Abstract
Purpose of Review
This review brings together current evidence from observational, acute, and chronic exercise training studies to inform public debate on the impact of physical activity and exercise on food reward.
Recent Findings
Low levels of physical activity are associated with higher liking and wanting for high-energy food. Acute bouts of exercise tend to reduce behavioral indices of reward for high-energy food in inactive individuals. A dissociation in liking (increase) and wanting (decrease) may occur during chronic exercise training associated with loss of body fat. Habitual moderate-to-vigorous physical activity is associated with lower liking and wanting for high-fat food, and higher liking for low-fat food.
Summary
Food reward does not counteract the benefit of increasing physical activity levels for obesity management. Exercise training appears to be accompanied by positive changes in food preferences in line with an overall improvement in appetite control.
Introduction
Among the reasons that people with obesity cite for avoiding exercise is a lack of enjoyment and perceived failure to lose weight [1, 2]. Moreover, there is a misconception that persists among some individuals that exercise is counter-productive for weight management. This common assertion is reinforced by occasional reports in the media about exercise and food reward [3, 4]. Biological explanations, reliant on soft evidence, have been put forward suggesting that glycogen depletion, reduced blood glucose levels, endorphin release or other signals generated during exercise can increase appetite or cause specific cravings for foods. Alternatively, psychological accounts propose that high fat or sugary foods are sought out post-exercise to counteract negative affect or reward virtuous behavior. Research over the past 10 years has shown that physical activity and eating behavior are loosely coupled, but the physiological and neurocognitive mechanisms that contribute to this relationship are complex [5]. Moreover, the evidence for the impact of physical activity on food reward is difficult to assess due to the absence of randomized controlled trials and differences between study designs—encompassing observational, acute, and chronic interventions. Differences also exist in the modality and intensity of physical activity examined and the variety of methodologies used to measure reward responses to food. In a review of longitudinal weight management interventions that measured food reward outcomes at baseline and follow-up, Oustric and colleagues [6••] identified 17 studies consisting of dietary, pharmacological, cognitive, and exercise-based intervention types. Overall, a post-intervention reduction in food reward was found across all treatment types—except exercise, where no consistent changes were reported. While it is interesting to speculate that exercise training may have a moderating influence on the relationship between weight management and food reward, only three exercise studies were eligible for inclusion in this systematic review. Moreover, interpretation was limited by small sample sizes, lack of a control condition in one study, and inconsistent use of food reward measures. Further research is needed to update and summarize the available evidence from observational, acute, and chronic study designs to gain an overview of the influence of physical activity and exercise on food reward. With this objective in mind, the specific aims of this review were to describe the role of physical activity and food reward in appetite control and weight management; to tabulate and synthesize the different types of evidence that have addressed the impact of physical activity on food reward; and to discuss, and where possible harmonize, the findings in the light of relevant moderators and methodological issues. To our knowledge, there is currently no comprehensive review of the literature on physical activity and food reward. A critical examination of the evidence may help to clarify some of the perceived barriers for engaging in physical activity and exercise for obesity management.
Current Thinking on the Role of Physical Activity in Appetite Control
In the last 5 years, evidence has accrued showing that physical activity affects both episodic (meal-to-meal) and tonic (basal) homeostatic mechanisms that influence appetite control [7]. Acute exercise influences gastric emptying [8], and attenuates the release of ghrelin and increases the secretion of satiety peptides, e.g., peptide YY, glucagon-like peptide-1, and pancreatic polypeptide [9]. Chronic exercise improves body composition [10,11,12], and leptin and insulin sensitivity [13,14,15].
Our research has shown that habitual physical activity has a small positive association with daily energy intake (accounting for around ~ 3% of the variance) [16], but this is only logical when considering the increase in energy requirements and the longer-term indirect effects from increased resting metabolic rate after changes in fat-free mass. It has been proposed that chronic exercise influences appetite control through an increase in hunger but also a strengthening of post-meal satiety [17]. Indeed, exercise and physical activity appear to interact with nutritional factors to enhance satiety signaling, with studies showing that people who engaged in more exercise and physical activity were better able to compensate for differences in the energy content of food (achieved by increasing the ratio of fat to carbohydrate) at a subsequent meal than their less active counterparts [18]. While more active individuals are driven to eat more due to their greater energy requirements, their stronger satiety response to food appears to allow for a better matching between energy intake and energy expenditure [19••]. This relationship between physical activity level and daily energy intake is best represented by a J-shaped curve, whereby individuals with low levels of physical activity on the left of the curve have dysregulated appetite with greater intake than expenditure, and individuals with greater levels of physical activity towards the right of the curve have a proportional increase in intake with increasing expenditure. These findings suggest that concerns about exercise or increased levels of physical activity being counter-productive for obesity management should be concerned with non-homeostatic and food reward-related mechanisms that may be driving unhealthy food choices.
Defining Food Reward and Its Importance for Weight Management
Food reward is important for understanding appetite control and has the logical status of an intervening variable that guides eating behavior. Food reward is encoded by distinct neural pathways in the brain and can be modulated by metabolic signaling, sensory stimuli from the food environment and cognitive processes such as attention, learning, and memory [20]. Food reward is often conceptualized to consist of two distinct sub-components—“liking” and “wanting.” These have been broadly studied [21], following extensive work demonstrating their dissociation in the brain and behavior of many species including humans [22]. Liking is the sensory pleasure exerted while eating a food and wanting is rather the, often implicit, drive to eat triggered by a food cue [23]. Liking and implicit wanting for energy-dense foods are related to excess energy intake in free-living and laboratory settings [23, 24]. However, liking accounts only for a small proportion of the variance in food intake [25, 26], and unconscious processes such as implicit wanting may play a larger role in driving overeating [27, 28]. Food reward is an important factor in weight management through its intervening status between the nutritional requirements of the body and stimulation from the food environment [23], but it is likely that liking and implicit wanting sometimes act separately to influence appetite control [6••].
While several techniques to measure food reward have been developed [21] (e.g., reinforcing value tasks, willingness to pay), the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire (LFPQ) has been designed to measure both liking and implicit wanting for distinct dimensions (e.g., fat and sweet taste) of foods common in the diet. Food reward measured by the LFPQ can be interpreted as both a state- and a trait-dependent measure depending on the timing and condition of measurement. Indeed, liking and wanting have been shown to be partially dissociable pre to post food consumption according to sensory-specific satiety [29]. On the contrary, food cravings, defined here as spontaneous instances of strong explicit wanting for a specific food, tend to be measured as a trait reflecting the frequency, intensity or quality of cravings experienced over a specified time period [30,31,32]. Neural activation to foods measured through functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are also used as an inference of food reward and this technique allows the analysis of different regions of interest and their functional connectivity [33]. However, fMRI measures of reward should be used in conjunction with behavioral measures or actual food intake to support their interpretation.
Review of Literature
Methods
The inclusion and exclusion criteria for this non-systematic review were established prospectively. We included studies with a general, healthy population of adults (≥ 18 years) or children (< 18 years), including those with overweight or obesity. Studies that included adults or children with overweight or obesity who were specifically trying to lose weight were also included. We excluded studies in populations with diseases (including substance related and addictive disorders), in vitro and animal studies. The interventions and comparisons of interest included any type of structured exercise or comparisons between different physical activity levels. All chronic training studies had to have a minimum intervention duration of 4 weeks. The outcomes of interest included all psychometric measures of food reward obtained either directly (e.g., ratings or pleasantness or desire to eat) or indirectly (e.g., measure of the willingness to work to obtain a food, reaction time), as well as neuronal response to food cues measured by fMRI. We included unpublished and ongoing studies where relevant.
The search strategy for this review combined electronic searches and hand searching. To identify ongoing or completed, but unpublished trials, ClinicalTrials.gov was searched on 22 November 2019 and researchers known to be active in the topic were contacted. Limits were set to include all papers published in English or French after 2009, in healthy human samples. Authors were contacted for additional information if unclear or not reported in the manuscript.
Observational Studies of Physical Activity and Exercise
As shown in Table 1, only three studies have examined food reward differences in defined active and inactive groups [34, 35, 38]. Horner et al. [38] found that in the fed state, overall liking, and liking for high-fat savory, high-fat sweet, and low-fat sweet food measured by the LFPQ was lower in active compared with inactive men differing in BMI, and differences for foods overall and low-fat sweet foods remained after adjusting for differences in percentage body fat. In both fed and hungry states, active men had a greater implicit wanting for low-fat savory foods compared with inactive men, but only the differences in the fed state remained after adjusting for percentage body fat. Faster gastric emptying was found to be associated with greater liking for savory food and lower implicit wanting for high-fat food. Two studies in men and women differing in physical activity levels but matched for BMI (~ 23 kg/m2) found no differences in liking or wanting for high-fat relative to low-fat food in the hungry or fed states [34, 35].
In terms of correlational studies, objectively measured MVPA was found to be inversely associated with liking (r = − 0.25, p < 0.001) and wanting (r = − 0.27, p = 0.001) for high-fat relative to low-fat food measured by the LFPQ in 156 women across a range of BMI [41]. This is in line with a study showing that self-reported physical activity was associated with reduced fMRI responses to high-energy relative to low-energy food [39]. Another fMRI study in participants ranging in BMI found an inverse association between self-reported moderate-to-vigorous physical activity and brain response to food cues [40•]. This relationship appeared to be more prominent in the participants with obesity than the lean participants. In individuals with overweight/obesity and impaired fasting glucose and/or glucose tolerance, food compared with non-food brain activation was negatively associated with leisure-time physical activity [36•]. Interestingly, there was a positive association between brain activation and work-related physical activity, which lost statistical significance after adjusting for BMI and age, and there was no association with sport-related physical activity.
Overall, these studies suggest a reduction in food reward (both liking and wanting) with increasing levels of habitual physical activity, particularly at higher levels of adiposity. Those who accumulate more time in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity tend to prefer low-fat food and those who engage in more sedentary activity prefer high-fat food. These findings do not seem to be restricted to those who perform structured exercise. The inter-relationships between food reward, physical activity, and adiposity remain to be fully disentangled, as well as the mechanisms underlying these observed effects. One simple explanation could be that avoidance of high-fat food perceived to be unhealthy, preference for low-fat food and greater physical activity levels are all driven by a dispositional desire to be healthy and engage in a range of positive health behaviors [64, 65]. Alternatively, it has been proposed that physical activity may act as a reward “buffer” against liking and wanting for high-fat foods, whereas low levels of physical activity may render people more susceptible to hedonic eating [66, 67]. The possibility of stronger inverse associations between physical activity and food reward in lean groups versus groups with obesity suggests that exercise and leisure-time physical activity may be an effective strategy for controlling hedonic eating in people with obesity.
Acute Exercise Studies
At least eight studies have investigated the effect of acute exercise on food reward measured by the LFPQ which allows direct comparison of outcomes (Table 1). All these studies had a no-exercise control group except for Alkahtani et al. [42], but the exercise varied in intensity (low to high), modality (aerobic vs resistance; cycling vs swimming) or in muscle contraction type (eccentric vs concentric). Two studies showed a decrease in food reward after acute exercise compared with the sedentary control. McNeil and colleagues revealed a decrease in the preference for high-fat relative to low-fat food independently of the modality of exercise (aerobic or resistance) in inactive adults within the normal range of BMI [49]. Miguet and colleagues showed a decrease in implicit wanting for sweet relative to savory food in response to an ad libitum meal after a session of high intensity interval exercise in inactive adolescents with obesity [50]. Interestingly, no change in food intake was observed in the McNeil study whereas a decrease in total energy intake (both lunch and dinner) was noted in the Miguet study. This might be related to the fact that changes in implicit wanting are a greater driver of food intake than changes in liking. Also, the McNeil study might have been underpowered to detect a change in implicit wanting. Four studies showed no changes in food reward (fat or taste bias for liking, relative preference or implicit wanting). One study compared eccentric vs concentric exercise on moderately active men (BMI 23.4 ± 3.3 kg/m2) and showed no effect on either appetite sensations, appetite-related hormones, or food reward [43]. Three studies compared the intensity of exercise (low versus high [51] or high- or moderate-intensity intermittent cycling [42, 48]). Two reported no effects on food reward or food intake [48, 51] whereas one found a decrease in wanting and increase in liking after both high- and moderate-intensity exercise [42]. Of note, these studies were conducted in moderately active normal-weight adults [51] and inactive adults with overweight/obesity [42, 48]. A recently completed study compared bouts of swimming or cycling to a no-exercise control in a within-subjects design (Thackray et al. unpublished). While they found that energy intake was increased after swimming but not cycling compared with control, no differences in food reward were detected except for a tendency for a main effect of trial for implicit wanting fat bias with wanting being smaller after cycling relative to swimming and control. Lastly, Finlayson and colleagues demonstrated that implicit wanting was increased in response to moderate-intensity exercise only in those individuals that increased their energy intake relative to no exercise (i.e., compensators) [47]. Consequently, even with the same methodology to assess food reward, the response to acute exercise seems to be equivocal and subject to individual variability. This could be explained by methodological issues; even though the same tool is used, the studies were conducted in different countries (UK, Saudi Arabia, Canada, France, and Norway) and the food images used may not have been cross-culturally validated [68]. The sample sizes were relatively small for most of the studies (ranging from 12 to 33), including mainly both genders and with different ranges of BMI. However, it can be noticed that exercise seems to affect food reward more clearly in inactive individuals compared with active ones in both adolescents and adults. One tentative hypothesis could be that inactive individuals (within the non-regulated zone of the J-shape curve) would benefit more from acute exercise than active individuals for whom the appetite control system is more sensitive.
Farah and colleagues used a computer-based task to measure the effect of acute exercise on liking (visual analogue scale) and other non-homeostatic indicators (ideal portion size, food utility, hunger) [46]. They found that a 60-min moderate-intensity exercise bout reduced hunger and ideal portion size but not liking. This is in concordance with previous results showing that implicit wanting rather than liking might be influenced by exercise. However, the physical activity level of the participants was not reported, and implicit wanting was not measured.
Acute exercise has also been shown to have an effect on brain reward measured by BOLD response to food cues with fMRI. Evero and colleagues showed that a 60-min high-intensity exercise bout decreased the neuronal response to food cues in brain regions related to food reward, visual attention, and inhibitory control [45]. Interestingly, regions related to both liking (i.e., insula, orbitofrontal cortex) and wanting (i.e., putamen) were reduced after exercise. This is in line with another study that found that exercise increases neural responses in reward-related regions in response to images of low-calorie foods and suppresses activation during the viewing of high-calorie foods [44]. These central responses were associated with exercise-induced changes in peripheral signals related to appetite control and hydration status. However, liking and implicit wanting were not measured directly in these studies (i.e., behavioral measures) nor was food intake. Lastly, Saanikoji et al. found no effect of an acute moderate-intensity aerobic exercise on brain food reward in lean men [52]. However, they showed that individuals who increased the most in endogenous opioid release had the highest brain reward response after the exercise compared with the control. Consequently, the opioid system might contribute to explain some individual variability in the food reward responses to exercise.
Chronic Exercise Training Studies
As mentioned above, our recent systematic review on weight management interventions found limited evidence on the impact of exercise interventions on food reward [6••]. Among the included studies, two investigated the impact of high-intensity interval exercise compared with either moderate-intensity interval training [53] or continuous training [60]. Using a cross-over design, Alkahtani et al. [53] found that in response to acute exercise after a 4-week exercise intervention, liking for high-fat savory food seemed to increase after MIIT and decrease after HIIT in men with overweight or obesity (interaction p = 0.09). Another study in individuals with obesity found no changes in food reward in response to a breakfast meal (hungry and fed states) after a 12-week intervention of either high-intensity interval exercise (125 or 250 kcal, 3 days/week) or moderate-intensity continuous exercise (250 kcal, 3 days/week) [60]. In another study, no changes in liking or implicit wanting for high-fat relative to low-fat food in the hungry state were observed after a 12-week aerobic exercise intervention (500 kcal, 5 days/week), although a trend towards a reduction in wanting was noted [69]. However, more recent analyses with a non-exercising control group revealed that in response to a high-fat and high-carbohydrate fixed lunch (hungry and fed states), overall implicit wanting decreased after the 12-week exercise intervention, whereas no changes in explicit liking were found [54]. This is corroborated by another study that found a decrease in implicit wanting for high-fat relative to low-fat food in women who underwent a 3-month exercise intervention (300 kcal, 5 days/week) [62]. When food reward was measured in response to an exercise bout post-intervention in that study, there was also a decrease in liking for savory foods whereas liking for savory foods increase after acute exercise at baseline. Thus, it appears that chronic exercise training may modulate the food reward response to acute exercise in inactive individuals, also shown in an fMRI study [56].
Interestingly, changes in food reward in the study by Beaulieu et al. [54] were not associated with changes in body weight or composition, suggesting a potential independent effect of exercise. Indeed, an inverse association was found between changes in leptin (adjusted for percentage body fat) and changes in liking, but not wanting, for high-fat food [69]. Thus, leptin may have a role in mediating changes in food reward during exercise training in individuals with overweight/obesity. In contrast, a 6-month exercise intervention led to a reduction in the fasted neuronal response to food compared with non-food, and some of these changes were associated with changes in fat mass, body weight and leptin [56]. These findings are interesting in light of the seminal study by Rosenbaum [70], who reported that leptin replacement after > 10% weight loss using a liquid formula diet, modulated food cue-elicited neuronal activation in reward-related regions (consistent with wanting), but did not affect liking for the diet. Beyond leptin, it is known that weight loss also impacts fasting levels of ghrelin. For example, the RESOLVE study (NCT00917917) showed that long-term physical activity may reverse the early enhancing effect of body weight loss on plasma ghrelin [71]. Future studies should examine whether favorable effects of exercise-induced weight loss on food reward can be partly explained by modulation of ghrelin as well as leptin.
Differences in food preferences have also been found between individuals who compensated (less than expected weight loss based on median split) compared with non-compensators during a 6-month intervention expending either ~ 700 kcal/week or ~ 1760 kcal/week [58]. Compensators had reduced preference for low-fat and high-carbohydrate food relative to non-compensators. Indeed, the hedonic response to acute exercise also appears to impact weight loss outcomes during chronic exercise training [57]. We have shown that after acute exercise, liking for all foods and wanting for high-fat sweet food increased only in compensators to a 12-week exercise intervention (those who achieved less than expected weight loss), compared with no change in non-compensators (those achieving at or above expected weight loss). These differences were independent of the exercise intervention and weight loss [57] and add to other evidence of improved appetite control in this group [72]. In a further unpublished study from our laboratory where sub-groups of 12-week exercise intervention non-compensators and compensators were compared with a non-exercising control group (Fig. 1), we observed that prior to the exercise intervention, compensators showed a strong liking and wanting for high-fat food whereas non-compensators showed no difference between high-fat and low-fat food. Secondly, we found that the greater reward for high-fat food in compensators reduced following the exercise intervention, compared with no change in controls. Lastly, there appeared to be a unique pattern of change in liking and wanting in the non-compensators who showed a small increase in liking for high-fat food after exercise training, but a simultaneous decrease in wanting for high-fat food.
The impact of a 12-week aerobic exercise intervention (70% HRmax, 500 kcal/day, 5 days/week) on liking and implicit wanting for high-fat food in weight loss non-compensators (n = 15), compensators (n = 15), and non-exercising controls (n = 15) (Finlayson et al. unpublished). *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, #p = 0.08, §p = 0.10
In adolescents with obesity, eccentric cycling exercise as part of a 12-week inpatient multidisciplinary weight loss intervention increased the relative preference for high-fat food and increased both the relative preference and implicit wanting for savory food, whereas no changes were observed in response to concentric exercise training [63]. Another study in adolescents with obesity showed that during a 10-month inpatient multidisciplinary weight loss intervention including physical activity, liking for food in the hungry state increased from baseline to 5 months, then returned to baseline values at 10 months, whereas liking for food in the fed state decreased (Miguet et al., under review). There were no changes in wanting observed.
These studies are suggestive that chronic exercise improves food reward (reduced response to high-energy foods and increased response to low-energy foods). However, the effect sizes were relatively small and inter-individual variability tended to be large. Two studies found a reduction in implicit wanting for high-fat relative to low-fat foods after exercise training [54, 62]. This may be a result of a direct effect of exercise on brain regions related to food reward, as shown by the fMRI studies included in the current review, and others [73, 74]. Furthermore, as exercise affects cognition and executive function, it has been proposed that processes such as inhibitory control could have a moderating effect on wanting and modulate eating behavior [66].
Another two studies found an increase in liking after exercise training, which might be explained by concomitant improvements in homeostatic appetite control in these studies (a small increase in hunger or a reduction in fasting leptin concentrations). Individual differences in food reward appear to act as pre-existing moderators of the impact of exercise training on weight loss and suggest that those with healthier preferences or better satiety signaling at baseline appear to lose more weight with exercise. No clear evidence exists regarding the optimal mode, frequency, intensity, duration, and time of day for exercise to have the most impact on food reward. Further systematic research into these factors is warranted.
Conclusions
One of the perceived barriers for engaging in exercise is its potential to promote hedonic eating. Food reward plays an important role in weight management through its intervening status between the nutrient requirements of the body and hedonic inputs from the food environment that promote food intake. This review brings together current evidence from observational, acute, and chronic exercise training studies to inform public debate on the impact of physical activity on food reward. A conceptual model, building on previous theory [19••] is shown in Fig. 2. Observational studies show that performance of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity is associated with lower liking and wanting for high-fat or high-energy food, and higher liking for low-fat/low-energy food. These findings may reflect improved appetite control and are supported by evidence from chronic exercise training interventions. Where exercise training leads to successful weight loss, it appears to be accompanied by a dissociation between liking and wanting evidenced by a reduction in wanting for high-energy food but increase in liking for low-energy food. Acute bouts of exercise tend to only impact behavioral indices of food reward in less active individuals or those with poor appetite control, where it tends to result in reduced food reward. These findings are corroborated by observational studies that demonstrate greater liking and especially wanting for high-energy foods (and greater susceptibility to food cravings) in inactive individuals. Food reward does not counteract the benefit of physical activity for obesity management. Rather, exercise appears to accompany positive changes in food preferences in line with improvements in appetite control.
Conceptual model of the impact of habitual physical activity and exercise on appetite control. The model builds upon the relationship between physical activity level, energy intake, and body fat recently demonstrated by Beaulieu et al. [19••]. Higher levels of physical activity are associated with enhanced satiety signaling—within a “regulated zone” of appetite control—resulting in a better matching between energy intake and energy expenditure. Lower levels of physical activity are associated with higher body fat, weaker satiety signaling, and greater responsiveness to hedonic inputs from the food environment—within a “non-regulated zone” of appetite control—allowing for overconsumption to occur and body fat to increase further. This review adds to this model by proposing effects of physical activity on liking and wanting as processes of food reward. Specifically, lower levels of physical activity are associated with greater liking and wanting for high-fat/high-energy food, with wanting as the stronger driver of excess energy intake. Acute exercise leads to a reduction in liking and wanting, especially in inactive individuals. As habitual levels of physical activity increase (including during chronic exercise interventions), there is a small increase in liking and decrease in wanting that accompany weight loss and improvement in appetite control. Finally, higher levels of habitual physical activity (e.g., regular bouts of moderate-to-vigorous physical activity) are associated with greater liking for low-fat/low-energy food and lower wanting for high-fat food
References
Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been highlighted as: • Of importance •• Of major importance
Guess N. A qualitative investigation of attitudes towards aerobic and resistance exercise amongst overweight and obese individuals. BMC Res Notes. 2012;5:191–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/1756-0500-5-191.
Leone LA, Ward DS. A mixed methods comparison of perceived benefits and barriers to exercise between obese and nonobese women. J Phys Act Health. 2013;10(4):461–9.
Schreiber K. Why your workout leaves you so hungry you could eat a horse. Greatist. 2014. https://greatist.com/grow/when-exercise-makes-you-overeat#1.
Hill A. Exercise helps you diet - if it’s the right kind. The Guardian. 2009. https://www.theguardian.com/lifeandstyle/2009/feb/22/diet-exercise-appetite.
Thivel D, Finlayson G, Blundell JE. Homeostatic and neurocognitive control of energy intake in response to exercise in pediatric obesity: a psychobiological framework. Obes Rev. 2019;20(2):316–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12782.
Oustric P, Gibbons C, Beaulieu K, Blundell J, Finlayson G. Changes in food reward during weight management interventions - a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2018;19(12):1642–58. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12754. Systematic review on the effect of weight management interventions on food reward, highlighting the very limited evidence pertaining to the role of exercise-induced weight loss interventions on food reward.
Blundell JE, Gibbons C, Caudwell P, Finlayson G, Hopkins M. Appetite control and energy balance: impact of exercise. Obes Rev. 2015;16:67–76. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12257.
Horner KM, Schubert MM, Desbrow B, Byrne NM, King NA. Acute exercise and gastric emptying: a meta-analysis and implications for appetite control. Sports Med. 2015;45(5):659–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-014-0285-4.
Schubert MM, Sabapathy S, Leveritt M, Desbrow B. Acute exercise and hormones related to appetite regulation: a meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2014;44(3):387–403. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-013-0120-3.
Donnelly JE, Blair SN, Jakicic JM, Manore MM, Rankin JW, Smith BK, et al. Appropriate physical activity intervention strategies for weight loss and prevention of weight regain for adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2009;41(2):459–71. https://doi.org/10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181949333.
Shaw K, Gennat H, O'Rourke P, Del Mar C. Exercise for overweight or obesity. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2006;4:CD003817. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD003817.pub3.
Stiegler P, Cunliffe A. The role of diet and exercise for the maintenance of fat-free mass and resting metabolic rate during weight loss. Sports Med. 2006;36(3):239–62.
Goodyear LJ, Kahn BB. Exercise, glucose transport, and insulin sensitivity. Annu Rev Med. 1998;49:235–61. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.med.49.1.235.
Dyck DJ. Leptin sensitivity in skeletal muscle is modulated by diet and exercise. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2005;33. https://doi.org/10.1097/00003677-200510000-00007.
Steinberg GR, Smith AC, Wormald S, Malenfant P, Collier C, Dyck DJ. Endurance training partially reverses dietary-induced leptin resistance in rodent skeletal muscle. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab. 2004;286(1):E57–63. https://doi.org/10.1152/ajpendo.00302.2003.
Hopkins M, Duarte C, Beaulieu K, Finlayson G, Gibbons C, Johnstone AM, et al. Activity energy expenditure is an independent predictor of energy intake in humans. Int J Obes. 2019;43:1466–74. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41366-018-0308-6.
King NA, Caudwell PP, Hopkins M, Stubbs RJ, Naslund E, Blundell JE. Dual-process action of exercise on appetite control: increase in orexigenic drive but improvement in meal-induced satiety. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90(4):921–7. https://doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.2009.27706.
Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Blundell JE, Finlayson G. Does habitual physical activity increase the sensitivity of the appetite control system? A Systematic Review. Sports Med. 2016;46(12):1897–919. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40279-016-0518-9.
Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Blundell J, Finlayson G. Homeostatic and non-homeostatic appetite control along the spectrum of physical activity levels: An updated perspective. Physiol Behav. 2018;192:23–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.12.032. Narrative review on the impact of habitual physical activity on the mechanisms of appetite control that proposes an updated model of the J-shape relationship between physical activity level and energy intake, with body composition, satiety signaling and non-homeostatic factors playing a role.
Berthoud HR, Munzberg H, Morrison CD. Blaming the brain for obesity: integration of hedonic and homeostatic mechanisms. Gastroenterology. 2017;152(7):1728–38. https://doi.org/10.1053/j.gastro.2016.12.050.
Pool E, Sennwald V, Delplanque S, Brosch T, Sander D. Measuring wanting and liking from animals to humans: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;63:124–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.01.006.
Berridge KC, Robinson TE, Aldridge JW. Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2009;9(1):65–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014.
Dalton M, Finlayson G. Psychobiological examination of liking and wanting for fat and sweet taste in trait binge eating females. Physiol Behav. 2014;136:128–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.03.019.
French SA, Mitchell NR, Wolfson J, Finlayson G, Blundell JE, Jeffery RW. Questionnaire and laboratory measures of eating behavior. Associations with energy intake and BMI in a community sample of working adults. Appetite. 2014;72:50–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.09.020.
de Castro JM, Bellisle F, Dalix AM, Pearcey SM. Palatability and intake relationships in free-living humans. Characterization and independence of influence in North Americans. Physiol Behav. 2000;70(3–4):343–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0031-9384(00)00264-x.
Cox DN, Perry L, Moore PB, Vallis L, Mela DJ. Sensory and hedonic associations with macronutrient and energy intakes of lean and obese consumers. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 1999;23(4):403–10. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0800836.
de Araujo IE, Schatzker M, Small DM. Rethinking food reward. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011643.
Mela DJ. Eating for pleasure or just wanting to eat? Reconsidering sensory hedonic responses as a driver of obesity. Appetite. 2006;47(1):10–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2006.02.006.
Finlayson G, King N, Blundell J. The role of implicit wanting in relation to explicit liking and wanting for food: implications for appetite control. Appetite. 2008;50(1):120–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2007.06.007.
Dalton M, Finlayson G, Hill A, Blundell J. Preliminary validation and principal components analysis of the Control of Eating Questionnaire (CoEQ) for the experience of food craving. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2015;69(12):1313–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/ejcn.2015.57.
Hill AJ, Weaver CF, Blundell JE. Food craving, dietary restraint and mood. Appetite. 1991;17(3):187–97.
White MA, Whisenhunt BL, Williamson DA, Greenway FL, Netemeyer RG. Development and validation of the food-craving inventory. Obes Res. 2002;10(2):107–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2002.17.
Stoeckel LE, Kim J, Weller RE, Cox JE, Cook EW 3rd, Horwitz B. Effective connectivity of a reward network in obese women. Brain Res Bull. 2009;79(6):388–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2009.05.016.
Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Blundell JE, Finlayson G. Impact of physical activity level and dietary fat content on passive overconsumption of energy in non-obese adults. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2017;14(1):14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12966-017-0473-3.
Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Long C, Blundell JE, Finlayson G. High habitual physical activity improves acute energy compensation in nonobese adults. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2017;49(11):2268–75. https://doi.org/10.1249/mss.0000000000001368.
Drummen M, Dorenbos E, Vreugdenhil ACE, Raben A, Westerterp-Plantenga MS, Adam TC. Insulin resistance, weight, and behavioral variables as determinants of brain reactivity to food cues: a prevention of diabetes through lifestyle intervention and population studies in Europe and around the World - a PREVIEW study. Am J Clin Nutr. 2019;109(2):315–21. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/nqy252. Using data from the Horizon 2020 PREVIEW study the authors measured fasting glucose and glucose tolerance as potential mechanisms to explain food compared to non-food brain activation and physical activity levels.
Baecke JA, Burema J, Frijters JE. A short questionnaire for the measurement of habitual physical activity in epidemiological studies. Am J Clin Nutr. 1982;36(5):936–42. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/36.5.936.
Horner KM, Finlayson G, Byrne NM, King NA. Food reward in active compared to inactive men: roles for gastric emptying and body fat. Physiol Behav. 2016;160:43–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.04.009.
Killgore WD, Kipman M, Schwab ZJ, Tkachenko O, Preer L, Gogel H, et al. Physical exercise and brain responses to images of high-calorie food. Neuroreport. 2013;24(17):962–7. https://doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0000000000000029.
• Luo S, O'Connor SG, Belcher BR, Page KA. Effects of physical activity and sedentary behavior on brain response to high-calorie food cues in young adults. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2018;26(3):540–6. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.22107. Tested the interaction between BMI status and physical activity on neuronal activation to high calorie foods and showed that inverse associations were stonger in young adults with obesity.
Oustric P, Myers A, Gibbons C, Buckland N, Dalton M, Long C, et al. Are objectively measured free-living physical activity and sedentary behaviour associated with control over eating and food preferences in women? Appetite. 2018;123:465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2017.11.067.
Alkahtani SA, Byrne NM, Hills AP, King NA. Acute interval exercise intensity does not affect appetite and nutrient preferences in overweight and obese males. Asia Pac J Clin Nutr. 2014;23(2):232–8. https://doi.org/10.6133/apjcn.2014.23.2.07.
Alkahtani SA, Aldayel A, Hopkins M. Effects of acute eccentric exercise on appetite-related hormones and food preferences in men. Am J Mens Health. 2019;13(4):1557988319861587. https://doi.org/10.1177/1557988319861587.
Crabtree DR, Chambers ES, Hardwick RM, Blannin AK. The effects of high-intensity exercise on neural responses to images of food. Am J Clin Nutr. 2014;99(2):258–67.
Evero N, Hackett LC, Clark RD, Phelan S, Hagobian TA. Aerobic exercise reduces neuronal responses in food reward brain regions. J Appl Physiol. 2012;112(9):1612–9.
Farah NM, Brunstrom JM, Gill JM. Using a novel computer-based approach to assess the acute effects of exercise on appetite-related measures. Appetite. 2012;58(1):196–204.
Finlayson G, Bryant E, Blundell JE, King NA. Acute compensatory eating following exercise is associated with implicit hedonic wanting for food. Physiol Behav. 2009;97(1):62–7.
Martins C, Stensvold D, Finlayson G, Holst J, Wisloff U, Kulseng B, et al. Effect of moderate- and high-intensity acute exercise on appetite in obese individuals. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2015;47(1):40–8. https://doi.org/10.1249/mss.0000000000000372.
McNeil J, Cadieux S, Finlayson G, Blundell JE, Doucet E. The effects of a single bout of aerobic or resistance exercise on food reward. Appetite. 2015;84:264–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.018.
Miguet M, Fillon A, Khammassi M, Masurier J, Julian V, Pereira B, et al. Appetite, energy intake and food reward responses to an acute high intensity interval exercise in adolescents with obesity. Physiol Behav. 2018;195:90–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.07.018.
Thivel D, Fillon A, Genin PM, Miguet M, Khammassi M, Pereira B, et al. Satiety responsiveness but not food reward is modified in response to an acute bout of low versus high intensity exercise in healthy adults. Appetite. 2019;145:104500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2019.104500.
Saanijoki T, Nummenmaa L, Tuulari JJ, Tuominen L, Arponen E, Kalliokoski KK, et al. Aerobic exercise modulates anticipatory reward processing via the mu-opioid receptor system. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39(10):3972–83. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24224.
Alkahtani SA, Byrne NM, Hills AP, King NA. Interval training intensity affects energy intake compensation in obese men. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab. 2014;24(6):595–604.
Beaulieu K, Hopkins M, Gibbons C, Oustric P, Caudwell P, Blundell J, et al. Exercise training reduces reward for high-fat food in adults with overweight/obesity. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2019.
Beaulieu K. The influence of physical activity level on the sensitivity of the appetite control system. Leeds: University of Leeds; 2017.
Cornier MA, Melanson EL, Salzberg AK, Bechtell JL, Tregellas JR. The effects of exercise on the neuronal response to food cues. Physiol Behav. 2012;105(4):1028–34.
Finlayson G, Caudwell P, Gibbons C, Hopkins M, King N, Blundell JE. Low fat loss response after medium-term supervised exercise in obese is associated with exercise-induced increase in food reward. J Obes. 2011;2011:615624. https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/615624.
Martin CK, Johnson WD, Myers CA, Apolzan JW, Earnest CP, Thomas DM, et al. Effect of different doses of supervised exercise on food intake, metabolism, and non-exercise physical activity: the E-MECHANIC randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1093/ajcn/nqz054.
Geiselman PJ, Anderson AM, Dowdy ML, West DB, Redmann SM, Smith SR. Reliability and validity of a macronutrient self-selection paradigm and a food preference questionnaire. Physiol Behav. 1998;63(5):919–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00542-8.
Martins C, Aschehoug I, Ludviksen M, Holst J, Finlayson G, Wisloff U, et al. High-intensity interval training, appetite, and reward value of food in the obese. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2017;49(9):1851–8. https://doi.org/10.1249/mss.0000000000001296.
Martins C, Kazakova I, Ludviksen M, Mehus I, Wisloff U, Kulseng B, et al. High-intensity interval training and isocaloric moderate-intensity continuous training result in similar improvements in body composition and fitness in obese individuals. Int J Sport Nutr Exerc Metab. 2016;26(3):197–204. https://doi.org/10.1123/ijsnem.2015-0078.
Riou ME, Jomphe-Tremblay S, Lamothe G, Finlayson GS, Blundell JE, Decarie-Spain L, et al. Energy compensation following a supervised exercise intervention in women living with overweight/obesity is accompanied by an early and sustained decrease in non-structured physical activity. Front Physiol. 2019;10:1048. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2019.01048.
Thivel D, Julian V, Miguet M, Pereira B, Beaulieu K, Finlayson G, et al. Introducing eccentric cycling during a multidisciplinary weight loss intervention might prevent adolescents with obesity from increasing their food intake: the TEXTOO study. Physiol Behav. 2019;112744. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.112744.
Pate RR, Heath GW, Dowda M, Trost SG. Associations between physical activity and other health behaviors in a representative sample of US adolescents. Am J Public Health. 1996;86(11):1577–81. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.86.11.1577.
Hart PD, Benavidez G, Erickson J. Meeting recommended levels of physical activity in relation to preventive health behavior and health status among adults. J Prev Med Public Health. 2017;50(1):10–7. https://doi.org/10.3961/jpmph.16.080.
Joseph RJ, Alonso-Alonso M, Bond DS, Pascual-Leone A, Blackburn GL. The neurocognitive connection between physical activity and eating behaviour. Obes Rev. 2011;12(10):800–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789X.2011.00893.x.
Annesi JJ, Porter KJ. Behavioural support of a proposed neurocognitive connection between physical activity and improved eating behaviour in obese women. Obes Res Clin Pract. 2014;8(4):e325–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.orcp.2013.08.001.
Oustric P, Thivel D, Dalton M, Beaulieu K, Gibbons C, Hopkins M, et al. Measuring food preference and reward: application and cross-cultural adaptation of the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire in human experimental research. Food Qual Prefer. 2020;80:103824. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2019.103824.
Hopkins M, Gibbons C, Caudwell P, Webb DL, Hellstrom PM, Naslund E, et al. Fasting leptin is a metabolic determinant of food reward in overweight and obese individuals during chronic aerobic exercise training. Int J Endocrinol. 2014;2014:323728. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/323728.
Rosenbaum M, Kissileff HR, Mayer LES, Hirsch J, Leibel RL. Energy intake in weight-reduced humans. Brain Res. 2010;1350:95–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.05.062.
Tremblay A, Dutheil F, Drapeau V, Metz L, Lesour B, Chapier R, et al. Long-term effects of high-intensity resistance and endurance exercise on plasma leptin and ghrelin in overweight individuals: the RESOLVE Study. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2019;44(11):1172–9. https://doi.org/10.1139/apnm-2019-0019.
Gibbons C, Blundell JE, Caudwell P, Webb DL, Hellstrom PM, Naslund E, et al. The role of episodic postprandial peptides in exercise-induced compensatory eating. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2017;102(11):4051–9. https://doi.org/10.1210/jc.2017-00817.
Legget KT, Wylie KP, Cornier MA, Melanson EL, Paschall CJ, Tregellas JR. Exercise-related changes in between-network connectivity in overweight/obese adults. Physiol Behav. 2016;158:60–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.02.031.
McFadden KL, Cornier MA, Melanson EL, Bechtell JL, Tregellas JR. Effects of exercise on resting-state default mode and salience network activity in overweight/obese adults. Neuroreport. 2013;24(15):866–71. https://doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0000000000000013.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent
This article does not contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of the authors.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
This article is part of the Topical Collection on Psychological Issues
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Beaulieu, K., Oustric, P. & Finlayson, G. The Impact of Physical Activity on Food Reward: Review and Conceptual Synthesis of Evidence from Observational, Acute, and Chronic Exercise Training Studies. Curr Obes Rep 9, 63–80 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13679-020-00372-3
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13679-020-00372-3
Keywords
- Physical activity
- Exercise
- Food reward
- Appetite
- Liking and wanting
- Obesity management