Skip to main content
Log in

A differential equation model for the stage theory of color perception

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Japan Journal of Industrial and Applied Mathematics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We propose a novel differential equation model assuming the stage theory (trichromatic theory and opponent-process theory) for color inputs based on our previous model for light-dark inputs, which is an extension of the lateral inhibition model proposed by Peskin (Partial Differential Equations in Biology: Courant Institute of Mathematical Sciences Lecture Notes, New York, 1976). First, we consider the stationary problem in our novel model for color inputs and show that the output of our model can be described by a convolution integral. Moreover, we derive the necessary and sufficient conditions for the appearance of Mexican hat-type integral kernels in the outputs of our model for color inputs. Second, we demonstrate numerical results for simple color inputs and provide a theoretical prediction that the self-control mechanism exerted at horizontal cells, in conjunction with the opponent-colors (red-green, yellow-blue, and light-dark (or white-black)) plays an important role for the occurrence and non-occurrence of typical color contrasts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Adams, E.Q.: A theory of color vision. Psychol. Rev. 30, 56–76 (1923)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Adelson, E.H., Bergen, J.R.: Spatiotemporal energy models for the perception of motion. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 2, 284–299 (1985)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Alvarez, L., Lions, P.L., Morel, J.M.: Image selective smoothing and edge detection by nonlinear diffusion. II. SIAM J. Numer. Anal. 29, 845–866 (1992)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  4. Amari, S.: Dynamics of pattern formation in lateral-inhibition type neural fields. Biol. Cybernet. 27, 77–87 (1977)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  5. Anstis, S., Verstraten, F.A., Mather, G.: The motion aftereffect. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2, 111–117 (1998)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Asari, H., Meister, M.: The projective field of retinal bipolar cells and its modulation by visual context. Visual Context Neuron 81, 641–652 (2014)

    Google Scholar 

  7. Attwell, D.: The Sharpey–Schafer lecture ion channels and signal processing in the outer retina. Q. J. Exp. Physiol. 71, 496–536 (1986)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Benham, C.E.: The artificial spectrum top. Nature 51, 200 (1894)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Berestycki, H., Nadin, G., Perthame, B., Ryzhik, L.: The non-local Fisher-KPP equation: travelling waves and steady states. Nonlinearity 22, 2813–2844 (2009)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  10. Bloomfield, S.A., Völgyi, B.: The diverse functional roles and regulation of neuronal gap junctions in the retina. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 10, 495–506 (2009)

  11. Bowmaker, J.K., Dartnall, H.: Visual pigments of rods and cones in a human retina. J. Physiol. 298, 501–511 (1980)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Burr, D.C., Morrone, M.C.: Impulse-response functions for chromatic and achromatic stimuli. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A 10, 1706–1713 (1993)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Catté, F., Lions, P.L., Morel, J.M., Coll, T.: Image selective smoothing and edge detection by nonlinear diffusion. SIAM J. Numer. Anal. 29, 182–193 (1992)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  14. Chapot, C.A., Euler, T., Schubert, T.: How do horizontal cells ‘talk’ to cone photoreceptors? Different levels of complexity at the cone-horizontal cell synapse. J. Physiol. 595, 5495–5506 (2017)

  15. Courtney, S.M., Buchsbaum, G.: Temporal differences between color pathways within the retina as a possible origin of subjective colors. Vis. Res. 31, 1541–1548 (1991)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. De Valois, R.L., De Valois, K.K.: A multi-stage color model. Vis. Res. 33, 1053–1065 (1993)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Dekel, R., Sagi, D.: Tilt aftereffect due to adaptation to natural stimuli. Vis. Res. 117, 91–99 (2015)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Dowling, J.E.: The Retina: An Approachable Part of the Brain. Harvard University Press, Cambridge (1987)

    Google Scholar 

  19. Ei, S.I., Guo, J.S., Ishii, H., Wu, C.C.: Existence of traveling wave solutions to a nonlocal scalar equation with sign-changing kernel. J. Math. Anal. Appl. 487, 124007 (2020)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  20. Ei, S.I., Ishii, H., Kondo, S., Miura, T., Tanaka, Y.: Effective nonlocal kernels on reaction-diffusion networks. J. Theor. Biol. 509, 110496 (2021)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  21. Favreau, O.E.: Interocular transfer of color-contingent motion aftereffects: positive aftereffects. Vis. Res. 18, 841–844 (1978)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Fermüller, C., Ji, H., Kitaoka, A.: Illusory motion due to causal time filtering. Vis. Res. 50, 315–329 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Fu, X., et al.: Clinical applications of retinal gene therapies. Prec. Clin. Med. 1, 5–20 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Fujii, K., Matsuoka, A., Morita, T.: Analysis of the optical illusion by lateral inhibition (in Japanese). Jpn. J. Med. Electron. Biol. Eng. 5, 25–34 (1967)

    Google Scholar 

  25. Fuortes, M.G.F., Simon, E.J.: Interactions leading to horizontal cell responses in the turtle retina. J. Physiol. 240, 177–198 (1974)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Gibson, J.J.: Adaptation with negative after-effect. Psychol. Rev. 44, 222–244 (1937)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Hata, W., Motoyoshi, I.: Bidirectional aftereffects in perceived contrast. J. Vis. 18, 1–13 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Hayashi, Y., Ishii, S., Urakubo, H.: A computational model of afterimage rotation in the peripheral drift illusion based on retinal ON/OFF responses. PLoS One 9, e115464 (2014)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hurvich, L.M., Jameson, D.: An opponent-process theory of color vision. Psychol. Rev. 64, 384–404 (1957)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Kamiyama, Y., Wu, S.M., Usui, S.: Simulation analysis of bandpass filtering properties of a rod photoreceptor network. Vis. Res. 49, 970–978 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Kaneko, A.: The functional role of retinal horizontal cells. Jpn. J. Physiol. 37, 341–358 (1987)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Kaplan, D.M., Craver, C.F.: The explanatory force of dynamical and mathematical models in neuroscience: a mechanistic perspective. Philos. Sci. 78, 601–627 (2011)

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  33. Kaufman, J.H., May, J.G., Kunen, S.: Interocular transfer of orientation-contingent color aftereffects with external and internal adaptation. Percept. Psychophys. 30, 547–551 (1981)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Keener, J., Sneyd, J.: Mathematical Physiology. Springer, New York (1998)

    Book  MATH  Google Scholar 

  35. Kirschmann, A.: Ueber die quantitativen Verhältnisse des simultanen Helligkeits- und Farben-Contrastes. Philos. Stud. 6, 417–491 (1891)

    Google Scholar 

  36. Kitaoka, A.: A brief classification of colour illusions. Colour Design Creativ. 5, 1–9 (2010)

    Google Scholar 

  37. Koenderink, J., van Doorn, A., Witzel, C., Gegenfurtner, K.: Hues of color afterimages. Iperception 11, 1–18 (2020)

    Google Scholar 

  38. Kolb, H., et al.: Are there three types of horizontal cell in the human retina? J. Comp. Neurol. 343, 370–386 (1994)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Kondo, S.: An updated kernel-based Turing model for studying the mechanisms of biological pattern formation. J. Theor. Biol. 414, 120–127 (2017)

    Article  MathSciNet  Google Scholar 

  40. Koshlyakov, N.S., Gliner, E.B., Smirnov, M.M.: Partial Differential Equations of Mathematical Physics (1970)

  41. Krylov, N.V.: Lectures on Elliptic and Parabolic Equations in Sobolev Spaces. American Mathematical Society, Providence (2008)

    Book  MATH  Google Scholar 

  42. Kuffler, S.W.: Discharge patterns and functional organization of mammalian retina. J. Vis. 16, 37–68 (1953)

    Google Scholar 

  43. Laing, C., Troy, W.: Two-bump solutions of Amari-type models of neuronal pattern formation. Physica D 178, 190–218 (2003)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  44. Lee, B.B., Dacey, D.M., Smith, V.C., Pokorny, J.: Dynamics of sensitivity regulation in primate outer retina, the horizontal cell network. J. Vis. 3, 513–526 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. MacKay, D.M., Mackay, V.: Orientation-sensitive after-effects of dichoptically presented colour and form. Nature 242, 477–479 (1973)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Maheswaranathan, N., Kastner, D.B., Baccus, S.A., Ganguli, S.: Inferring hidden structure in multilayered neural circuits. PLoS Comput. Biol. 14, e1006291 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Masuda, O., Uchikawa, K.: Temporal integration of the chromatic channels in peripheral vision. Vis. Res. 49, 622–636 (2009)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. McCollough, C.: Color adaptation of edge-detectors in the human visual system. Science 149, 1115–1116 (1965)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. McLachlan, N.W.: Bessel Functions for Engineers, 2nd edn. Clarendon Press, Oxford (1955)

    MATH  Google Scholar 

  50. Mikaelian, H.H.: Interocular generalization of orientation specific color aftereffects. Vis. Res. 15, 661–663 (1975)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Ninomiya, H., Tanaka, Y., Yamamoto, H.: Reaction, diffusion and non-local interaction. J. Math. Biol. 75, 1203–1233 (2017)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  52. Ninomiya, H., Tanaka, Y., Yamamoto, H.: Reaction-diffusion approximation of nonlocal interactions using Jacobi polynomials. Jpn. J. Ind. Appl. Math. 35, 613–651 (2018)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  53. Nishida, S.Y., Sato, T.: Positive motion after-effect induced by bandpass-filtered random-dot kinematograms. Vis. Res. 32, 1635–1646 (1992)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Ohtsuka, T., Kouyama, N.: Electron microscopic study of synaptic contacts between photoreceptors and HRP-filled horizontal cells in the turtle retina. J. Comp. Neurol. 250, 141–156 (1986)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Ohtsuka, T., Kouyama, N.: Physiological and morphological studies of cone-horizontal cell connections in the turtle retina. Neurosci. Res. 4, 69–84 (1986)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Perona, P., Malik, J.: Scale-space and edge detection using anisotropic diffusion. IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell. 12, 629–639 (1990)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Peskin, C.S.: Partial Differential Equations in Biology: Courant Institute of Mathematical Sciences Lecture Notes, New York (1976)

  58. Raynauld, J.P., Wagner, H.J.: Goldfish retina: a correlate between cone activity and morphology of the horizontal cell in clone pedicules. Science 204, 1436–1438 (1979)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Reindl, A., Schubert, T., Strobach, T., Becker, C., Scholtz, G.: Adaptation aftereffects in the perception of crabs and lobsters as examples of complex natural objects. Front. Psychol. 9, 1905 (2018)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Rodieck, R.W., Stone, J.: Quantitative analysis of cat retinal ganglion cell response to visual stimuli. Vis. Res. 5, 583–601 (1965)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Rodieck, R.W.: Analysis of receptive fields of cat retinal ganglion cells. J. Neurophysiol. 28, 833–849 (1965)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Schrauf, M., Lingelbach, B., Wist, E.R.: The scintillating grid illusion. Vis. Res. 37, 1033–1038 (1997)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Schrödinger, E.: On the relationship of four-color theory to three-color theory. Translation by National Translation Center, commentary by Qasim Zaidi. Color Res. Appl. 19, 37–47 (1994)

    Google Scholar 

  64. Schwartz, S.: Visual Perception: A Clinical Orientation, 4th edn. McGraw-Hill Medical, New York (2009)

    Google Scholar 

  65. Schweinberger, S.R., Zäske, R., Walther, C., Golle, J., Kovács, G., Wiese, H.: Young without plastic surgery: perceptual adaptation to the age of female and male faces. Vis. Res. 50, 2570–2576 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Shimakura, N.: Elliptic partial differential operator (in Japanese). Kinokuniya Company LTD. (1978)

  67. Shinozaki, T., Wakabayashi, T., Kimura, M.: Partial Differential Equation and Green Function (in Japanese). Gendai Kougakusya Press, Tokyo (2002)

    Google Scholar 

  68. Stell, W.K., Lightfood, D.O.: Color-specific interconnections of cones and horizontal cells in the retina of the goldfish. J. Comp. Neurol. 159, 473–502 (1975)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Stell, W.K., Lightfood, D.O., Wheeler, T.G., Leeper, H.F.: Goldfish retina: functional polarization of cone horizontal cell dendrites and synapses. Science 190, 989–990 (1975)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Sushida, T., Kondo, S., Sugihara, K., Mimura, M.: A differential equation model of retinal processing for understanding lightness optical illusions. Jpn. J. Ind. Appl. Math. 35, 117–156 (2018)

    Article  MathSciNet  MATH  Google Scholar 

  71. Tomita, T., Kaneko, A., Murakami, M., Pautler, E.L.: Spectral response curves of single cones in the carp. Vis. Res. 7, 519–531 (1967)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Tsofe, A., Yucht, Y., Beyil, J., Einav, S., Spitzer, H.: Chromatic Vasarely effect. Vis. Res. 50, 2284–2294 (2010)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Twig, G., Levy, H., Perlman, I.: Color opponency in horizontal cells of the vertebrate retina. Prog. Retin. Eye Res. 22, 31–68 (2003)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Uchikawa, K., Shinomori, K.: Visual Perception I—Structure and Early Visual Processing of Visual System (in Japanese). Asakura Publishing, Tokyo (2007)

    Google Scholar 

  75. Vos, J.J., Walraven, P.L.: On the derivation of the foveal receptor primaries. Vis. Res. 11, 799–818 (1971)

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Watson, G.N.: A Treatise on the Theory of Bessel Functions, 2nd edn. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge (1952)

    Google Scholar 

  77. Wong, K.Y., Dowling, J.E.: Retinal bipolar cell input mechanisms in giant danio. III. ON–OFF bipolar cells and their color-opponent mechanisms. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 265–272 (2005)

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the reviewers for helpful comments and fruitful suggestions. This research was supported by a Startup Grant from the Keio Research Institute at SFC. This research was also partially supported by MEXT Private University Research Branding Project. Additionally, in this work, we used the computer of the MEXT Joint Usage / Research Center “Center for Mathematical Modeling and Applications”, Meiji University, Meiji Institute for Advanced Study of Mathematical Sciences (MIMS).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shintaro Kondo.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Appendices

1.1 Appendix A: Color contrasts

In Sect. 4, we focus on the input corresponding to Fig. 4g and show numerical results on the occurrence and non-occurrence of color contrasts, which are contingent on certain parameters. Furthermore, we also demonstrate numerical results that correspond to the other examples described in Fig. 4. The parameters of our model are specified in Table 1, \(\gamma ^j = 1.0\), and \(d_h^j = 100.0\). Figure 8 shows the inputs with parameter \(\ell = 10\).

Fig. 8
figure 8

Two-dimensional images of the input (49) with \(\ell = 10\). a \(({\hat{I}}^{L}, {\hat{I}}^{M}, {\hat{I}}^{S}) = (1,0,0)\). b \(({\hat{I}}^{L}, {\hat{I}}^{M}, {\hat{I}}^{S}) = (0,1,1)\). c \(({\hat{I}}^{L}, {\hat{I}}^{M}, {\hat{I}}^{S}) = (0,1,0)\). d \(({\hat{I}}^{L}, {\hat{I}}^{M}, {\hat{I}}^{S}) = (1,0,1)\). e \(({\hat{I}}^{L}, {\hat{I}}^{M}, {\hat{I}}^{S}) = (1,1,0)\)

Figure 9 shows numerical profiles of \(U^{rg}(x_1,L/2)\), \(U^{yb}(x_1,L/2)\), \(U^{ld}(x_1,L/2)\) for the input shown in Fig. 8. Similar to the above numerical results of Fig. 8a, we have \(U^{rg}(x_1,L/2) \not = 0\) or \(U^{yb}(x_1,L/2) \not =0\) in the region \({\hat{\varOmega }}\). That is, purely gray colors in \({\hat{\varOmega }}\) are depicted as the other colors.

Fig. 9
figure 9

ae Show profiles of \(U_p^{rg}(x_1,L/2)\), \(U_p^{yb}(x_1,L/2)\), and \(U^{ld}(x_1,L/2)\) for the input shown in Figs. 8a–e, respectively. Solid, dashed, and dotted lines indicate \(U_p^{yb}(x_1, L/2)\), \(U_p^{rg}(x_1, L/2)\), and \(U_p^{ld}(x_1, L/2)\), respectively

As shown in Fig. 9e, \(U^{rg}_p(L/2, L/2) = 0\) and \(U^{yb}_p(L/2, L/2) > 0\). This implies that the purely gray square that is surrounded by a blue color is perceived to be slightly yellow. Conversely, from the numerical result shown in Fig. 8e, \(U^{rg}_p(L/2, L/2) = 0\) and \(U^{yb}_p(L/2, L/2) < 0\). That is, the purely gray squares within a yellow region is perceived to be slightly blue.

On the other hand, in Fig. 9a–d, the signs of \(U^{rg}_p(L/2, L/2)\) and \(U^{yb}_p(L/2, L/2)\) are either the same or opposite. From the results shown in Fig. 9a, b, both signs of \(U^{rg}_p(L/2, L/2)\) and \(U^{yb}_p(L/2, L/2)\) are negative and positive, respectively. This implies that the purely gray is slightly depicted to be a mixed color of green-blue in the former case and a mixed color of red-yellow in the latter case. In particular, because we have \(|U^{rg}_p(L/2, L/2)| > |U^{yb}_p(L/2, L/2)|\) from Fig. 9a, b, it is conjectured that the influence of green and red is stronger than that of blue and yellow.

Also, Fig. 9c, d demonstrate \(U^{rg}_p(L/2, L/2)>0\), \(U^{yb}_p(L/2, L/2)<0\) and \(U^{rg}_p(L/2, L/2)<0\), \(U^{yb}_p(L/2, L/2)>0\), respectively. This implies that the purely gray squares is slightly perceived as a mixed color of red-blue in the former and mixed color of green-yellow in the latter case. Similar to what was described in Fig. 9a and b, c and d demonstrate \(|U^{rg}_p(L/2, L/2)| > |U^{yb}_p(L/2, L/2)|\), suggesting that the influence of red and green are stronger than that of blue and yellow. Thus, the above results are equivalent to the visual impressions observed in Fig. 4, supporting the ability of our model to explain typical color contrasts.

Moreover, we focus on the two-dimensional influence in our model output. Here, we provide an input (49) with \(\ell = 50\) and \(({\hat{I}}^{L}, {\hat{I}}^{M}, {\hat{I}}^{S}) = (0,0,1)\), as shown in Fig. 10a. Figure 10b shows a two-dimensional contour profile of the output \(U^{yb}_p\), which demonstrates that values of \(U_p^{yb}\) at the four corners marked by \(\bigstar\) are higher than ones on the boundary of a given purely gray rectangular region. This result is obtained by using the shape of two-dimensional corners and implies that our model can be used to analyze other contrast phenomena such as the Vasarely effect [72].

Fig. 10
figure 10

a A two-dimensional image of the input (49) with \(\ell = 50\) and \(({\hat{I}}^{L}, {\hat{I}}^{M}, {\hat{I}}^{S}) = (0,0,1)\). b A two-dimensional profile with contours of the output \(U_p^{yb}\). The marks \(\bigcirc\), \(\blacksquare\), \(\blacktriangle\), \(\bullet\), \(\blacklozenge\), \(\blacktriangledown\), and \(\bigstar\) indicate contour values \(-0.5\), \(-0.57\), \(-0.64\), 0, 0.07, 0.14, and 0.21, respectively

1.2 Appendix B: Opponent-colors induced by afterimages

We consider opponent-color induced by afterimages using two-dimensional color inputs in the region \(\varOmega\) shown in Fig. 11a. In the beginning, observers watch the red rectangular region closely amidst a on the green background, as shown in the left panel of Fig. 11a. After the red region and the green background are removed as shown in the right panel of Fig. 11a, the observers perceive opponent-colors for red and green. That is, an afterimage on the red region can be perceived as green, while an afterimage on the green region can be perceived as red. Here we give the following inputs \(I^i(t,x_1, x_2)\) (\(i=L, M, S\)) in order to discuss the occurrence and non-occurrence of afterimages observed in Fig. 11a:

$$\begin{aligned} I^L(t,x_1, x_2)&= {\left\{ \begin{array}{ll} {\hat{I}}(x_1, x_2), &{}\quad 0\le t \le T_0, \\ 1, &{}\quad T_0< t \le T_1, \end{array}\right. } \nonumber \\ I^M(t,x_1, x_2)&= {\left\{ \begin{array}{ll} 1 - {\hat{I}}(x_1, x_2), &{}\quad 0\le t \le T_0, \\ 1, &{}\quad T_0< t \le T_1, \end{array}\right. } \nonumber \\ I^S(t,x_1, x_2)&= {\left\{ \begin{array}{ll} 0, &{}\quad 0\le t \le T_0, \\ 1, &{}\quad T_0 < t \le T_1, \end{array}\right. } \end{aligned}$$
(50)

where \(T_0\) and \(T_1\) are the switching time of patterns and the finishing time for numerical computation, respectively. In our numerical simulations, we specify \(T_0 = 5.0\) and \(T_1 = 10.0\). \({\hat{I}}(x_1, x_2)\) is defined by

$$\begin{aligned} {\hat{I}}(x_1, x_2) = {\left\{ \begin{array}{ll} 1, &{}\quad (x_1, x_2) \in {\hat{\varOmega }}, \\ 0, &{}\quad (x_1, x_2) \in \varOmega \backslash {\hat{\varOmega }}, \end{array}\right. } \end{aligned}$$

where we specify the parameter \(\ell\) as \(\ell =50.0\) in Fig. 11a. We also use the parameters shown in Table 1, the control parameter \(\gamma ^j = 1.0\) (\(j=rg,yb,ld\)), and the diffusion coefficient \(d_h^j = 100.0\).

Fig. 11
figure 11

a A two-dimensional image of the input (50) with \(\ell = 50.0\) This image induces an afterimage (green or red) for opponent colors of red and green, respectively. bd One-dimensional profiles of the input \(I^{i}\) (\(i=L, M, S\)) for \(0 \le t \le T_0\) at the black line of a. A horizontal axis indicates the \(x_1\)-axis and a vertical axis indicates values of \(I^i\). eg Profiles \(u_h^j(t,x_1,L/2)\) \((j=rg, yb, ld)\) for \(t=5.0\) and \(t=6.0\), which are obtained as a numerical result for inputs \(I^{i}\) (\(i=L, M, S\)) of bd. Black arrows indicate directions for movement of profiles

Figure 11b–d are one-dimensional profiles of the input \(I^{i}\) (\(i=L, M, S\)) for \(t=0\) at the black line of Fig. 11a. Figure 11e–g show numerical results obtained as one-dimensional profile of \(u_p^j\) \((j=rb, yb, l d)\) for the input (50). From the profile of \(u_p^{rg} (t,x_1,L/2)\) for \(t=6.0\) in Fig. 11e, we can confirm that the opponent-colors (red-green) appear as an afterimage. On the other hand, the profiles of \(u_p^{yb} (t,x_1,L/2)\) and \(u_p^{l d} (t,x_1,L/2)\) for \(t=6.0\) in Fig. 11f, g have values close to 0. Therefore, the occurrence of opponent-colors induced by the afterimage can be explained using our model.

Finally, we show a dependency of the size of red regions and two parameters, \(\gamma ^j\) and \(d_h^j\). In order to evaluate opponent-color afterimages, we define the following index:

$$\begin{aligned} A_p^{rg} = \int _{T_0}^{T_1}u_p^{rg}(t, L/2, L/2) \mathrm{d}t \end{aligned}$$

Figure 12 shows profiles of \(A_p^{rg}\) for the input (50) with \(\ell \in [5.0, 50.0]\) in cases with three parameter pairs \((\gamma ^j, d_h^j) = (1.0, 100.0)\), (0.1, 100.0), and (1.0, 10.0). Since an opponent-color afterimage for the input (50) indicates green, the opponent-color afterimage appears when \(A_p^{rg}\) is negative. In the case of \(\gamma ^j=0.1\), we have \(A_p^{rg}>0\) for all \(\ell \in [5.0, 50.0]\). This implies that when the control parameter \(\gamma ^j\) is small, the opponent-color afterimage does not occur.

On the other hand, a profile of \(A_p^{rg}\) with \(d_h^j=10.0\) has negative values that are larger than the case of \(d_h^j = 100.0\). This implies that when the magnitude of diffusion coefficient \(d_h^j\) (the range of lateral inhibition) is decreased, the occurrence of opponent-color afterimage becomes remarkable. Therefore, our numerical results can predict that afterimages are easier to recognize in the central vision than in the peripheral vision because the fovea having an influence on the lateral inhibition that is weaker than the surrounding region of the retina.

Fig. 12
figure 12

Profiles of \(A_p^{rg}\). Solid, dashed, and dotted lines indicate profiles with the parameter pairs \((\gamma ^j, d_h^j) = (1.0, 100.0)\), (0.1, 100.0), and (1.0, 10.0), respectively

1.3 Appendix C: Relationship between our model equations (7), (8) and time evolution equations with convolution integrals

In the subject of neurophysiology, it has been known as experimental results that the receptive field structure of the bipolar cells and the retinal ganglion cells consist of the short-range activation and the long-range inhibition and its structure can be represented by the Mexican hat type function. After that, convolution integral models with the Mexican hat type kernel were proposed and the relation between inputs and outputs has been studied. Therefore, a major direction of mathematical modeling for retinal processing is to construct the convolution integral modeling.

In this section, we investigate the relationship between our model equations (7), (8) and time evolution equations with convolution integrals.

1.3.1 Stage 1

In the major direction of mathematical modeling for retinal processing, the convolution integral has been adopted as one of mathematical frameworks describing the blurring effect. A blurred output \(U_e = U_e(x)\) for an input I(x) \((x \in {\mathbb {R}}^{n}, \ n=1,2)\) is described by the following convolution integral

$$\begin{aligned} U_e(x) = \int _{{\mathbb {R}}^{n}} G(x-y) I(y) \mathrm{d}y = (G*I)(x), \quad x \in {\mathbb {R}}^{n}, \end{aligned}$$
(51)

where G(x) is often given by the Gaussian. Moreover, we can consider the following time evolution equation with a convolution integral:

$$\begin{aligned} \tau _e \frac{\partial u_e}{\partial t}= a \left( - u_e + (G*I)(x) \right) , \quad t>0, \ x \in {\mathbb {R}}^{n}, \end{aligned}$$
(52)

where \(u_e=u_e(t, {x})\) is a blurred output, \(\tau _e>0\) stands for the time constant. Here we assume that the shape of spatial-temporal receptive field structure of the photoreceptor cells does not depend on the time t.

First, we show that the stationary solution of the Eq. (7) is equivalent to (51) if we choose a special integral kernel. Let us consider the following integral kernel instead of the Gaussian:

$$\begin{aligned} G_i(x) = \left\{ \begin{array}{ll} \displaystyle \frac{k_i}{2} e^{- k_i |x|}, &{}\quad x\in {\mathbb {R}}^{1}, \\ \displaystyle \frac{k_i^2}{2 \pi } K_0 (k_i |x| ), &{}\quad x\in {\mathbb {R}}^{2} \end{array} \right. \end{aligned}$$
(53)

with the parameter \(k_1 = \sqrt{a/d_e}\) as the index \(i = 1\), where a and \(d_e\) are positive constants and \(K_0\) is a modified Bessel function of the second kind of the 0-th order. Then (51) with (53) is equivalent to the Eq. (46). See (A.14) of Appendix in our paper [70], and Lemma 1 of this paper.

Next we compare the numerical solution of the Eq. (7) with the exact solution of the Eq. (52) with \(G(x) = G_1(x)\) for \(n=1\). Here the initial condition is given by \(u_e(0,x) = 0\) for any \(x\in {\mathbb {R}}^{1}\), and the input is given by

$$\begin{aligned} I = I(x) = I_1 \quad \mathrm{for} \ x \le 0, \ \mathrm{and} \ I = I(x) = I_2 \quad \mathrm{for} \ x > 0, \end{aligned}$$
(54)

where \(I_1\) and \(I_2\) are constants. The exact solution of the Eq. (52) with \(G(x) = G_1(x)\) is obtained as follows. For \(t>0\), we have

$$\begin{aligned} \left\{ \begin{array}{ll} \displaystyle \tau _e \frac{ \partial u_e}{\partial t} = a \left( I_1 - \frac{I_1}{2} e^{k_1 x} + \frac{I_2}{2} e^{k_1 x} - u_e \right) , &{}\quad x \le 0, \\ \displaystyle \tau _e \frac{ \partial u_e}{\partial t} = a \left( I_2 + \frac{I_1}{2} e^{-k_1 x} - \frac{I_2}{2} e^{-k_1 x} - u_e \right) , &{}\quad x >0. \end{array} \right. \end{aligned}$$

Let \(\eta = - \frac{a}{\tau _e}\), then we have

$$\begin{aligned} \left\{ \begin{array}{ll} \displaystyle u_e(t,x) = (1 - e^{\eta t} ) \left( I_1 - \frac{I_1}{2} e^{k_1 x} + \frac{I_2}{2} e^{k_1 x} \right) , &{}\quad x \le 0, \\ \displaystyle u_e(t,x) = (1 - e^{\eta t} ) \left( I_2 + \frac{I_1}{2} e^{-k_1 x} - \frac{I_2}{2} e^{-k_1 x} \right) , &{}\quad x >0. \end{array} \right. \end{aligned}$$

From Fig. 13, we recognize that the numerical solution of the Eq. (7) is similar to the exact solution of the Eq. (52) with \(G(x) = G_1(x)\).

In the above discussion, we consider for the case of the integral kernel (53). On the other hand, recently, it has been shown in Theorem 2.6 [52] that for one-dimensional case, a finite linear combination of (53) gives an approximation to any even continuous function G(x). See also Corollary 1 [51] for the case of the periodic boundary condition. Therefore, it is reasonable to adopt the Eq. (7) as the model of the first stage.

Fig. 13
figure 13

a Numerical solution \(u_e\) of the Eq. (7). b The solution of the Eq. (52) with \(G(x) = G_1(x)\). Here \(t=0.1, 0.2, 2.0\), the initial condition is given by \(u_e(0,x) = 0\) for any \(x\in {\mathbb {R}}^{1}\), and and the input is given by (54)

1.3.2 Stage 2

In the second stage, we focus on the non-local transmission of inhibitory signal at the horizontal cells. It has been often considered non-local interactions as the following formulation by differential equation with convolution term: Let \(u_p=u_p(t, {x})\) be an output for the input \(u_e(t,x)\) of the second stage at the time \(t>0\) and the position \(x \in {\mathbb {R}}^{n}\). We consider the equation

$$\begin{aligned} \tau _p \frac{\partial u_p}{\partial t} = b(-u_p + u_e(t, {x})) - \gamma (G * \beta u_p )(x), \quad t>0, \ x\in {\mathbb {R}}^{n}, \end{aligned}$$
(55)

where \(\tau _p\) is a time constant and b, \(\beta\), \(\gamma\) are positive constants. If we specify the integral kernel \(G(x)= G_2(x)\) which is (53) with the parameter \(k_2 = \sqrt{c/d_h}\) \((c>0, d_h>0)\) as the index \(i = 2\), and put \(u_h = (G * \beta u_p )(x)\), then the Eq. (55) is written as the following system:

$$\begin{aligned} \left\{ \begin{array}{ll} \displaystyle \tau _p \frac{\partial u_p}{\partial t} = b(-u_p+u_e(t, {x})) - \gamma u_h, &{}\quad t>0, \ x\in {\mathbb {R}}^{n}, \\ \displaystyle 0 = d_h \varDelta u_h + \beta u_p - c u_h, &{}\quad t>0, \ x\in {\mathbb {R}}^{n}, \end{array} \right. \end{aligned}$$
(56)

which implies that the non-local transmission of inhibitory signal exerts instantaneously.

In the subject of neurophysiology, it is known that temporal responses of the S-potential differ in the cases of light adaptation and dark adaptation. Hence we gave a prediction that slow temporal response of the self-control mechanism is an important role for perceptions of afterimage illusions, and we consider the Eq. (8) as governing equations of the second stage.

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kondo, S., Mori, M. & Sushida, T. A differential equation model for the stage theory of color perception. Japan J. Indust. Appl. Math. 39, 283–318 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13160-021-00490-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Revised:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13160-021-00490-y

Keywords

Mathematics Subject Classification

Navigation