporting odds ratio of greater than 2: duloxetine, paroxetine, venlafaxine and desvenlafaxine [73]. When there was contradictory categorisation, we chose the higher risk category on the precautionary principle. However, for some of the antidepressants outlined here only case reports were available [15], thus this summary can only be considered preliminary. Notably, the categories of risk generated from this procedure are consistent with findings of an earlier analysis that found that escitalopram had significantly lower rates of withdrawal effects than venlafaxine or paroxetine [58].

Table 5 Common antidepressants stratified by risk of withdrawal symptoms, derived from Henssler et al. [15], calls to a withdrawal helpline, normalised to prescription numbers [23] and analysis of reports to a WHO pharmacovigilance service [73]

Additionally, it is possible that formulations of antidepressants that convey sustained-release properties may be associated with less withdrawal effects than instant-release preparations, given the association observed between short-half and long half-life antidepressants [73], although these comparisons have not yet been studied.

3.4.4 Other Determinants of Withdrawal Risk

Individual characteristics may influence the risk of antidepressant withdrawal symptoms, related to the metabolism of the SSRI, sensitivity of SERT to inhibition and psychological factors [10, 75]. It has been hypothesised that different rates of metabolism of drugs (determined by cytochrome P450 polymorphisms) may affect the risk of withdrawal [59] but no such studies have investigated this relationship. The likelihood of withdrawal symptoms has been be associated with the C(-1019)G polymorphism of the 5HT1A receptor gene, known to be affected by long-term antidepressant treatment [33]. One clinical indication of likelihood of withdrawal is past experience of withdrawal, either when accidentally forgetting medication (e.g. when going on holiday) or on previous attempts to stop. There are likely to be other factors that determine the incidence and severity of withdrawal but this has not been widely studied.

Although there has been limited research into the topic, analyses conducted so far indicate that there is no difference in the risk of withdrawal effects depending on the diagnosis for which the medication was commenced [58]. This is consistent with the understanding that it is the effect of the drug on the brain that determines withdrawal effects [25].

3.5 Stratifying Patients on Risk of Withdrawal Effects

Based on the above characteristics that influence the risk of withdrawal symptoms, we have derived a broad means of stratifying patients with regard to their risk of withdrawal symptoms via means of a preliminary tool. From clinical experience, the strongest predictor of withdrawal symptoms is past experience of withdrawal symptoms (in a previous attempt at discontinuation, a drug switch or after skipped doses), as recognised in similar efforts to determine risk [76], and so this is given strong weighting (3 points) [Table 6]. Duration of use appears to have a strong effect on the risk of withdrawal symptoms, including their severity and therefore this has been given strong emphasis (3 points). Antidepressant type (4 points) has been associated with varying risk, and higher doses (1 point) as well, though to a lesser extent. An assessment of these risk factors can help to estimate the risk of withdrawal for a particular individual (Table 6 legend), which may inform the rate of taper suggested, with lower risk patients more likely to be able to tolerate quicker tapers, and higher risk patients selected for slower approaches.

Table 6 Preliminary tool for evaluation of risk of withdrawal for an individual patient

This approach to risk estimation can only be seen as preliminary, building on similar previous efforts [76]. This tool requires validation before it is widely adopted to guide the choice of discontinuation strategies in clinical practice. This validation could be performed by the aggregation of sociodemographic, clinical and medication usage characteristics from large numbers of patients discontinuing medication, and their rate of tapering and severity (and duration) of withdrawal effects or analysis of existing databases containing such information. This would allow development of the tool to iteratively reflect the risk of withdrawal effects, allowing better prediction for individual patients.

4 Conclusions

We have reviewed the existing literature on the incidence, severity and duration of withdrawal symptoms as well as examined the relationship between characteristics of use (such as dosage, duration of use and type of antidepressant) and withdrawal symptoms. Information in these domains is limited and more research is required to draw firmer conclusions about the determinants of withdrawal symptoms, particularly regarding severity and duration.

From existing data, there appears to be a relationship between the duration of antidepressant use and the risk of withdrawal symptoms, consistent with the idea that a greater duration of use will produce greater neurological adaptation. We also reviewed data suggesting some evidence of a weak relationship between a greater dosage of antidepressant and a greater chance of withdrawal symptoms, but that there may be ceiling effects, consistent with the hyperbolic relationship between the dose and effect of antidepressants resulting in target receptor saturation [59]. There appears also to be a wide variation of risk of withdrawal based on which antidepressant is taken. Past experience of withdrawal symptoms on reducing or stopping medication is a strong predictor of withdrawal symptoms on subsequent attempts to reduce or stop.

From these risk factors, we have derived a simple rubric for determining withdrawal risk for a given patient, which may be useful in clinical practice for stratifying people according to risk. We hope that future empirical work will be able to offer a refined version of this risk calculator.