Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Boredom and Cognitive Engagement: A Functional Theory of Boredom

  • Published:
Review of Philosophy and Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The functional theory of boredom maintains that boredom ought to be defined in terms of its role in our mental and behavioral economy. Although the functional theory has recently received considerable attention, presentations of this theory have not specified with sufficient precision either its commitments or its consequences for the ontology of boredom. This essay offers an in-depth examination of the functional theory. It explains what boredom is according to the functional view; it shows how the functional theory can account for the known characteristics of boredom; and it articulates the theory’s basic commitments, virtues, and limitations. Ultimately, by furthering our understanding of the functional theory of boredom, the essay contributes to a better theoretical grounding of boredom.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The psychodynamic theory has fallen out of fashion. However, recent work on boredom has emphasized the importance of a volitional component in the experience of boredom and argued that the presence of an unfulfilled or ill-formed desire during the experience of boredom reveals a disruption of agency that is characteristic of the experience of boredom (Danckert and Eastwood 2020; Eastwood and Gorelik 2021).

  2. Even though the MAC Model combines insights from both the existential and cognitive (specifically, attentional) theories of boredom, I am inclined to count it as a distinct account of boredom. This is for two reasons. First, it holds that attentional difficulties and perceived meaninglessness are individually sufficient but not necessary for the experience of boredom. This commitment on behalf of the MAC Model distinguishes it from attentional and existential views: those views insist, respectively, that attentional difficulties and perceived meaninglessness are necessary for the experience of boredom (for a discussion of the differences between the MAC Model and the attentional view, see Tam et. 2021). Second, and perhaps more importantly, the MAC Model entails that, depending on its causes and outcomes, there can be distinct kinds of boredom: attentional boredom, meaningless boredom, and mixed boredom. On the issue of whether there are distinct kinds of boredom, see Elpidorou (2021) and Goetz et al. (2014).

  3. Throughout the essay I consider boredom to be an emotion. Such treatment is in line with common practice within the psychology of boredom. Although I believe that there are good reasons to treat boredom as an emotion, it is not necessary to rehearse them here. The characterization of the functional view of boredom that I advance in this paper holds regardless of whether state boredom is ultimately an emotion, a mood, a cognitive attitude (Yao 2021), a feeling (Eastwood and Gorelik 2019), or some other kind of psychological state. Still, readers interested in arguments in support of the view that boredom is an emotion should consult Bortolotti and Allifi (2021); Elpidorou (2018b), (2022); Van Tilburg & Igou (2017a); and Yucel and Westgate (2021).

  4. Danckert (2019) offers a different account of boredom’s evolutionary origins. He argues that boredom arose as a signal to balance the drives of exploration and exploitation. See also Gomez-Ramirez & Costa (2017).

  5. For comedy routines, see, e.g., James Acaster’s Kettering Town FC routine or Kurt Braunohler and Kristen Schaal’s Kristen Schall is a Horse sketch, both of which rely on repetition in order to provoke laughter. Many thanks to an anonymous reviewer for helpful comments on the information-processing view of boredom.

References

  • Acee, T. W., H. Kim, H. J. Kim, J. I. Kim, H. N. R. Chu, M. Kim, … and Boredom Research Group. 2010. Academic boredom in under-and over-challenging situations. Contemporary Educational Psychology 35 (1): 17–27.

  • Adolphs, R., and D.J. Anderson. 2018. The neuroscience of emotion: A new synthesis. Princeton University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., and D. Andler. 2018. Investigating emotions as functional states distinct from feelings. Emotion Review 10 (3): 191–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ahmed, S.M.S. 1990. Psychometric properties of the boredom proneness scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills 71: 963–966.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Amsel, A. 1992. Frustration Theory: An analysis of dispositional learning and memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Barbalet, J.M. 1999. Boredom and social meaning. The British Journal of Sociology 50 (4): 631–646.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bargdill, R. 2000. The study of life boredom. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology 31 (2): 188–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barmack, J.E. 1937. Boredom and other factors in the physiology of mental effort: An exploratory study. Archives of Psychology 31: 9–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Belton, T., and E. Priyadharshini. 2007. Boredom and schooling: A cross-disciplinary exploration. Cambridge Journal of Education 37 (4): 579–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bench, S.W., and H.C. Lench. 2013. On the function of boredom. Behavioral Sciences 3 (3): 459–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bench, S.W., and H.C. Lench. 2019. Boredom as a seeking state: Boredom prompts the pursuit of novel (even negative) experiences. Emotion 19 (2): 242–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berlyne, D.E. 1960. Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, H.E. 1975. Boredom and the ready-made life. Social Research 42: 512–537.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bortolotti, L., and M. Aliffi. 2021. The epistemic benefits of irrational boredom. In In The moral psychology of boredom, ed. A. Elpidorou, 209–229. London: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boylan, J., P. Seli, A.A. Scholer, and J. Danckert. 2021. Boredom in the COVID-19 pandemic: Trait boredom proneness, the desire to act, and rule-breaking. Personality and Individual Differences 171: 110387.

  • Braby, C.D., D. Harris, and H.C. Muir. 1993. A psychophysiological approach to the assessment of work underload. Ergonomics 36: 1035–1042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bull, P. E. 1987. Posture gesture (International series in experimental social psychology, volume 16). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

  • Burn, C.C. 2017. Bestial boredom: A biological perspective on animal boredom and suggestions for its scientific investigation. Animal Behaviour 130: 141–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caldwell, L.L., N. Darling, L.L. Payne, and B. Dowdy. 1999. “Why are you bored?”: An examination of psychological and social control causes of boredom among adolescents. Journal of Leisure Research 31 (2): 103–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carriere, J.S., J.A. Cheyne, and D. Smilek. 2008. Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: The affective consequences of mindlessness. Consciousness and Cognition 17 (3): 835–847.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C.S., and M.F. Scheier. 1990. Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psychological Review 97 (1): 19–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.1.19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carver, C.S., and M.F. Scheier. 2013. Goals and emotion. In Guilford handbook of cognition and emotion, ed. M.D. Robinson, E.R. Watkins, and E. Harmon-Jones, 176–194. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chan, C.S., W.A. van Tilburg, E.R. Igou, C.Y. Poon, K.Y. Tam, V.U. Wong, and S.K. Cheung. 2018. Situational meaninglessness and state boredom: Cross-sectional and experience-sampling findings. Motivation and Emotion 42 (4): 555–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheyne, J.A., J.S.A. Carriere, and D. Smilek. 2006. Absent-mindedness: Lapses of conscious awareness and everyday cognitive failures. Consciousness and Cognition 15 (3): 578–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chin, A., A. Markey, S. Bhargava, K.S. Kassam, and G. Loewenstein. 2017. Bored in the USA: Experience sampling and boredom in everyday life. Emotion 17 (2): 359–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides, L., and J. Tooby. 2000. Evolutionary Psychology and the Emotions. In Handbook of Emotions, 2nd ed., ed. M. Lewis and J.M. Haviland-Jones, 91–115. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Craig, S.D., S. D’Mello, A. Witherspoon, and A. Graesser. 2008. Emote aloud during learning with AutoTutor: Applying the facial action coding system to cognitive–affective states during learning. COgnition and Emotion 22 (5): 777–788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. 1975. Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damrad-Frye, R., and J.D. Laird. 1989. The experience of boredom: The role of the self-perception of attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57: 315–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J. 2019. Boredom: Managing the delicate balance between exploration and exploitation. In Boredom is in your mind, ed. J. Ros Velasco, 37–53. Cham: Springer.

  • Danckert, J. 2021. “Rage spread thin”: Boredom and aggression. In The moral psychology of boredom, ed. A. Elpidorou, 97-109. London: Rowman & Littlefield.

  • Danckert, J.A., and A.A.A. Allman. 2005. Time flies when you’re having fun: Temporal estimation and the experience of boredom. Brain and Cognition 59 (3): 236–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J., and C. Merrifield. 2016. Boredom, sustained attention and the default mode network. Experimental Brain Research, 1–12.

  • Danckert, J., and J.D. Eastwood. 2020. Out of My Skull: The Psychology of Boredom. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert, J., T. Hammerschmidt, J. Marty-Dugas, and D. Smilek. 2018a. Boredom: Under-aroused and restless. Consciousness and Cognition 61: 24–37.

  • Danckert, J., J. Mugon, A. Struk, and J. Eastwood. 2018b. Boredom: What is it good for? In The function of emotions, ed. H. Lench, 93–119. Cham, Switzerland: Springer.

  • Daniels, L.M., V.M. Tze, and T. Goetz. 2015. Examining boredom: Different causes for different coping profiles. Learning and Individual Differences 37: 255–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dal Mas, D.E., and B.C. Wittmann. 2017. Avoiding boredom: Caudate and insula activity reflects boredom-elicited purchase bias. Cortex 92: 57–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Daschmann, E.C., T. Götz, and R.H. Stupnisky. 2011. Testing the predictors of boredom at school: Development and validation of the precursors to boredom scales. British Journal of Educational Psychology 81 (3): 421–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Chenne, T.K. 1988. Boredom as a clinical issue. Psychotherapy: Theory. Research, Practice, Training 25: 71–81.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J.D., A. Frischen, M.J. Fenske, and D. Smilek. 2012. The unengaged mind: Defining boredom in terms of attention. Perspectives on Psychological Science 7 (5): 482–495.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eastwood, J. D., and D. Gorelik. 2019. Boredom is a feeling of thinking and a double-edged sword. In Boredom is in your mind, ed. J. Ros Velasco, 55–70. Cham: Springer.

  • Eastwood, J. D., and D. Gorelik. 2021. Losing and finding agency: The crisis of boredom. In The moral psychology of boredom, ed. A. Elpidorou, 111-131. London: Rowman & Littlefield.

  • Eccleston, C., and G. Crombez. 1999. Pain demands attention: A cognitive–affective model of the interruptive function of pain. Psychological Bulletin 125 (3): 356–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. 2014. The bright side of boredom. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. 2016. The significance of boredom: A sartrean reading. In Philosophy of mind and phenomenology: Conceptual and empirical approaches, ed. D. Dahlstrom, A. Elpidorou, and W. Hopp, 268–283. New York: Routledge.

  • Elpidorou, A. 2018a. The good of boredom. Philosophical Psychology 31 (3): 323–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. 2018b. The bored mind is a guiding mind: Toward a regulatory theory of boredom. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences 17 (3): 455–484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. 2020. Propelled: How boredom, frustration, and anticipation lead us to the good life. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. 2021. Is boredom one or many? A functional solution to the problem of heterogeneity. Mind & Language 36 (1): 491–511. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elpidorou, A. 2022. Précis: Propelled: How boredom, frustration, and anticipation lead us to the good life. Journal of Philosophy of Emotion 3(2).

  • Fahlman, S.A., K.B. Mercer, P. Gaskovski, A.E. Eastwood, and J.D. Eastwood. 2009. Does a lack of life meaning cause bore- dom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology 28: 307–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fahlman, S.A., K.B. Mercer-Lynn, D.B. Flora, and J.D. Eastwood. 2013. Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment 20: 68–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fenichel, O. 1953. On the psychology of boredom. In The collected papers of Otto Fenichel, vol. 1, ed. O. Fenichel, 292–302. New York, NY: W.W. Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, C.D. 1993. Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Human Relations 46: 395–417.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, D.W., and S.R. Maddi. 1961. Functions of Varied Experience. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frankl, V.E. 1984. Man’s search for meaning: An introduction to logotherapy. New York, NY: Pocket Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N.H. 1986. The Emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frijda, N.H., and B. Mesquita. 1994. The social roles and functions of emotions. In Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence, ed. S. Kitayama and H.R. Markus, 51–87. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Geana, A., R. Wilson, N. Daw, and J. D. Cohen. 2016. Boredom, information-seeking and exploration. In Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (Vol. 1),ed. A. Papafragou, D. Mirman, D. Grodner, & J. Trueswell, 1751–1756. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

  • Geiwitz, P.J. 1966. Structure of boredom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 3: 592–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gana, K., G. Broc, and N. Bailly. 2019. Does the Boredom Proneness Scale capture traitness of boredom? Results from a six-year longitudinal trait-state-occasion model. Personality and Individual Differences 139 (1): 247–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giakoumis, D., A. Vogiannou, I. Kosunen, K. Moustakas, D. Tzovaras, and G. Hassapis. 2010. Identifying psychophysiological correlates of boredom and negative mood induced during HCI, 3–12. Valencia, Spain: BIOSTEC 2010.

  • Goetz, T., A.C. Frenzel, N.C. Hall, U.E. Nett, R. Pekrun, and A.A. Lipnevich. 2014. Types of boredom: An experience sampling approach. Motivation and Emotion 38 (3): 401–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldberg, Y.K., J.D. Eastwood, J. LaGuardia, and J. Danckert. 2011. Boredom: An emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 30 (6): 647–666.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, A., R. Wilkinson, A. McGown, and A. Jovanoska. 1997. The psychometric properties of the boredom proneness scale: An examination of its validity. Psychological Studies 42: 85–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greenson, R.R. 1953. On boredom. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association 1: 7–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grimm, J. 2008. Frustration aggression theory. The International Encyclopedia of Communication. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781405186407.wbiecf049.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D.O. 1966. A textbook of psychology. Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J.A. 1981. Attention, personality, and the self-regulation of mood: Absorbing interest and boredom. Progress in Experimental Personality Research 10: 281–315.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J.A., R.J. Haier, and M.S. Buchsbaum. 1984. Intrinsic enjoyment and boredom coping scales: Validation with personality, evoked potential, and attention measures. Personality and Individual Differences 5: 183–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M.B. 2000. Correlates and characteristics of boredom proneness and boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 30: 576–598.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartocollis, P. 1972. Time as a dimension of affects. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association 20: 92–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasselton, M.G., and T. Ketelaar. 2006. Irrational Emotions or Emotional Wisdom? The Evolutionary Psychology of Emotions and Behavior. In Affect in Social Thinking and Behavior, ed. J.P. Forgas, 21–39. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hill, A.B., and R.E. Perkins. 1985. Towards a model of boredom. British Journal of Psychology 76: 235–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoemann, K., Z. Khan, M. J. Feldman, C. Nielson, M. Devlin, J. Dy, ... and K. S. Quigley. 2020. Context-aware experience sampling reveals the scale of variation in affective experience. Scientific Reports 10(1): 1–16.

  • Hunter, A., and J.D. Eastwood. 2018. Does state boredom cause failures of attention? Examining the relations between trait boredom, state boredom, and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research 236 (9): 2483–2492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jeronimus, B.F., and O.M. Laceulle. 2017. Frustration. In Encyclopedia of personality and individual differences, ed. V. Zeigler Hill, and T. K. Shackelford. New York: Springer.

  • Johnstone, T., and K.R. Scherer. 2000. Vocal communication of emotion. In Handbook of Emotions, 2nd ed., ed. M. Lewis and J.M. Haviland-Jones, 220–235. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katahira, K., Y. Yamazaki, C. Yamaoka, H. Ozaki, S. Nakagawa, and N. Nagata. 2018. EEG correlates of the flow state: A combination of increased frontal theta and moderate frontocentral alpha rhythm in the mental arithmetic task. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., and J.J. Gross. 1999. Functional Accounts of Emotions. Cognition & Emotion 13 (5): 467–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., and J. Haidt. 1999. Social Functions of Emotions at Four Levels of Analysis. Cognition & Emotion 13 (5): 505–521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keltner, D., J. Haidt, and M. N. Shiota. 2006. Social functionalism and the evolution of emotions. Evolution and Social Psychology 115–142.

  • Kim, J., J. Seo, and T.H. Laine. 2018. Detecting boredom from eye gaze and EEG. Biomedical Signal Processing and Control 46: 302–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klapp, O. 1986. Overload and boredom: Essays on the quality of life in the information society. Connecticut: Greenwood Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E.H., G. Crombez, S. Van Damme, B. Verschuere, and J. De Houwer. 2004. Does imminent threat capture and hold attention? Emotions 4: 312–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreutzer, J.S., R.T. Seel, and E. Gourley. 2001. The prevalence and symptom rates of depression after traumatic brain injury: A comprehensive examination. Brain Injury 15 (7): 563–576.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroes, S. 2005. Detecting boredom in meetings, 1–5. Enschede: University of Twente.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A.W., E.P. Thompson, E.T. Higgins, M.N. Atash, A. Pierro, J.Y. Shah, and S. Spiegel. 2000. To “do the right thing” or to “just do it”: Locomotion and assessment as distinct self-regulatory imperatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79 (5): 793–815. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurzban, R., A. Duckworth, J.W. Kable, and J. Myers. 2013. An opportunity cost model of subjective effort and task performance. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (6): 661–679.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lench, H.C., S.W. Bench, K.E. Darbor, and M. Moore. 2015. A functionalist manifesto: Goal-related emotions from an evolutionary perspective. Emotion Review 7 (1): 90–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levenson, R.W. 1994. Human Emotions: A Functional View. In The Nature of Emotion: Fundamental Questions, ed. P. Ekman and R. Davidson, 123–126. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, J. 2018. Functionalism. In The stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Fall 2018 Edition), ed. E. N. Zalta. https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/fall2018/entries/functionalism.

  • Lewinsky, H. 1943. Boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology 13: 147–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lhommet, M., and S.C. Marsella. 2015. Expressing emotion through posture and gesture. In The Oxford handbook of affective computing, ed. R. Calvo, S. D’Mello, J. Gratch, and A. Kappas, 273–285. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindquist, K.A., T.D. Wager, H. Kober, E. Bliss-Moreau, and L. Feldman Barrett. 2012. The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 35: 121–143.

  • London, H., and L. Monello. 1974. Cognitive manipulation of bore- dom. In Thought and feeling, ed. H. London and R. Nisbett, 44–59. Chicago, IL: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • London, H., D.S. Schubert, and D. Washburn. 1972. Increase of autonomic arousal by boredom. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 80: 29–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lundberg, U., B. Melin, G.W. Evans, and L. Holmberg. 1993. Physiological deactivation after two contrasting tasks at a video display terminal: Learning vs. repetitive data entry. Ergonomics 36: 601–611.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maddi, S.R. 1970. The search for meaning. In The Nebraska symposium on motivation, ed. W.J. Arnold and M.M. Page, 134–183. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

  • Mann, S., and A. Robinson. 2009. Boredom in the lecture theatre: An investigation into the contributors, moderators, and outcomes of boredom amongst university students. British Educational Research Journal 35: 243–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malkovsky, E., C. Merrifield, Y. Goldberg, and J. Danckert. 2012. Exploring the relationship between boredom and sustained attention. Experimental Brain Research 221 (1): 59–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martarelli, C., W. Wolff, and M. Bieleke. 2021. Bored by bothering: A cost-value approach to pandemic boredom. Humanities & Social Sciences Communications 8: 218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, M., G. Sadlo, and G. Stew. 2006. The phenomenon of boredom. Qualitative Research in Psychology 3 (3): 193–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meagher, R.K., and G.J. Mason. 2012. Environmental enrichment reduces signs of boredom in caged mink. PLoS One 7(11): e49180.

  • Melton, A.M., and S.E. Schulenberg. 2009. A confirmatory factor analysis of the boredom proneness scale. The Journal of Psychology 143: 493–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mercer-Lynn, K.B., R.J. Bar, and J.D. Eastwood. 2014. Causes of boredom: The person, the situation, or both? Personality and Individual Differences 56: 122–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merrifield, C., and J. Danckert. 2014. Characterizing the psychophysiological signature of boredom. Experimental Brain Research 232 (2): 481–491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mikulas, W.L., and S.J. Vodanovich. 1993. The essence of boredom. Psychological Record 43: 3–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, C., and K. Christoff. 2018. Finding consistency in boredom by appreciating its instability. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 22 (9): 744–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mugon, J., A. Struk, and J. Danckert. 2018. A failure to launch: Regulatory modes and boredom proneness. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neander, K. 2017. Functional Analysis and the Species Design. Synthese 194 (4): 1147–1168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nederkoorn, C., L. Vancleef, A. Wilkenhöner, L. Claes, and R.C. Havermans. 2016. Self-inflicted pain out of boredom. Psychiatry Research 237: 127–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Niedenthal, P.M., and M. Brauer. 2012. Social Functionality of Human Emotion. Annual Review of Psychology 63: 259–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nisbet, R. 1982. Boredom. Commentary 74 (3): 48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oatley, K., and J.M. Jenkins. 1992. Human Emotions: Function and Dysfunction. Annual Review of Psychology 43 (1): 55–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oddy, M., M. Humphrey, and D. Uttley. 1978. Subjective impairment and social recovery after closed head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 41 (7): 611–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, W. 2014. Boredom. Analysis 74: 236–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hanlon, J.F. 1981. Boredom: Practical consequences and a theory. Acta Psychologica 49: 53–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oswald, I. 1962. The EEG of sleep. In Sleeping and waking: Physiology and psychology, ed. I. Oswald, 35–41. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Pattyn, N., X. Neyt, D. Henderickx, and E. Soetens. 2008. Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: Boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiology & Behavior 93: 369–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., T. Goetz, L.M. Daniels, R.H. Stupnisky, and R.P. Perry. 2010. Boredom in achievement settings: Exploring control-value antecedents and performance outcomes of a neglected emotion. Journal of Educational Psychology 102: 531–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pekrun, R., N.C. Hall, T. Goetz, and R.P. Perry. 2014. Boredom and academic achievement: Testing a model of reciprocal causation. Journal of Educational Psychology 106 (3): 696–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perone, S., E. H. Weybright, and A. J. Anderson. 2019. Over and over again: Changes in frontal EEG asymmetry across a boring task. Psychophysiology 56(10): e13427.

  • Pfattheicher, S., L.B. Lazarević, E.C. Westgate, and S. Schindler. 2020. On the relation of boredom and sadistic aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000335.

  • Polger, T.W. 2012. Functionalism as a philosophical theory of the cognitive sciences. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 3 (3): 337–348.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plutchik, R. 2001. The Nature of Emotions. American Scientist 89 (4): 344–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raffaelli, Q., C. Mills, and K. Christoff. 2018. The knowns and unknowns of boredom: A review of the literature. Experimental Brain Research 236 (9): 2451–2462.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ralph, B.C., K. Onderwater, D.R. Thomson, and D. Smilek. 2017. Disrupting monotony while increasing demand: Benefits of rest and intervening tasks on vigilance. Psychological Research Psychologische Forschung 81 (2): 432–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez-Ramirez, J., and T. Costa. 2017. Boredom begets creativity: A solution to the exploitation–exploration trade-off in predictive coding. Bio Systems 162: 168–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sander, D., D. Grandjean, and K.R. Scherer. 2018. An appraisal-driven componential approach to the emotional brain. Emotion Review 10 (3): 219–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scerbo, M.W. 1998. What’s so boring about vigilance? In Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember, ed. R. R. Hoffman, M. F. Sherrick, and J. S. Warm, 135–166. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

  • Scerbo, M.W., C.Q. Greenwald, and D.A. Sawin. 1992. Vigilance: It's boring, it's difficult, and I can't do anything about it. In Proceedings of the Human factors society annual mMeeting (Vol. 36, No. 18, pp. 1508–1512). Sage CA: Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.

  • Scherer, K.R. 2013. Nonlinguistic vocal indicators of emotion and psychopathology. In Emotions in Personality and Psychopathology, ed. C. Izard, 495–529. New York and London: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scherer, K.R., and H. Ellgring. 2007. Multimodal expression of emotion: Affect programs or componential appraisal patterns? Emotion 7 (1): 158–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaw, S.M., L.L. Caldwell, and D.A. Kleiber. 1996. Boredom, stress and social control in the daily activities of adolescents. Journal of Leisure Research 28 (4): 274–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, E.H., M.K. Sands, W. van den Noortgate, P. Condon, Y. Chang, J. Dy, and L.F. Barrett. 2018. Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categories. Psychological Bulletin 144 (4): 343–393. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000128.

  • Smith, A., and D. Page. 2015. U.S. smartphone use in 2015. PewResearch Internet Project. Retrieved July 03, 2020. https://www.pewresearch.org/internet/2015/04/01/us-smartphone-use-in-2015/

  • Steinberger, F., A. Moeller, and R. Schroeter. 2016. The antecedents, experience, and coping strategies of driver boredom in young adult males. Journal of Safety Research 59: 69–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A.A., J.S.A. Carriere, J.A. Cheyne, and J. Danckert. 2017. A short boredom proneness scale: Development and psychometric properties. Assessment 24 (3): 346–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A.A., A.A. Scholer, and J. Danckert. 2021. Perceptions of Control Influence Feelings of Boredom. Frontiers in Psychology 12: 2698.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A.A., A.A. Scholer, and J. Danckert. 2016. A self-regulatory approach to understanding boredom proneness. Cognition and Emotion 30 (8): 1388–1401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Struk, A.A., A.A. Scholer, J. Danckert, and P. Seli. 2020. Rich environments, dull experiences: How environment can exacerbate the effect of constraint on the experience of boredom. Cognition and Emotion 34 (7): 1517–1523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Svendsen, L. 2019. Animal boredom. In Boredom is in your mind. a shared psychological-philosophical approach, ed. J. Ros Velasco, 135–147. Cham: Springer.

  • Tabatabaie, A.F., M.R. Azadehfar, N. Mirian, M. Noroozian, A. Yoonessi, and A. Yoonessi. 2014. Neural correlates of boredom in music perception. Basic and Clinical Neuroscience 5: 259–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tam, K., A.P. Van Tilburg, C. Chan, E. R. Igou, and H. Lau. 2021. “Attention drifting in and out: The boredom feedback model.” Personality and Social Psychology Review, 25(3): 251–272.

  • Thackray, R.I., J.P. Bailey, and R.M. Touchstone. 1977. Physiological, subjective, and performance correlates of reported boredom and monotony while performing a simulated radar control task. In Vigilance: Theory, operational perfor- mance and physiological correlates, ed. R.R. Mackie, 203–216. New York, NY: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • The National Center on Addiction and Substance Use. (2003). Report on Teen Cigarette Smoking and Marijuana Use. Retrieved July 03, 2020. https://www.centeronaddiction.org/addiction-research/reports/report-teen-cigarette-smoking-and-marijuana-use

  • Todman, M. 2021. Boredom mismanagement and attributions of social and moral costs. In The moral psychology of boredom, ed. A. Elpidorou, 133–169. London: Rowman & Littlefield.

  • Todman, M. 2003a. Boredom and psychotic disorders: Cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes 66(2): 146–67.

  • Todman, M. 2003b. Boredom and psychotic disorders: Cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry 66: 146–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tooby, J., and L. Cosmides. 2008. The Evolutionary Psychology of the Emotions and their Relationship to Internal Regulatory Variables. In Handbook of Emotions, 3rd ed., ed. M. Lewis and J.M. Haviland-Jones, 103–129. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, M., J. Keller, K. Hoenig, C. Waller, and G. Grön. 2014. Neural correlates of experimentally induced flow experiences. NeuroImage 86: 194–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Damme, S., G. Crombez, and J. Lorenz. 2007. Pain draws visual attention to its location: Experimental evidence for a threat-related bias. Journal of Pain 8: 976–982.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Hooft, E. A., and M. L. van Hooff. 2018. The state of boredom: Frustrating or depressing? Motivation and Emotion, 1–16.

  • Van Tilburg, W.A., and E.R. Igou. 2011. On boredom and social identity: A pragmatic meaning-regulation approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 37: 1679–1691.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W.A., and E.R. Igou. 2012. On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motivation and Emotion 36 (2): 181–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W.A., and E.R. Igou. 2016. Going to political extremes in response to boredom. European Journal of Social Psychology 46 (6): 687–699.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W.A., and E.R. Igou. 2017a. Boredom begs to differ: Differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion 17 (2): 309–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W.A., and E.R. Igou. 2017b. Can boredom help? Increased prosocial intentions in response to boredom. Self and Identity 16 (1): 82–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Tilburg, W.A., E.R. Igou, and C. Sedikides. 2013. In search of meaningfulness: Nostalgia as an antidote to boredom. Emotion 13 (3): 450–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vodanovich, S.J. 2003. Psychometric measures of boredom: A review of the literature. The Journal of Psychology 137 (6): 569–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vogel-Walcutt, J.J., L. Fiorella, T. Carper, and S. Schatz. 2012. The definition, assessment, and mitigation of state boredom within educational settings: A comprehensive review. Educational Psychology Review 24 (1): 89–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallbott, H.G. 1998. Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Psychology 28: 879–896.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, J.C., S.J. Vodanovich, and B.M. Restino. 2003. Predicting cognitive failures from boredom proneness and daytime sleepiness scores: An investigation within military and undergraduate samples. Personality and Individual Differences 34 (4): 635–644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watt, J.D. 1991. Effect of boredom proneness on time perception. Psychological Reports 69 (1): 323–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wemelsfelder, F. 1985. Animal boredom: Is a scientific study of the subjective experiences of animals possible? In Advances in Animal Welfare Science 1984, ed. M.W. Fox and L.D. Mickley, 115–154. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wemelsfelder, F., 1991. Animal boredom: do animals miss being alert and active? In: M.C. Appleby, R.I. Horrell, J.C. Petherwick and SM. Rutter (Eds.), Applied Animal Behaviour: Past, Present and Future (pp. 120–123.). Potters Bar, UK: U.F.A.W.

  • Wemelsfelder, F. 2005. Animal boredom: Understanding the tedium of confined lives. In Mental Health and Well-Being in Animals, ed. F. McMillan, 77–91. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Westgate, E.C. 2020. Why Boredom is Interesting. Current Directions in Psychological Science 29 (1): 33–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Westgate, E.C., and T.D. Wilson. 2018. Boring thoughts and bored minds: The MAC model of boredom and cognitive engagement. Psychological Review 125 (5): 689–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, T.D., D.A. Reinhard, E.C. Westgate, D.T. Gilbert, N. Ellerbeck, C. Hahn, C.L. Brown, and A. Shaked. 2014. Social psychology. Just think: The challenges of the disengaged mind. Science 345: 75–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Witowska, J., S. Schmidt, and M. Wittmann. 2020. What happens while waiting? How self-regulation affects boredom and subjective time during a real waiting situation. Acta Psychologica, 205, 103061.

  • Wojtowicz, Z., N. Chater, and G. Loewenstein. 2021. Boredom and flow: An opportunity cost theory of attention-directing motivational states.

  • Wolff, W., and C. Martarelli. 2020. Bored into depletion? Towards a tentative integration of perceived self-control exertion and boredom as guiding signals for goal-directed behavior. Perspectives on Psychological Sciences. Advance Publication.

  • Wolff, W., C.S. Martarelli, J. Schüler, and M. Bieleke. 2020. High boredom proneness and low trait self-control impair adherence to social distancing guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 17 (15): 5420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wright, L. 1973. Functions. Philosophical Review 82 (2): 139–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wyatt, S., J.A. Fraser, and F.G. Stock. 1929. The Effects of Monotony in Work: A Preliminary Study. Industrial. Health Research Board 56: 47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yao, V. 2021. Boredom as a cognitive attitude. In The moral psychology of boredom, ed. A. Elpidorou, 231–249. London: Rowman & Littlefield.

  • Yakobi, O., and J. Danckert. 2021. Boredom proneness is associated with noisy decision-making, not risk-taking. Experimental Brain Research 239: 1807–1825.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yakobi, O., Boylan, J., & Danckert, J. 2021. Behavioral and electroencephalographic evidence for reduced attentional control and performance monitoring in boredom. Psychophysiology e13816.

  • Yu, R. 2016. The Neural Basis of Frustration State. In Neuroimaging Personality, Social Cognition, and Character, ed. J.R. Absher, 223–243. Amsterdam: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Zuckerman, M. 1979. Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yucel M., and E. C. Westgate. 2021. From electric shocks to the electoral college: how boredom steers moral behavior. In The moral psychology of boredom, ed. A. Elpidorou, 35–56. London: Rowman & Littlefield.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andreas Elpidorou.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Elpidorou, A. Boredom and Cognitive Engagement: A Functional Theory of Boredom. Rev.Phil.Psych. 14, 959–988 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00599-6

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00599-6

Navigation