Dear Editor,

What is the molecular basis of memory formation? Many genes have been implicated in this process, including those involved in neural cell adhesion, mRNA transport, translation control, and cAMP-PKA signaling. Drosophila, with easy accessibility to genetic, molecular, and behavioral analyses, was also employed in olfactory learning studies and many key genes underlying memory formation were identified in these studies (Bellen et al., 2010). The majority of identified genes have been shown to be intensively expressed in the mushroom body (MB) of the Drosophila brain (Davis, 2005), including rut (adenylyl cyclase), DCO (the catalytic subunit of PKA), and AKAP Yu (Heisenberg, 2003; Davis, 2005; Lu et al., 2007). However, these genes are far from enough to understand memory formation comprehensively.

In order to identify more genes involved in memory formation, we previously generated 2,667 enhancer trap mutants, each of which contains a p-element (P{GawB}) insertion (Liu et al., 2008). The insertion includes a Gal4 sequence that can be used to label the expression pattern of the disrupted gene and 368 mutants were selected because their perturbed-gene expression enriched in the mushroom body. These selected mutants were then screened for 3 h memory performance in a well-defined olfactory conditioning paradigm, and one strain (No. 1021) was found defective. Plasmid rescue showed that the p-element of this mutant located in the first intron of the gene silver (svr) (Fig. 1A). Western blot analysis revealed that protein expression level of Svr was significantly decreased in this mutant (svr 1021, Fig. 1B).

Figure 1
figure 1

The memory formation of svr 1021 mutant is impaired and acute expression of svr + transgene in the svr 1021 mutant can rescue the impairment. (A) P element insertion sites of the svr gene in svr 1021 and svr KG02090 alleles. Boxes indicate exons and the lines represent introns. (B) Western blot analysis showed that Svr expression was greatly reduced in svr 1021 mutant (t-test, P < 0.05). n = 3 (C). Memory retention at 3 min and 3 h after one-cycle training and 24 h memory after spaced training were significantly decreased in svr 1021 mutant (t-test, P = 0.001, 0.004, 0.005 compared to w 1118 for 3 min, 3 h, 24 h after spaced training, respectively). n = 6–8. (D) Genetic complementation analysis established that disruption the svr gene was responsible for the memory defect. The 3-min memory in double-heterozygote (svr 1021/svr KG02090) was significantly lower than the other two heterozygous flies (svr 1021/+ or svr KG02090/+) or the control flies (ANOVA, P < 0.001). n = 6–8. (E) The 3-min memory defect in svr mutant was reversed by restoration of expression in svr 1021 labeled neuron after 3 day heat shock (ANOVA, Induced: P = 0.008 for svr 1021/Y compared to UAS-svr/+; GAL80 ts/+, P = 0.387 for svr 1021/Y; UAS-svr/+; GAL80 ts/+ compared to UAS-svr/+; GAL80 ts/+; Uninduced, P = 0.013 for svr 1021/Y compared to UAS-svr/+; GAL80 ts/+, P = 0.003 for svr 1021/Y; UAS-svr/+; GAL80 ts/+ compared to UAS-svr/+; GAL80 ts/+). n = 6–7. Data are shown as the mean ± SEM *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001

Svr has been reported to involve in viability and behaviors such as cold and ethanol sensitivity, as well as long-term memory in courtship behavior (Sidyelyeva et al., 2010). To explore the function of Svr in olfactory memory formation, we tested the performance index of svr 1021 in different time points after training. Memory of the svr mutant (svr 1021) exhibited a significant impairment at both 3 min and 3 h after one-cycle training (Fig. 1C). And 24 h memory of the mutant was also disrupted in svr 1021 mutant after spaced training (Fig. 1C). The memory defect was unlikely to be caused by deficiency in sensorimotor system since no abnormality in shock or odor avoidance was observed (Table S1). Finally, in order to confirm that the 3 min memory defect is not a result of random background mutation, we conducted a genetic complementation experiment, using svr 1021 and another independent P-element insertion allele, svr KG02090. Indeed, although both heterozygote mutants (svr 1021/+ and svr KG02090/+) had normal 3 min memory, such memory was impaired in double-heterozygote mutant (svr 1021/svr KG02090) (Fig. 1D).

One previous study showed that svr is involved in Drosophila development (Sidyelyeva et al., 2010). To figure out whether the disrupted memory formation of silver mutant resulted from the abnormal development or the interference of physiological process of neural system, we acutely manipulated the expression of svr + transgene in adult flies with the TARGET system. In this system, the Gal4-induced expression is suppressed by a ubiquitously expressed Gal80ts protein at the permissive temperature (18°C), but not at the restrictive temperature (30°C). The gene svr contains three carboxypeptidase domains, and has five endogenous transcriptional forms as a result of alternative splicing (Sidyelyeva et al., 2010). Previous findings suggested that the functions of CPD domain 1 and 2 are largely redundant, and the inactive CPD domain 3 is required for fully rescuing the mutant’s phenotype (Sidyelyeva et al., 2010). As a result, we used a longer form containing all three CPD domains (UAS::svr1B-2-3-t1 construct) to restore memory (Sidyelyeva et al., 2010). Acute expression of svr transgene in svr-labeled-neurons of svr mutant flies (svr 1021/Y; UAS-svr/+; Gal80 ts/+) effectively rescued the perturbed memory in svr mutant to a comparable level in the control group (UAS-svr/+; Gal80 ts/+) (left panel, Fig. 1E). On the other hand, no significant difference between svr acute expression group (svr 1021/Y; UAS-svr/+; Gal80 ts/+) and svr mutant group (svr 1021/Y) was detected in un-induced conditions (right panel, Fig. 1E). These results suggest that Svr interferes the physiological process of memory formation.

Next, in order to find out the functioning neural circuit of Svr in fly brain, we visualized the Gal4 expression pattern of svr 1021 using GFP labeling. Confocal imaging of the GFP signal in svr 1021/+; UAS-mCD8::GFP/+ revealed a preferential expression in two major compartments: the MB and a small group of neurosecretory cells located in the dorsal/medial region of the fly brain (Fig. 2A and 2B). Other svr enhancer trap lines svr NP2073 and svr NP3600 showed similar expression patterns to svr 1021 (Fig. 2A). And the memory formation of these svr-GAL4 mutants was also impaired (Fig. S1). Considering the fact that insulin-producing cells (IPCs) overlap with this cluster of neurosecretory cells (Nässel et al., 2013), we proposed that both the MB and IPC could be candidate brain areas where svr affects memory formation. The GAL4-UAS binary system was used to specifically express svr + transgene in these cells of interest. We took the OK107-Gal4 to cover all mushroom body neurons, and dilp2-Gal4 to cover the IPCs (Nässel et al., 2013). Subsequent behavioral assays showed that specific expression of svr in the IPCs (svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+; dilp2-Gal4/+) rescued the memory deficiency in svr mutant (svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+, Fig. 2D). However, svr expression in the MB (svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+; OK107-Gal4/+), was not able to generate similar rescuing effect (Fig. 2C). Thus, to our surprise, IPCs rather than MB are the crucial region where Svr influences memory formation.

Figure 2
figure 2

Restricted expression of svr + in the insulin-producing cells is sufficient to rescue the svr memory formation defect and insulin signaling is involved in svr regulated memory formation. (A) The confocal imaging of three types of svr-Gal4 driven GFP expression in the adult brain revealed preferential expression in the MB and clusters of median neurosecretory cells. Scale bar is 100 μm. (B) Enlarged views of anterior, middle, and posterior section of svr 1021 -Gal4 driven GFP expression. Arrow indicates IPCs. Scale bar is 50 μm. (C and D) Immediate memory defect in svr KG02090 mutant was rescued by inducing svr + transgene in IPCs (dilp-Gal4 driven) (ANOVA, P = 0.0007 for svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+; dilp2-Gal4/+ compared to svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+, P = 0.857 for svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+; dilp2-Gal4/+ compared to dilp2-Gal4/+), but not by inducing svr + transgene in MB (OK107-Gal4 driven) (ANOVA, P = 0.426 for svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+; OK107-Gal4/+ compared to svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+, P < 0.0001 for svr KG02090/Y; UAS-svr/+; OK107-Gal4/+ compared to OK107-Gal4/+). n = 6–7. For the X chromosome-located svr mutant, only male flies results were shown. (E) Expressing constitutively active InR (UAS-InR del) in nervous system partially reversed the memory impairment in svr KG02090 (ANOVA, P = 0.016 for elav/+ compared to svr KG02090/Y; UAS-InR del/+; elav/+, P = 0.023 for svr KG02090/Y; elav/+ compared to svr KG02090/Y; UAS-InR del/+; elav/+). n = 6–7. Data are shown as the mean ± SEM. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001

Drosophila IPCs are functionally similar to mammalian pancreatic islet β cells, because they produce several kinds of Drosophila insulin-like peptides (dilp1-dilp8), which have analogous functions to insulin (Nässel et al., 2013). Apart from the modulation of energy homeostasis, insulin and its receptors also participate in cognitive processes in the central nervous system (Babri et al., 2007). Previous studies showed that in both rats and humans, appropriate increase of insulin in specific brain areas can improve certain cognitive abilities, such as spatial memory (Benedict et al., 2004; Babri et al., 2007). In addition, in C. elegans, insulin/IGF-1 receptor mutant daf-2 has augmented short-term and long-term memory performance in early adulthood (Kauffman et al., 2010). All these findings suggest the participation of insulin pathway in memory formation, although the responsible mechanism has not been clarified. In the present study, Svr functioning in a restricted group of IPCs—dilp2 neurons to regulate memory formation implies that the potential connection between Svr and insulin pathway may be involved in memory formation (Fig. 2D).

Silver encodes the homolog of human carboxypeptidase D (CPD) in Drosophila. Carboxypeptidases and endopeptidases can turn precursors into peptides (Fricker, 2005). CPD is a member of the carboxypeptidase family and has a wide range of substrates, including growth factors, hormones, and neuropeptides (Skidgel and Erdos, 1998). Another member of the carboxypeptidase family, carboxypeptidase E (CPE) has been reported to cause proinsulin processing defect in its mice mutant (Naggert et al., 1995). Based on the fact that CPD and CPE share similar enzymatic properties and comparable distribution in the rat central neural system, CPD is speculated to be functionally redundant with CPE (Dong et al., 1999). Consequently, insulin processing is a potential substrate pathway of svr. The Drosophila insulin/insulin-like growth factor signaling (IIS) system is similar to its human counterpart. It comprises a single insulin receptor (InR) that mediates the function of all eight insulin-like peptides (ILPs), from dilp1 to dilp8 (Nässel et al., 2013). InRs are expressed ubiquitously, but the eight ILPs are expressed in specific tissues, presumably in response to different inputs. Therefore, we overexpressed a constitutively active InR to increase the insulin signal in the pan-neural system of the svr mutant. We found that the expression of constitutively active InR (svr KG02090/Y; UAS-InR del/+; elav/+) partially rescued the memory impairment in svr mutant (svr KG02090, Fig. 2E).

All our findings suggest that Svr regulates the memory formation via insulin pathway in neurosecretory cells outside MB.