Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

How can I become a member of my culture?: shared representations of community-related moral violation in Japan and the U.S.

  • Original Research Article
  • Published:
Culture and Brain

Abstract

Morality not only serves as the standard of right and wrong behavior, it also plays a central role in people’s everyday social interactions. The current study examined how mental representations of morality violations are shared across Western and non-Western cultures, utilizing Shweder’s three ethics of Autonomy, Community, and Divinity. First, Japanese and American undergraduates were instructed on the definitions of these three ethic domains and were asked to freely describe any situations they could think of concerning violations in each domain. A subset of these descriptions of violations that correctly matched Shweder’s original definitions were then given to a separate sample of Japanese and American college students, who then categorized each situation into one of the three domains and rated its wrongness. The results showed that Autonomy-violating situations resulted in a greater number of “errors” (i.e., classified into an unintended domain) compared to the other ethical violations, regardless of the place of origin for both Japanese and American participants. In addition, both Japanese and American participants were generally more accurate when categorizing home-generated (vs. foreign-generated) situations, particularly when they involved the Community ethic. On average, participants gave more intense ratings of wrongness to situations generated by members of their same culture. The own-culture advantage was most consistent for the Community ethic. Violations of the Community ethic may have the most robust and shared within-group interpretation because this promotes an adaptive fit into one’s own culture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The sphericity assumption for the variable of ethic was violated, Mauchly’s W(2) = .87, p < .001, so the Greenhouse–Geisser adjustment is reported here.

  2. The Greenhouse–Geisser adjustment is reported here to correct for violation of the sphericity assumption, Mauchly’s W(2) = .78, p < .001.

References

  • Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin,117, 497–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bennis, W. M., Medin, D. L., & Bartels, D. M. (2010). The costs and benefits of calculation and moral rules. Perspectives on Psychological Science,5, 187–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2009). Culture and the evolution of human cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B,364, 3281–3288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brewer, M. B. (2004). Taking the social origins of human nature more seriously: Toward a more imperialist social psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review,8, 107–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carnes, N. C., Lickel, B., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (2015). Shared perceptions: Morality is embedded in social contexts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,41, 351–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiu, C. Y., Gelfand, M. J., Yamagishi, T., Shteynberg, G., & Wan, C. (2010). Intersubjective culture: The role of intersubjective perceptions in cross-cultural research. Perspectives on Psychological Science,5, 482–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clifford, S., Iyengar, V., Cabeza, R., & Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (2015). Moral foundations vignettes: A standardized stimulus database of scenarios based on moral foundations theory. Behavior research methods, 47, 1178–1198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Côté, S., Piff, P. K., & Willer, R. (2013). For whom do the ends justify the means? Social class and utilitarian moral judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,104(3), 490–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F., & De Waal, F. B. M. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong in humans and other animals. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dehghani, M., Johnson, K., Hoover, J., Sagi, E., Garten, J., Parmar, N. J., … Graham, J. (2016). Purity homophily in social networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(3), 366.

  • DeScioli, P., & Kurzban, R. (2013). A solution to the mysteries of morality. Psychological Bulletin,139, 477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flack, J. C., & De Waal, F. B. (2000). Being nice is not a building block of morality. Journal of Consciousness Studies,7, 67–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., & Haidt, J. (2010). Beyond beliefs: Religions bind individuals into moral communities. Personality and Social Psychology Review,14, 140–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S Pl, et al. (2013). Moral Foundations Theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology,47, 55–130.

    Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,96, 1029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,101, 366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. New York: Vintage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., Graham, J., & Ditto, P. (2015). The Volkswagen of moral psychology. Retrieved from http://www.spsp.org/news-center/blog/volkswagen-of-morality.

  • Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How five sets of innate intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. The Innate Mind,3, 367–391.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., Koller, S. J., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,4, 613–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hashimoto, H. (2011). Interdependence as a self-sustaining set of beliefs. The Japanese Journal Experimental Social Psychology,50, 182–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J. (2015). Cultural psychology: Third International Student Edition. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick, D. T., Maner, J. K., Butner, J., Li, N. P., Becker, D. V., & Schaller, M. (2002). Dynamical evolutionary psychology: mapping the domains of the new interactionist paradigm. Personality and Social Psychology Review,6, 347–356.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick, D. T., Maner, J. K., & Li, N. P. (2005). Evolutionary social psychology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 803–827). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K. R., Kang, J. S., & Yun, S. (2012). Moral intuitions and political orientation: Similarities and differences between South Korea and the United States. Psychological Reports,111, 173–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,72(6), 1245–1267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K. (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,97(2), 236–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review,98, 224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meindl, P., & Graham, J. (2014). Know thy participant: The trouble with nomothetic assumptions in moral psychology. Advances in Experimental Moral Psychology, 233–252.

  • Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1992). Culture and moral judgment: How are conflicts between justice and interpersonal responsibilities resolved? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,62, 541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin,128, 3–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,76, 574.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A. (1990). Ethical relativism: Is there a defensible version? Ethos,18, 205–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R. A., & Haidt, J. (2000). The cultural psychology of the emotions: Ancient and new. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 397–414). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shweder, R., Much, N., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (1997). Divinity and the “big three” explanations of suffering. Morality and Health,119, 119–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zou, X., Tam, K. P., Morris, M. W., Lee, S. L., Lau, I. Y. M., & Chiu, C. Y. (2009). Culture as common sense: Perceived consensus versus personal beliefs as mechanisms of cultural influence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,97, 579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Phylicia Hardy, Ariel Kershner, Kayla Sansevere for their assistance in data collection.

Funding

The present study was supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research (Grant Nos. 1503446, 18H01078) from Japan Society for the Promotion of Science granted to the fourth author.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Akiko Matsuo.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 1.

Table 1 All the morality-violating situations on the questionnaire that were used in the current research

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Matsuo, A., Brown, C.M., Norasakkunkit, V. et al. How can I become a member of my culture?: shared representations of community-related moral violation in Japan and the U.S.. Cult. Brain 8, 96–115 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-019-00084-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-019-00084-z

Keywords

Navigation