Mindfulness meditation (MM) is an umbrella term to describe types of meditation practice that train attention to aspects of the present moment without judgment (e.g. the breath, bodily sensations, awareness) (Crane et al., 2017; Van Dam et al., 2018). Over recent decades, MM has taught as part of mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs), which attempt to alleviate symptoms of depression, pain, and addiction (Hayes, 2012; Kuyken et al., 2008). Our understanding of the mechanisms of MM is rapidly improving, with studies replicating mechanistic relationships between mindful attention, emotional regulation, and well-being outcomes with moderate consistency (Britton et al., 2018; Chambers et al., 2009; Kiken et al., 2015). However, there are an array of theoretical perspectives regarding the neurophysiological mechanisms that underpin the effects of MM, and not enough empirical evidence to draw strong, comprehensive, or specific conclusions about the accuracy of the proposed mechanisms (Hölzel et al., 2011; Tang et al., 2015; Van Dam et al., 2018). A better mechanistic understanding of MM is thus required. Specifically, there is the need to elucidate the neurophysiological changes that underlie the benefits of the practice to well-being. This might allow the design of MM interventions with enhanced efficacy by specifically targeting the effective mechanism.

One promising psychological mechanism that may underlie the effects of MM could be improved attentional function (Kiken et al., 2015; Tang et al., 2007), with meta-analyses indicating that mindfulness meditation and mindfulness-based programs are associated with improved performance in a range of attention tasks (Sumantry & Stewart, 2021; Verhaeghen, 2021; Yakobi et al., 2021). Meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging research has also indicated the improved attention function is underpinned by altered neural activity in the default mode, salience, and executive attention networks of the brain (Ganesan et al., 2022). The suggestion that MM improves attention is also supported by controlled behavioral studies that show MM practice increases sustained and executive attention (Jha et al., 2007; Lutz et al., 2009; Slagter et al., 2009; Tang et al., 2007) and improves performance on various attentional tasks (Atchley et al., 2016; Bailey et al., 2022; Bailey, Freedman, et al., 2019a; Van Dam et al., 2018).

One sophisticated approach to measure potential MM-related changes in attention could be to examine the limited temporal capacity of attention using the attentional blink (AB) phenomenon (Martens & Wyble, 2010; Shapiro et al., 1997). In a typical AB task, individuals are presented with a rapid stream of ~20 distractor stimuli. Within that rapid stream of stimuli, two targets (T1 and T2) are presented in close temporal succession, with T1 typically appearing randomly after 2–8 stimuli have already been presented and the T2 stimulus appearing 200 to 700 ms after T1 (Ward et al., 1996). The AB phenomenon refers to a reduction in accuracy at recalling the T2 stimulus when it is presented within 200–500 ms after T1, with AB trials presenting T2 stimuli at this brief delay often referred to as a “short interval” attention blink trials (Shapiro et al., 1997). A number of cognitive models have been proposed to explain the AB phenomenon (for a review, see Martens & Wyble, 2010). Capacity-based models suggest competition between stimuli for attentional resources, so T1 induces a drain on limited attentional resources and insufficient attentional resources are available to successfully process T2 (Potter et al., 1998; Shapiro et al., 1997). In contrast, selection-based models consider the role of attentional control to be more important in explaining the attention blink effect, where the magnitude of an individual’s AB is affected by the extent to which distracting information is suppressed (Di Lollo et al., 2005; Olivers & Meeter, 2008). However, it is worth noting that thus far, evidence supporting one analytical model of the AB phenomenon does not necessarily negate the explanations provided by other models, and it is possible that the AB phenomenon involves mechanisms and functional processes proposed by multiple models (further discussion of this point is available in the Supplementary Materials Section 1).

The neurophysiological mechanisms that underpin the AB phenomena have been explored using EEG (Slagter et al., 2007; Vogel et al., 1998). This research has focused on an event-related potential (ERP) known as the P3b, which is a positive voltage occurring maximally in parietal electrodes around 350 to 600 ms following stimulus presentation, and which has been associated with voluntary attention when examined in healthy non-meditators (Falkenstein et al., 1991, 1993). Research in healthy non-meditators has found the P3b time-locked to the T2 stimuli to be entirely suppressed in trials in which the second target is “blinked” (not consciously perceived) and ultimately not recalled (Dell’Acqua et al., 2015). A reduced AB effect (i.e. increased accuracy at detecting T2 stimuli) has also been associated with an earlier onset and smaller amplitude of the T1-induced P3b, suggesting that when less neural activity is devoted to the T1 stimulus, more neural resources are available to detect and encode the T2 stimulus (Sergent et al., 2005; Slagter et al., 2007). In addition to the P3b AB effect, research in healthy non-meditators has also suggested that short interval AB trials reduce the amplitude of the visual processing related posterior-N2, an ERP peaking approximately 200 ms after stimuli presentation, with posterior-maximal negative voltages (Zivony et al., 2018). This is thought to reflect the lack of engagement of attention processes time-locked to T2 stimuli (Zivony et al., 2018). In addition to the ERP AB findings, research in healthy non-meditators has suggested that theta oscillations (rhythmic brain activity occurring between 4 and 8 Hz) are related to a range of cognitive processes, including attention (Mizuhara & Yamaguchi, 2007). Within research on meditators, a positive relationship between the successful detection of T2 stimuli and theta phase synchronisation (TPS) to the onset of the T2 stimuli has also been identified (Slagter et al., 2009). An increase in phase synchronisation reflects an increase in the consistency of the angle of ongoing oscillatory cycles within neural activity to stimuli presentation (Slagter et al., 2009). Finally, decreased synchronisation of alpha oscillations (8-13Hz) to the onset of the distractor stimuli presentation (which are presented prior to T1) and increased alpha-power (8–13 Hz) just prior to T1 stimuli presentation has also been associated with improved performance in the AB task after a 3-month meditation retreat (Slagter et al., 2009). Alpha oscillations are thought to be related to the functional inhibition of brain regions when examined in healthy non-meditators (Klimesch, 2012). As such, it is possible that desynchronisation of alpha oscillations around the time of the stimulus presentation and increased alpha-power just prior to the target stimulus onset inhibits processing of the distractors. This may be followed by a release of any inhibitory processes ongoing in brain regions responsible for processing the target AB stimuli, resulting in better AB performance.

Perhaps unsurprisingly, MM training and experience have been shown to reduce the AB phenomenon, with increased accuracy at the detection of T2 stimuli, both in long-term meditators, following a 3-month meditation retreat, and after an 8-week mindfulness-based stress reduction program (Slagter et al., 2007; van Leeuwen et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2022). However, to date, only one study (Slagter et al., 2007) has measured neural activity while meditators perform the AB task. They compared EEG activity from non-meditator controls and experienced mindfulness meditators (with an average of 2967 hr of meditation experience) before and after the experienced-meditators underwent an intensive 3-month meditation retreat, and the non-meditators practised MM for 20 min per day for 1 week. Following the retreat, the experienced-meditators were better at identifying the T2 AB stimuli compared to the controls (demonstrating a reduced AB effect) (Slagter et al., 2007; Slagter et al., 2009). The improved accuracy in responding as to which number was presented as the T2 stimuli was correlated with a reduced P3b following T1 stimuli, as well as increased T2-locked TPS (Slagter et al., 2007; Slagter et al., 2009). Slagter et al. (2007) suggested that the reduction in T1-elicited P3b in meditators may reflect “decreased mental capture by any stimulus” in the meditators, whereas the elevated TPS may reflect an increased capacity to process experience from moment to moment. They also found a reduction in alpha phase synchronisation (APS) to the distractor stimuli (prior to the onset of T1) in meditators, potentially implicating the release of alpha inhibiting the processing of distractor stimuli before T1 presentation (Slagter et al., 2009). Notably, these findings were only after an intensive 3-month retreat, meditation training that is not typical of many mindfulness training programs, and it is unclear if more typical daily MM practice will produce similar effects. Exploring a community sample of MM may provide findings that are more generalisable to a typical (and increasingly popular) MM practice (Cramer et al., 2016). Additionally, while Slagter et al. (2007) have been cited over 1000 times, no replications of their study have been attempted.

Given this background, the primary aim of the study was to compare brain activity related to the AB phenomenon (P3b, TPS, APS, and alpha-power) between a cross-sectional sample of experienced community-meditators and healthy control non-meditators in order to assess whether the findings demonstrated following intensive meditation retreats translate to more typical meditation practice. The present study also utilised advanced EEG analysis methods, which can separately detect differences in overall neural response strength and differences in the distribution of brain activity. Following the research by Slagter et al. (2007, 2009), our primary hypotheses were that (PH1) compared to non-meditator controls, meditators would show a smaller allocation of attention-related neural resources to T1 as indexed by a lower amplitude T1-elicited P3b during short interval trials; (PH2) meditators would show more consistency in the timing of theta oscillatory neural activity (higher TPS) in response to T2 during short interval trials but not long interval trials, indexed by higher T2-locked TPS values; and (PH3) the meditators would show greater alpha-power around stimuli presentation in short and long interval T1 trials compared to controls. Finally, the AB task presented stimuli every 100 ms (at 10 Hz), which is within the alpha frequency. This is likely to produce alpha synchronisation to the task stimuli, an effect that may be modified in the meditation group, which has undergone considerable training in an attention-based practice. Slagter et al. (2009) reported a reduction in APS during the presentation of the distractor stimuli prior to T1 presentation after the meditation retreat (in contrast to the increased alpha-power). As such, we had one further primary hypothesis: (PH4) APS would be reduced in the meditation group during the presentation of the distractor stimuli prior to T1 stimuli. Additionally, while we tested these primary hypotheses within the time windows reported by Slagter et al. (2007, 2009), to ensure we did not miss significant effects that appeared outside these specific windows, we conducted additional exploratory analyses for the ERP, TPS, APS, and alpha-power variables which included all time points in the EEG epochs for each of these measures (exploratory hypotheses are explained below), while employing data-driven multiple comparison controls. Additionally, since behavioral research using a cross-sectional design has previously shown that meditators show a reduced AB effect compared to non-meditator controls, we had a non-primary replication hypothesis, (RH1) that our meditation group would show a reduced AB effect as indicated by meditators showing higher accuracy than controls in short interval T2 trials. Further, while Slagter et al. (2007) focused on the P3b in response to T1 only, our view is that it is sensible to hypothesise that (EH1) ERPs to T2 would be increased in meditators, or (EH2) the relationship between ERP amplitude to T1 and T2 is different in meditators, perhaps reflecting an increased ability to attend to the T2 stimulus as a result of a reduced focus on the T1 stimuli. Additionally, since previous research has not examined potential differences in the topographical distribution of neural activity in meditators during the AB task, four non-directional exploratory hypotheses were that: (EH3) meditators would show differences in the scalp distribution of ERPs, (EH4) meditators would show differences in the scalp distribution of TPS, (EH5) meditators would show differences in the scalp distribution of alpha-power, and (EH6) meditators would show differences in the scalp distribution of APS.

Method

Participants

A sample of 39 experienced community-meditators and 36 healthy control non-meditators were recruited after responding via phone call or email to community advertising at universities, meditation organisations, and on social media. To meet the eligibility criteria for classification as an experienced-meditator, participants were required to have had at least 2 years of meditation experience and have practised meditation for a minimum of 2 hr per week over the last 3 months. Meditation was defined by Kabat-Zinn’s definition: “paying attention in a particular way: on purpose, in the present moment, and nonjudgmentally” (Kabat-Zinn, 1994). This definition included participants who practice both open-monitoring-meditation, which involves simple awareness without a specific focus besides awareness itself and focused attention meditation, which involves deliberate attention on a specific object, such as the breath (Cahn & Polich, 2009; Lutz et al., 2008). Trained MM researchers (OB, JEP, GH, HG) interviewed and screened participants to ensure the participants’ practices fit the criteria, and screening uncertainties were resolved through discussion and consensus between the principal investigator (NWB) and one other researcher. Eligibility as a non-meditator control required participants to have less than 2 hr of lifetime meditation experience.

Participants were considered ineligible to participate if they were currently taking psychoactive medication; had experienced brain injury; had previously been diagnosed with a psychiatric or neurological condition; or met the criteria for any drug, alcohol, or neuropsychiatric disorders as measured by the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (Sheehan et al., 1998). Participants who scored above the moderate range (greater than 25) in the Beck Anxiety Inventory (BAI) (Beck et al., 1988) or the mild range (greater than 19) in the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (Beck et al., 1961) were also excluded to reduce potential confounds, as depression and anxiety are associated with alterations to brain activity (Bailey et al., 2014; Miljevic et al., 2023; Murphy et al., 2019).

Ethical approval of the study was provided by the ethics committees of the Alfred Hospital and Monash University. All participants provided written informed consent prior to participation in the study. Before participants underwent EEG recording, participants provided their gender, age, years of education, and meditation experience (total years of practice, frequency of practice, and the usual length of a meditation session). Participants also completed the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ; Baer et al., 2006), BAI, and BDI-II. Example items from these scales are provided in the Supplementary Materials. Two controls were excluded from the study due to scoring above the moderate anxiety range on the BAI. Two controls and one meditator were excluded after scoring in the mild depression range on the BDI-II. Another control was excluded after revealing a history of meditation. Two meditators were excluded due to a previous history of seizures, substance abuse, or mental illness, and another three were excluded from the analysis due to not completing the AB task. Lastly, two meditators and one control were excluded from the study as their performance of the AB task was near chance.

The final sample included 31 meditators aged between 20 and 64 years and 30 healthy controls aged between 20 and 60. The two groups did not differ in any demographic or self-report measure except for the FFMQ score (all p > 0.05, except for the FFMQ, where p < 0.001). Table 1 summarises all measures (note that one participant did not complete the BAI, and another did not complete the FFMQ, so their data were excluded from those measures). The final sample of meditators had a mean of 6.44 (SD = 4.25) years of meditation experience, 7.65 hr (SD = 2.21) of current practice per week, and a mean of 55.65 min (SD = 44.90) of meditating per session.

Table 1 Demographic and self-report means (M), standard deviations (SD), and statistics. BAI Beck Anxiety Inventory, BDI-II Beck Depression Inventory II, FFMQ Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire

Procedure

The current study was a single component of a larger research program that assessed the associations between mindfulness practice and a number of cognitive functions. As such, participants completed multiple cognitive tasks within the EEG session, the results of which have been or will be reported in separate publications. Participants first performed a Go/Nogo task and an auditory oddball task (Payne et al., 2020), followed by the AB task. The AB task was a replication of the task used by Slagter et al. (2007). The task involved 12 practice trials followed by four blocks of 90 trials where the participants viewed a stream of 19 stimuli (letters and numbers) presented for 66 ms, with a 33-ms blank screen between each stimulus. Before the task began, participants were instructed that there could be one or two numbers in each trial. They were instructed to enter the number/s they observed on a number pad once each trial ended. Each new trial began after the participant pressed the Enter key to continue, and participants were offered the option of a short break between each of the four blocks. T1 occurred at a random position from 3 to 9 in the stream, after 2–8 distractor stimuli had already been presented. In trials with two numbers, T2 could occur either 300 ms (short interval) or 700 ms (long interval) after T1. Each block contained 54 short interval trials, 18 long interval trials, and 18 T1-only trials (where no T2 stimulus was presented). The order of the trials within each block was randomised. The number of correct trials (both T1 and T2 correct), the number of trials where T1 was incorrect, and the number of trials where T2 was incorrect were recorded for each participant. The total task time was approximately 45 min (see Fig. 1 for a visual depiction of the task). After the AB task, participants were administered transcranial magnetic stimulation concurrent with EEG to assess for potential meditation-related differences in cortical reactivity to magnetic stimulation.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Visual representation of the procedure for the attentional blink (AB) task. Each trial presented a fixation cross, followed by 19 items in the centre of the screen. The majority of the items were letters, presented for 66 ms each with a 33-ms blank screen between each stimulus. Target stimuli (T1 and T2) were numbers presented within the stream of letters. T1 appeared after between 2 and 8 letters had been presented, and T2 appeared either 300 ms after T1 (short interval) or 700 ms after T1 (long interval), unless it was a T1 only trial (in which case T2 was not presented)

Measures

Electrophysiological Recording and Pre-Processing

EEG data including 64 channels were recorded continuously during the tasks using a Quick-Cap containing Ag/AgCl electrodes and SynAmps 2 amplifier (Compumedics, Melbourne, Australia). Data were recorded by Neuroscan Acquire software, with samples obtained at 1000 Hz and an online bandpass filter from 0.05 to 200 Hz (24 dB/octave roll-off). Each electrode was connected to a reference electrode positioned between CPz and Cz. Prior to the start of the recording, all electrode impedances were reduced to < 5kΩ.

EEG recordings were pre-processed offline in MATLAB R2018b (The MathWorks, Inc.) using the RELAX EEG cleaning pipeline (Bailey, Biabani, et al., 2023a; Bailey, Hill, et al., 2023b), which calls EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004) and fieldtrip functions (Oostenveld et al., 2011). Within the RELAX pipeline, data were first bandpass filtered with a fourth-order Butterworth filter from 0.25 to 80 Hz and bandstop filtered from 47 to 53 Hz to reduce the line noise. Next, the default RELAX settings were used to reject extreme outlying channels using multiple validated methods (Bailey, Biabani, et al., 2023a; Bailey, Hill, et al., 2023b; Bigdely-Shamlo et al., 2015), followed by the marking of extreme outlying EEG periods for exclusion from the Multiple Wiener Filter cleaning and deletion before independent component analysis (see Bailey et al., 2022 for details). Three sequential Multiple Wiener Filters were used to reduce (1) muscle activity (Fitzgibbon et al., 2016), (2) eye blinks, then (3) horizontal eye movement and electrode drift (Somers et al., 2018). Finally, data were re-referenced to the robust average reference (Bigdely-Shamlo et al., 2015), and the remaining artifacts were cleaned using wavelet-enhanced independent component analysis (ICA) (Castellanos & Makarov, 2006) to reduce artifactual components identified by ICLabel (Pion-Tonachini et al., 2019) after ICA decomposition using cudaICA (Raimondo et al., 2012). Full details of the pre-processing pipeline are available in Bailey, Biabani, et al. (2023a) and Bailey, Hill, et al. (2023b).

After cleaning, EEG activity was epoched to the onset of the AB task stimuli from −200 to 1000 ms surrounding the T1 or T2 stimuli for ERP analysis and from −2000 to 2000 ms for oscillation analyses. The fieldtrip “ft_freqanalysis” function was used with Morlet wavelet analysis settings and a cycle width of 5 to compute frequency power.

ERP data were baseline corrected using the baseline subtraction method to the average activity in the −200 to 0 ms period prior to target stimulus onset, as per the methods of Slagter et al. (2007). To test our first primary hypothesis (PH1) for the P3b ERP, we averaged data within the 350 to 600 ms time window following the stimuli.

TPS and APS were quantified through the calculation of a phase-locking factor (PLF) value within the theta range (4 to 8.5 Hz) and alpha range (8.5 to 15 Hz, in replication of Slagter et al., 2009) (Lachaux et al., 1999; Ueno et al., 2009). PLF values range from 0 to 1, where 1 represents perfectly correlated phase differences between trials, and 0 represents completely uncorrelated phase differences (Ueno et al., 2009; Varela et al., 2001). The methods for this computation are described in more detail in the Supplementary Materials (Section 2b). To test hypothesis PH2, TPS data were averaged within the 121 to 501 ms window after T2 stimuli. To test hypothesis PH4, APS data were averaged within the −414 to −214 ms window prior to T1 stimuli.

For alpha frequency power analyses, trials were baseline corrected to oscillatory power across the entire epoch (in replication of Slagter et al., 2009). While this means a potential signal reduction in potential “active” periods (as the data from those periods is contained within the baseline subtraction), this approach prevents spurious conclusions about differences in active periods being, in fact, driven by an arbitrarily selected baseline period. As such, significant differences at any time point in the epoch reflect an increase or decrease of oscillatory power at those time points relative to the ongoing oscillatory power across the entire epoch. Baseline correction of frequency power data was performed using the relative method ([all active period datapoints − the mean baseline activity] / mean baseline activity). To test hypothesis PH3, alpha power was averaged within the −31 to 160 ms time window following T1. Only epochs from target stimuli that participants responded to correctly were used in the EEG analysis (for epochs locked to T1, this meant trials where T1 was responded to correctly, while for T2 locked epochs, this meant trials where participants correctly identified both T1 and T2 stimuli). Each condition was averaged separately within each participant for ERP and oscillation analyses (note that the conditions were: short vs long interval and T1 vs T2 stimuli).

Data Analyses

EEG data comparisons of ERPs, TPS, alpha-power, and APS, between meditators and non-meditators, were performed using the randomized graphical user interface (RAGU) method (Koenig et al., 2011). RAGU compares scalp field differences over all epoch time points and electrodes using rank order randomisation statistics with no preliminary assumptions about time windows and electrodes to analyse (Koenig et al., 2011). Prior to conducting primary tests, a topographical consistency test (TCT) was conducted to confirm the consistent distribution of scalp activity within each group and condition. A significant TCT result suggests that potential between-group differences in the global field power (GFP) and topographic analysis of variance (TANOVA) tests (described later in this paragraph) are due to real group differences instead of variation within one of the groups (Koenig & Melie-García, 2010). RAGU allows for comparisons of global neural response strength (independent of the distribution of activity) with the GFP test. The GFP is an index of the total voltage differences across all channels, regardless of the specific locations of the activity; it is equivalent to the standard deviation across all channels at each time point (Habermann et al., 2018). The GFP test compares differences between Groups or Conditions from the real data against randomised permutation data to identify specific time periods following a stimuli where Groups or Conditions significantly differed in neural response strength. RAGU also allows for comparisons of the distribution of neural activity with the TANOVA (with the recommended L2 normalisation of the amplitude of neural activity which transforms data for such that the overall GFP = 1 within each individual, providing distribution comparisons that are independent of differences in global amplitude). Note that there are currently no Bayesian statistical approaches analogous to the TANOVA.

TPS, alpha-power, and APS values were compared with root mean square (RMS) and TANOVA tests (to separately compare overall neural response strength and distribution of neural activity, respectively). The RMS is computed in the same manner as the GFP, but without implementing an average re-referencing across the data prior to its computation. This is the recommended approach when oscillatory power or phase synchronisation comparisons are computed with RAGU, as the average reference was computed prior to the oscillation measurement transforms. As such, the RMS test is a comparison of the RMS between Groups rather than the GFP, a measure which is a valid indicator of neural response strength in the power or phase synchronisation domain (Habermann et al., 2018). In other respects, the statistic used to compare RMS between Groups is identical to the GFP test described in the previous paragraph.

RAGU controls for multiple comparisons in space by using only a single value representing all electrodes for the GFP/RMS and TANOVA tests (the GFP/RMS value for the GFP/RMS test and the global dissimilarity value for the TANOVA). RAGU also controls for multiple comparisons across time points in the epoch using global duration statistics (referred to as the “global duration control”) which calculate the periods of significant effects within the epoch that are longer than 95% of significant effects in the randomised data with the alpha level at 0.05 (Koenig et al., 2011). However, because the computation of measures of oscillatory power or phase consistency elicits a dependence in values across neighbouring timepoints, RAGU’s global duration control method is only appropriate for ERP analyses. For our oscillatory power and phase measures, we implemented the same duration controls as Slagter et al. (2009). Because our primary hypotheses were obtained from Slagter et al. (2007, 2009), we averaged data within specific windows of interest for our primary analyses. However, to explore potential effects outside of these windows, we also used RAGU for whole epoch analyses (from −100 to 800 ms for ERPs and from −500 to 1500 ms for oscillatory analyses), with multiple comparison controls implemented using the global duration statistics. The recommended 5000 randomisation permutations were conducted with an alpha of p = 0.05. For more in-depth information about RAGU and its analyses, please refer to Koenig et al. (2011), Koenig and Melie-García (2010), and Habermann et al. (2018). The p-values from our primary hypotheses (with data averaged within a priori hypothesised time windows of interest) were submitted to false discovery rate (FDR) multiple comparison controls (Benjamini & Hochberg, 2000) to control for experiment-wise multiple comparisons (referred to as FDR-p). For the sake of brevity, only main effects and interactions involving Group are reported in the manuscript, while other results of interest are reported in the Supplementary Materials (Section 3). For brevity, the full details of all statistical analyses are reported in the Supplementary Materials (Section 2). However, we note here that some time windows of interest occurred prior to the presentation of T1 stimuli, in line with Slagter et al. (2009). These time windows were analysed as the results from Slagter et al. (2009) suggested differences in the meditation group in the synchronisation of neural activity to the distractor stimuli that were presented prior to T1, perhaps suggesting less reactivity to those stimuli in preparation for processing the target.

To test our hypotheses for ERPs (PH1, EH1, EH2, and EH3), global field power (GFP) and topographical analysis of variance (TANOVA) tests were averaged between 350 and 600 ms (P3b period) (Polich, 1997) after T1 onset to make direct comparisons with Slagter et al. (2007). For this averaged activity, GFP and TANOVA tests were used to conduct repeated measures ANOVA design statistics, examining 2 Groups (meditators vs controls) × 2 Conditions (short and long interval). To test our exploratory hypotheses that differences might be present outside of this specific time window or might be present following T2 (EH1, EH2, and E3), GFP and TANOVA tests were used to conduct the repeated measures ANOVA design statistics, examining 2 Groups (meditators vs controls) × 2 Conditions (short and long interval) × 2 Targets (T1 and T2) for event-related potential (ERP) data across the entire −100 to 800 ms interval after T1 onset.

To test our hypotheses for TPS (PH2 and EH4), we compared TPS between the Groups; root mean squared (RMS) and TANOVA tests were used to conduct repeated measures ANOVA design, examining 2 Groups (meditators vs controls) × 2 Conditions (short and long interval) comparisons for TPS data surrounding T2 onset. To make comparisons with Slagter et al. (2009), RMS and TANOVA tests were averaged within the 121 to 501 ms window (where Slagter et al., 2009 detected an effect that was maximal at electrodes FC6 and Fz) and the 309 to 558 ms window (where Slagter et al., 2009 detected an effect that was maximal at electrode T8) after the T2 stimuli. An additional exploratory analysis was performed on TPS data from −500 to 1500 ms around the stimuli, to determine if any effects were missed by the analysis focused only on T2. This analysis included T1 stimuli in a repeated measures ANOVA design examining 2 Groups (meditators vs controls) × 2 Conditions (short and long interval) × 2 Conditions (T1 and T2).

To test our hypotheses related to alpha-power and APS (PH3, PH4, EH5, and EH6), RMS and TANOVA tests were used to conduct repeated measures ANOVA design comparisons of alpha-power and APS (separately), examining 2 Groups (meditators vs controls) × 2 Conditions (short and long interval) comparisons for data averaged within a −31 to 160 ms period for alpha-power and averaged within a −414 to 214 ms period for APS. Similar to the ERPs and TPS tests, we also performed a whole epoch analysis from −500 to 1500 ms surrounding T1 onset to test for effects outside those reported by Slagter et al. (2009).

In addition to the RAGU analysis, traditional single electrode comparisons were conducted for comparison with previous research, using time windows and electrodes that showed significant results in comparisons by Slagter et al. (2007, 2009). Methods and results for these comparisons are reported in the Supplementary Materials (Sections 2 and 3 respectively).

Between-group comparisons of the demographic and behavioral data were performed using SPSS v25 or the robust statistics WRS2 package from R where parametric assumptions were not met (Field and Wilcox, 2017). Independent samples t-tests compared age, BAI, BDI-II, FFMQ, and years of education. A three-way repeated measures ANOVA was planned to analyse behavioral data. Interval (short or long) and Target (T1 or T2) were within-subjects factors and Group (meditators vs controls) the between-subjects factor. The dependent variable was AB accuracy, defined as the percentage of correctly responded to trials (T1 and T2 identified correctly). This tested hypothesis RH1, with post hoc tests planned to assess the specific hypothesis that meditators showed a reduced AB effect (defined by increased short interval T2 accuracy) if an interaction between Group, Target, and Interval were present. Where possible, Bayesian analyses were also performed using JASP (Love et al., 2019) to provide the strength of evidence for either the null or alternative hypotheses (for all of the behavioral, demographic, and EEG comparisons), and a small number of follow-up exploratory linear mixed models were used to test our explanations for significant results (described in full in the Supplementary Materials, Section 3). For these Bayesian analyses, Bayes factor (BF) values were provided to indicate the strength of evidence. BF10 is provided to indicate the strength of support for the alternative hypothesis, BF01 to indicate the strength of support for the null hypothesis. BFincl is reported to indicate the strength of support for the positive hypothesis of an interaction (indicating the support for the alternative hypothesis when the interaction was included in the model compared to when the interaction was excluded), and BFexcl to indicate the strength of support for the null hypothesis of an interaction (indicating the support for the null hypothesis when the interaction was not included in the model compared to when the interaction was included).

Results

ERP Comparisons

To test our first primary hypothesis (PH1) that meditators would show a smaller allocation of attention-related neural resources to T1, reflected by a lower amplitude of the P3b neural response strength to T1 stimuli in meditators compared to controls, the GFP test was performed on the P3b time window (from 350 to 600 ms following T1, consistent with Slagter et al., 2007). No difference was detected for the main effect of Group in GFP averaged across the P3b period (p = 0.798, FDR-p = 0.798, ηp2 = 0.001, see Table 2 and Fig. 2), nor was there a significant interaction between Group and Interval (p = 0.732, ηp2 = 0.004). To test the strength of evidence for the null hypothesis, averaged P3b GFP values from within the time window of interest (350 to 600ms) were tested with a Bayesian repeated measures ANOVA. This analysis showed that the null hypothesis was more likely than the alternative hypothesis for both the Group factor and the interaction between Group and Interval. Comparing models including Group and a Group by Interval interaction to the model only including Interval provided BF01 = 6.520, while comparing the main effect of Group independently to equivalent models stripped of the Group effect and excluding higher-order interactions, BFexcl = 1.835, and for the interaction between Group and Interval, BFexcl = 3.553. Our single electrode analyses, which focused on time windows and electrodes reported to be significant by Slagter et al. (2007), showed similarly null results (Supplementary Materials Section 3b).

Table 2 Global field potential (GFP) values averaged across the P3b period of interest
Fig. 2
figure 2

Event-related potential data time-locked to T1 stimuli, averaged within the 350 to 600 ms time window for direct comparison with Slagter et al., (2007). Left: Grand averaged ERP data from Pz time-locked to short (top) and long (bottom) interval T1 stimuli (error shading reflects 95% confidence intervals). Right top: Global field potential (GFP) values averaged across the P3b period of interest (from 350 to 600 ms after T1 presentation). Right bottom: The mean (non-normalised) topography within the 350 to 600 ms window after T1 presentation

As mentioned in our hypotheses, while Slagter et al. (2007) focused on the P3b in response to T1 only, our view is that it is sensible to hypothesise that effects might occur in components other than the P3b, that ERP amplitudes time locked to T2 might be increased in meditators (EH1), or for the relationship between ERP amplitudes time locked to T1 and T2 to be different in meditators (EH2). To test these exploratory hypotheses (EH1 and EH2), a GFP test was performed across the entire epoch (−100 to 800 ms), including all conditions (both T1 and T2 targets and short/long intervals). This test showed a significant interaction between Group and Target from 214 to 258 ms following the stimuli (averaged across this time Interval: p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.0914, see Fig. 3), which survived multiple comparison controls for duration (global duration control = 41ms). This effect falls within the typical posterior-N2 time window. Within this interaction, controls showed significantly higher GFP amplitudes in response to T1 compared to T2 (p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.1657), while meditators showed no difference between T1 and T2 (p = 0.279, ηp2 = 0.0403). When Group comparisons were restricted to short interval T1 stimuli only (averaged within the 214 to 258 ms window), meditators showed significantly lower posterior-N2 GFP amplitudes than controls (p = 0.029, ηp2 = 0.0784, see Figs. 3 and 4). To determine the strength of evidence for this significant interaction between Group and Target, averaged GFP values for each participant across both short and long intervals were calculated for both T1 and T2 targets separately and submitted to a repeated measures Bayesian ANOVA design. When comparing the interaction effect against models that did not include the interaction effect, the Bayes factor showed moderate evidence for the effect (BFincl = 3.411). As such, while hypothesis EH1 was not supported (as meditators did not show larger amplitude ERPs following T2 stimuli), hypothesis EH2 was supported, as meditators showed a more equal distribution of ERP amplitudes between T1 and T2 than controls (although not within the P3b window). Finally, in our test of the exploratory hypothesis that the distribution of ERPs would differ between meditators and controls (EH3), the TANOVA showed no significant main effect of Group or interaction involving Group that exceeded multiple comparison controls for the number of comparisons across the epoch (all p > 0.05).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Left: p-value graphs for the main effect of Group and interactions involving Group for the whole epoch comparisons of the event-related potential (ERP) global field potential (GFP). The black line reflects the p-value, white areas reflect significant time points, and green periods reflect windows where the effect passed global duration controls. Top right: GFP activity in response to the first target (T1) and second target (T2), averaged over the significant window for the test of the interaction between Group and Target (from 214 to 258 ms following the stimuli) and averaged across both short and long intervals. Bottom right: the mean (non-normalised) topography within the significant 214 to 258 ms period from each group, averaged across T1 and T2 locked epochs separately

Fig. 4
figure 4

Averaged event-related potentials (ERPs) averaged within fronto-central (top) (F1 Fz F2 FC1 FCz FC2) and parietal-occipital (bottom) electrodes (PO7 PO5 PO6 PO8 O1 Oz O2) time-locked to T1 with the significant period marked (red dashed lines). Note that our analyses were based on the GFP, so while the averaged electrodes demonstrate the difference (with N2 ERPs showing smaller amplitudes in meditators regardless of polarity), our significance tests were not based on these values. Note also the oscillatory pattern in the alpha frequency, synchronised to the stimuli presentation rate

In the Supplementary Materials, we report exploratory linear mixed models and generalised linear mixed models to explore the potential associations between single trial GFP values within the posterior-N2 effect and whether single trials were responded to correctly assess potential explanations for this result (Supplementary Materials, Section 3b). In brief, these exploratory analyses showed that correct identification of short interval T2 stimuli was associated with lower posterior-N2 GFP time-locked to T1 (similar to the pattern shown by the meditators) (Supplementary Materials, Fig. S3). This suggests that when fewer attentional resources were devoted to processing T1, T2 could be more accurately identified. Additionally, in single trial analysis, the relationship between T2 posterior-N2 GFP, trial number, and response accuracy differed between the Groups. To begin with, both meditators and controls were less likely to identify T2 stimuli if their T2 posterior-N2 GFP was high. Controls showed the same pattern throughout the task. However, by the end of the task, this pattern reversed for the meditators who were more likely to identify T2 targets when they showed high posterior-N2 GFP values.

Theta Phase Synchronisation (TPS) Comparisons

The TCT for TPS showed consistent neural activity across groups and conditions from −280 ms across the first 600 ms after stimulus presentation, with TCT inconsistency in controls locked to the T1 stimuli prior to this time that did not overlap with any of our significant effects in the RMS test, but did overlap with some of the significant effects within the TANOVA tests. This demonstrated that our RMS TPS results were not driven simply by inconsistent topographical activation within a single group or condition (Supplementary Materials, Fig. S5). For our test of hypothesis PH2, that meditators would show higher TPS following short interval T2, RMS TPS was averaged within short interval T2 trials across the 121 to 501 ms window for direct comparison with Slagter et al. (2009) (who found an effect within this window, maximal at Fz and FC6). No significant difference was detected, indicating that meditators did not show higher TPS following short interval T2 stimuli within the 121 to 501 ms window (p = 0.086, FDR-p = 0.173, ηp2 = 0.0482, BF01 = 1.104). Similarly, for the 309 to 558 ms period (where Slagter et al., 2009 found an effect within this window that was maximal at electrode T8), no significant difference was detected (p = 0.118, ηp2 = 0.0418, BF01 = 1.373).

However, when all conditions and time points were included in an exploratory analysis of RMS TPS, a significant interaction between Group, Target, and Interval was present from 117 to 295 ms (averaged across the significant window: p = 0.0002, ηp2 = 0.2358, Fig. 5). This effect lasted longer than the duration controls for multiple comparisons over time used by Slagter et al. (2009) (175.1 ms). When RMS TPS was averaged within the significant window (117 to 295 ms), Bayesian analysis of the interaction indicated strong support for the alternative hypothesis (BFincl = 41.612), and the model including this Group, Target, and Interval interaction effect as well as the nested comparisons was 5.502e+9 times more likely than the null model (BF10 = 5.502e+9). In assessing the cause of the 3-way interaction with reduced ANOVA designs (where data was averaged across one of the original factors prior to re-analysis to enable easier interpretation), our results indicated it was driven by two features: firstly, controls showed larger RMS TPS during long interval T2 trials than short interval T2 trials, while meditators showed very little difference in RMS TPS between the short and long interval conditions (p = 0.0094, ηp2 = 0.1718, BFincl = 29.574). Secondly, the interaction was also driven by an effect where meditators showed a more even distribution of RMS TPS between T1 and T2, in comparison to controls who showed higher RMS TPS values to T1 compared to T2 (short interval T1 vs short interval T2) (p = 0.0022, ηp2 = 0.1626, BFincl = 25.192). However, counter to the results of Slagter et al. (2009), the interaction was not driven specifically by a difference between Groups in short interval T2 TPS (averaged within the 117 to 295 ms window showing the significant interaction, there was no significant difference between the groups in short interval T2 RMS TPS, p = 0.136, ηp2 = 0.0373). Single electrode analyses replicating the electrode and window of interest used by Slagter et al. (2009) showed the same pattern of results as the effect we detected within the 117 to 295 ms window, with Bayesian evidence supporting the alternative hypothesis for the interaction between Group and Interval for T2 stimuli (BFincl = 4.621 within the time window used by Slagter et al. (2009), and BFincl = 35.908 when restricted to the significant time period detected in our exploratory analysis, reported in full in the Supplementary Materials, Fig. S6).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Root mean squared (RMS) comparisons of Group, Target, and Interval for theta phase synchronisation. Left: P-graphs for the main effect of Group and interactions involving Group. The black line reflects the p-value, white areas reflect significant time points, and the light blue area indicates the effect that passed the duration control used by Slagter et al. (2009). Right: Theta phase synchronisation RMS showing the significant interaction of interest between Group, Interval, and Target from the averaged activity within the 117 to 295 ms window (p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.2526, BFincl = 41.612)

To assess whether these differences in TPS might have behavioral relevance, we performed Pearson’s correlations between TPS and percentage correct from short interval T2 trials across both groups together. These results indicated that TPS from all conditions correlated with short interval T2 accuracy (statistics reported in full in Table 3, and scatterplots for these comparisons can be viewed in Fig. 6). We also conducted the same correlations within each group separately. While these separate within-group correlations are lower in statistical power, they suggest that the TPS correlates with short interval T2 accurately more strongly in the control group than in the meditator group, and that within the control group, TPS time locked to T1 correlates more strongly to short interval T2 accuracy (see Table 3). However, the 95% confidence intervals of the Pearson r values (based on 1000 bootstrap replications) overlapped between the two groups, so we cannot be confident that the difference in correlation strength between the groups represented a statistical difference.

Table 3 Pearson’s correlations between percent correct responses to the second target stimuli (T2) in short interval trials and the averaged root mean squared (RMS) theta phase synchronisation (TPS) within the 117 to 295 ms period in response to both the first target stimulus (T1) and T2
Fig. 6
figure 6

Scatterplots depicting the correlations between root mean squared (RMS) theta phase synchronisation (TPS) averaged within the significant window (117 to 295 ms) from each condition and accuracy at detecting the second target stimuli (T2) in short interval trials. Note the common pattern across all groups and conditions. The grey and light green areas reflect relative variance from the line of best fit at point on the x-axis

There was also an interaction between Group and Interval from 455 to 560 ms (averaged across the significant window: p = 0.0218, ηp2 = 0.1520). However, this period did not survive the 175.1 ms minimum duration used by Slagter et al. (2009). No other main effect or interaction involving Group was significant for any part of the epoch (all p > 0.10).

With regard to our exploratory hypothesis that the scalp distribution of TPS would differ between Groups (EH4), the TANOVA, including all conditions and all time points, showed an interaction between Group and Target during the presentation of the distractor stimuli from −385 to −100 ms prior to T1, which lasted longer than the duration control for multiple comparisons (175.1 ms) used by Slagter et al. (2009). When averaged across the significant window, the statistics were as follows: p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.0609 (Fig. 7). When the interaction was explored by averaging TPS within the significant period and performing TANOVA comparisons between the groups for T1 and T2 stimuli separately, the effect was shown to be driven by a difference in TPS distribution between the Groups prior to T1 stimuli (p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.0336), with meditators showing more TPS in occipital electrodes (meditator minus control t-max at Oz = 2.908) and meditators showing less TPS in right frontal electrodes (meditator minus control t-min at F6 = −3.384). Groups did not differ in TPS locked to T2 stimuli (p = 0.1358). It is worth noting that the period that showed the significant result overlapped with a period of topographical inconsistency in T1-locked TPS in the control group (with inconsistent topographical distributions across the control group prior to −280 ms). This suggests that at least part of the interaction may have been driven by an inconsistent topographical pattern in the control group (rather than a between-group difference during that time period). No other differences were present in any of the main effect or interactions involving Group within any time point in the epoch (all p > 0.05).

Fig. 7
figure 7

Topographical analysis of variance (TANOVA) test results for the theta phase synchronisation (TPS). Left: p-graphs for the main effect of Group and each interaction involving Group. The black line reflects the p-value, the white areas reflect significant time points, and the light blue periods reflect windows where the effect passed the duration control used by Slagter et al. (2009). Right top: A multi-dimensional scaling graph depicting the differences between each group’s TPS topographies in response to the first (T1) and second (T2) target stimuli averaged during the window of the significant Group × Target interaction (−390 to −85 ms around the target). Within the multi-dimensional scaling graph, the topography maps indicate the ends of the eigenvector spectrum in each of the x- and y-axis, and the points on the graph indicate where each group and condition’s mean topography lay on that spectrum (for both the x- and y-axis) relative to the other points in the graph (note that the topographies along the x- and y-axis do not represent the actual topography for a group/condition). As such, the interaction between Group and Target in topographical activation is demonstrated by the graph. Right bottom: the t-map for the meditator minus control theta phase synchronisation topography for T1 stimuli (averaged from −390 to −85 ms around T1), after normalisation for overall amplitude (so that all individuals had a GFP = 1). Red indicates areas where meditators showed higher values, blue indicates areas where controls showed higher values (indicating that topographical differences were present, without suggesting that TPS was higher in the control group in a specific electrode, due to the normalisation for amplitude)

Alpha-power Comparisons

The TCT for RMS alpha-power showed consistent neural activity across all groups and conditions from −400 ms until the end of the epoch, indicating our alpha-power results were not driven simply by inconsistent topographical activation within a single group or condition (details are reported in the Supplementary Materials, Fig. S7). When RMS alpha-power was averaged across the −31 to 160 ms window for direct comparison with Slagter et al. (2009) and test of our third primary hypothesis (PH3 — that meditators would show greater alpha-power around T1 presentation), no significant difference was detected (p = 0.2976, FDR-p = 0.3968, ηp2 = 0.0189, BF01 = 2.379). The exploratory RMS test for alpha-power, including all time points within the epoch time-locked to T1 stimuli, showed a significant main effect of Group from 475 to 685 ms, in which meditators showed less alpha-power (averaged within this window: p = 0.023, ηp2 = 0.0844, see Fig. 8). This effect was longer than the duration control criteria implemented by Slagter et al. (2009) (83.5 ms for alpha). No interaction was detected between Interval and Group in RMS alpha-power that lasted longer than the duration controls used by Slagter et al. (2009). Nor was there any Group main effect or interaction between Group and Interval in the alpha-power TANOVA (all p > 0.05). This provided a null result for hypothesis EH5 (that there would be differences between the groups in the scalp distribution of alpha-power).

Fig. 8
figure 8

Root mean squared (RMS) alpha-power comparisons time-locked to T1 stimuli onset. Top left: The cumulative variance explained (ηp2) at each time point across the epoch by each main effect and condition, with each colour reflecting the ηp2 from the effect being tested, colour coded to match the p-graphs. Top right and middle: the p-graphs for the main effect of Interval (orange, middle left), Group (blue), and interaction between Interval and Group (yellow, middle right). The black line reflects the p-value, white areas reflect significant time points, and light blue periods reflect windows where the effect passed the duration control used by Slagter et al. (2009). Bottom: Mean RMS alpha-power averaged within the 475 to 685 ms window following the first target stimuli (T1) for each participant (baseline corrected to alpha-power across the entire epoch)

To explore potential explanations for these results, we performed a number of additional tests of the pattern of relationships between trial number, single trial accuracy (to assess potential learning across the task), and alpha-power within this significant period (these are reported in the Supplementary Materials Section 3e). In brief, the baseline-corrected RMS alpha-power within the 475 to 685 ms window decreased across trials as participants completed the task, which was concurrent with improved performance across the task, suggesting participants may have been learning attention-based strategies to enable improved short interval T2 detection. However, across all participants, averaged baseline-corrected alpha-power RMS within the 475 to 685 ms window after T1 did not correlate with the accuracy of short interval T2 detection. Further, an exploratory linear mixed model indicated that incorrect responses were associated with slightly, yet significantly, lower short interval RMS alpha-power than correct responses (Supplementary Materials, Fig. S11). However, lower short interval trial RMS alpha-power within a later 685 to 1050 ms window was strongly associated with correct responses (Fig. S12). Short interval alpha-power RMS was also strongly correlated between these two periods (between the 475 to 685ms period and the 685 to 1050 ms period). This relationship was stronger within incorrect trials than for correct trials, and long interval RMS alpha-power increased in the later 685 to 1050 ms window compared to the earlier period in both groups. This suggests that lower short interval RMS alpha-power in the later 685 to 1050 ms window was required to identify the T2 stimuli. As such, perhaps the lower RMS alpha-power in the earlier period might have been a compensatory mechanism on trials when participants noticed their attention waning, reflecting an attempt to regulate alpha-power in the later period, during which low alpha-power was more important for stimulus processing.

Alpha Phase Synchronisation Comparisons

In test of our fourth primary hypothesis (PH4 — that APS would be reduced in the meditation group during the presentation of the distractor stimuli prior to T1 stimuli), we conducted an RMS test of APS time-locked to T1 stimuli averaged across the period where distractor stimuli were presented prior to T1 (within the −414 to −214 ms window for direct comparison with Slagter et al. (2009), our results indicated a non-significant main effect of Group, where meditators showed higher APS, which is in the opposite direction to the findings provided by Slagter et al. (2009) (p = 0.061, FDR-p = 0.173, ηp2 = 0.0586). Additionally, our exploratory analysis of APS across the entire epoch showed a significant main effect of Group from −258 to −90 ms, and from 288 to 1500 ms (both of which survived the duration controls of 83.5ms used by Slagter et al. (2009), see Fig. 9). Within both the shorter pre-stimulus and longer post-stimulus period, meditators showed larger RMS APS (averaged within the −258 to −90 ms period = 0.031, ηp2 = 0.072, BFincl = 2.089, averaged within the 288 to 1500 ms period: p = 0.018, ηp2 = 0.092, BFincl = 1.752, with the best model including the main effect of Group and the main effect of Interval, BF10 = 42.23 for the average Interval from 288 to 1500 ms). Our results also indicated a brief significant interaction between Group and Interval in APS RMS (706 to 786 ms) which did not pass the duration controls used by Slagter et al. (2009).

Fig. 9
figure 9

Root mean squared (RMS) alpha phase synchronisation (APS) comparisons time-locked to T1 stimuli onset. Top left: The cumulative variance explained (ηp2) at each time point across the epoch by each main effect and condition, with each colour reflecting the ηp2 from the effect being tested, colour coded to match the p-graphs. Top right and middle: the p-graphs for the main effect of Interval (orange, middle left), Group (blue), and interaction between Interval and Group (yellow, middle right). The black line reflects the p-value, white areas reflect significant time points, and light blue periods reflect windows where the effect passed the duration controls used by Slagter et al. (2009). Bottom: Mean root mean squared alpha phase synchronisation (RMS APS) from each group in response to T1 long (LIT1) and short (SIT1) interval trials, averaged within the significant window from the RMS APS test

With regard to the TANOVA test of APS (which tested exploratory hypothesis EH6 — that meditators would show a different scalp distribution of APS), a significant Group main effect was detected from 990 to 1500 ms where meditators showed higher APS values in fronto-central and parieto-occipital electrodes and lower APS values in lateral central electrodes (p = 0.007, ηp2 = 0.0408, with a meditator minus control t-max of 3.417 at PO5 and t-min of −3.035 at C5, see Fig. 10). This effect passed the duration control (83.5ms) used by Slagter et al. (2009). There was also a Group main effect in the TANOVA from −244 to −2 ms (p = 0.030, ηp2 = 0.0329) and brief significant interaction between Group and Interval in the APS TANOVA (150 to 280 ms, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.0364), both of which passed the duration control (83.5 ms) used by Slagter et al. (2009).

Fig. 10
figure 10

Alpha phase synchronisation (APS) topographical analysis of variance (TANOVA) comparisons time-locked to the onset of the first target stimuli (T1). Top left: The cumulative variance explained (ηp2) at each time point across the epoch by each main effect and condition, with each colour reflecting the ηp2 from the effect being tested, colour coded to match the p-graphs. Top right and middle: the p-graphs for the main effect of Interval (orange, middle left), Group (blue), and interaction between Interval and Group (yellow, middle right). The black line reflects the p-value, white areas reflect significant time points, and light blue periods reflect windows where the effect passed the duration controls used by Slagter et al. (2009). Bottom: Topography maps for APS averaged within the 990 to 1500 ms period for each group and the t-map of meditator APS minus control APS after normalisation for overall amplitude (so that all individuals had a GFP = 1). Red indicates areas where meditators showed higher values, blue indicates areas where controls showed higher values (indicating that topographical differences were present, without suggesting that APS was higher in the control group in a specific electrode, due to the normalisation for amplitude)

RMS APS averaged within the 282 to 1500 ms period significantly correlated to percentage correct for short interval T2 trials, in both short interval and long interval trials — for the correlation between APS RMS during short interval T1 trials and T2 short interval percentage correct: Pearson’s r = 0.314, p = 0.014, BF10 = 3.093, and for the correlation between APS RMS during long interval T1 trials and T2 short interval percentage correct: Pearson’s r = 0.307, p = 0.016, BF10 = 2.717. Scatterplots depicting these correlations can be viewed in the Supplementary Materials (Fig. S14). These correlations may indicate that participants who synchronised their alpha oscillations more consistently with the stimulus stream (which was presented at 10 Hz, within the alpha frequency) were better able to perceive and correctly identify the T2 stimuli. It is worth noting that the T2 stimuli in short interval trials were presented at 300 ms, just after the point at which the meditation group showed higher alpha synchronisation to the stimuli.

Behavioural and EEG Epoch Inclusion Comparisons

Levene’s test indicated the assumption of equality of variances was met for all conditions within the analysis of the behavioral data (all p > 0.15). However, the Shapiro-Wilk test indicated significant deviations from normality for 8/10 of the variables included in the Condition × Group combinations, so robust statistics were implemented in R, using the mixed ANOVA (bwtrim function) from the WRS2 package (Field and Wilcox, 2017). Violations of the assumptions of traditional parametric ANOVAs (including normality violations) do not affect these robust statistics. However, only Group × Condition designs are currently available (rather than Group × Condition × Condition), so this analysis was restricted to a Group × Interval comparison for T2 responses only (as the primary comparison of interest), and the originally planned parametric statistical analyses are reported in the Supplementary Materials (Section 3a). Means and standard deviations, as well as both parametric and robust statistics, are presented in Table 4, and the data can be viewed in Fig. 11.

Table 4 Attentional blink behavioural performance means (M), standard deviations (SD), and statistics for each group and condition. T1 the first target stimuli, T2 the second target stimuli
Fig. 11
figure 11

Attentional blink performance, measured in percentage correct for each group and condition. Long interval refers to conditions in which the T2 stimulus was presented 700 ms after T1. Short interval refers to conditions in which the T2 stimulus was presented 300 ms after T1. Figures on the left (T1) indicate the percentage of T1 stimuli correctly identified by each participant, whilst figures on the right (T2) indicate the percentage of T2 stimuli correctly identified by each participant. The single trial T1 label refers to T1-only trials (where no T2 stimulus was presented)

In testing our first replication hypothesis (RH1 — that our meditation group would show a reduced AB effect, with more correct responses to short interval T2 stimuli), the robust statistics showed no main effect of Group for percent correct in response to T2: value (1,33.997) = 0.325, p = 0.572, and no interaction between Group and Interval: value (1,33.898) = 0.220, p = 0.642. The parametric statistics showed the same pattern of null results. The Bayesian statistical model that included Group or interactions that involved Group as a factor was 259.326 less likely than the model that only included Target, Interval, and the interaction between Target and Interval (BF01 = 259.326). These results suggest it is highly unlikely that the meditation group showed higher percentage correct in any condition compared to the control group.

No main effects or interactions involving Group were significant for the number of epochs provided by each participant for each Condition (all p > 0.1). The TCT for the ERP data also showed mostly consistent neural activity across Groups and Conditions, with a brief period of inconsistency in the pattern of topographical activation within some Group × Condition combinations that did not overlap with any of our significant effects. These two tests indicate our ERP results were not driven simply by differences in the number of epochs included in ERP averages or inconsistent topographical activation within a single group or condition (details of these tests are reported in the Supplementary Materials, Table S1 and Fig. S1).

Discussion

This study aimed to comprehensively examine if neurophysiological markers of attention differed between community-meditators and non-meditator controls. In our sample of meditators with typical daily MM practice, our results did not show support for our primary hypotheses regarding the neurophysiological markers obtained from within our time windows of interest (the P3b, TPS, alpha-power, and APS, with the windows of interest overlapping with the significant effects reported by Slagter et al., 2007, 2009). No differences were found between meditators and non-meditators in the amplitude or distribution of the P3b neural response following T1 or T2 stimuli in the attention blink task. Nor were there any differences between meditators and non-meditators in TPS, alpha-power, or APS within our a priori selected time windows of interest. Frequentist statistics provided null results, and Bayesian statistics provided weak to moderate evidence against these primary hypotheses, suggesting we can be slightly to moderately confident there was no difference between the groups in TPS, alpha-power or APS within our a priori selected time windows of interest.

However, our exploratory analyses (which included all time points within the epochs around all T1/T2 and short/long interval conditions) did show significant effects, which were further supported by very strong Bayesian evidence in favour of the alternative hypothesis. In particular, meditators showed more equal posterior-N2 amplitudes across T1 and T2 stimuli than non-meditators (who showed larger posterior-N2 amplitudes to T1 than T2). Similarly, meditators showed more equal TPS values between the first and second target in short interval trials, and meditators showed similar TPS values to T2 in both short and long interval trials, in comparison to controls who showed higher TPS following the first target, and higher TPS to T2 in long interval compared to short interval trials. Meditators also showed lower alpha-power than controls during a period where short interval T2 stimuli would be processed, and increased APS to T1 stimuli. These effects are aligned with theoretical perspectives on the effects of mindfulness on attention function and align with the explanation that Slagter et al. (2007, 2009) provided for their results — that meditators distribute their neural activity more equally across stimuli, rather than biasing responses towards T1 (however, our results did not align with the time windows of significant results reported by Slagter et al., 2007, 2009). Each pattern of neural activity shown by the meditation group was also associated with higher performance, either correlated with percentage correct across all participants, or associated with correct responses rather than incorrect responses in single trial analyses, suggesting the activity shown by meditators might reflect functionally relevant attentional mechanisms. However, unexpectedly, our analyses of behavioral performance provided non-significant frequentist results, and our results showed strong overall Bayesian evidence against any main effect or interaction that involved Group. Combined with our null results for our primary analyses, this suggests caution is warranted in the interpretation of our results, and conclusions drawn from our exploratory analysis should be considered tentative. We discuss the details and implications of these findings in the following.

Our primary analysis did not detect a difference in the P3b following T1 stimuli in our sample of community-meditators. However, our exploratory analyses showed that the meditator group generated an equal amplitude posterior-N2 response across T1 and T2 stimuli, while controls showed higher posterior-N2 responses to T1 stimuli than T2 stimuli. As such, while our study did not replicate the findings reported by Slagter et al. (2007) with regard to the P3b, our result is conceptually similar, suggesting that meditators distributed attentional resources more equally across the two stimuli. While a frontally distributed N2 is often detected in tasks requiring cognitive control (Folstein & Van Petten, 2008), our study indicated the AB task generated a posterior-N2 instead, similar to previous research using the AB task (Zivony et al., 2018). Previous research in healthy control individuals has also demonstrated a reduced posterior-N2 to T2 stimuli following short interval trials, which has been suggested to reflect a lack of attentional engagement to enable stimuli processing (Zivony et al., 2018). As such, our results suggest that meditators are more equally distributing the engagement of attentional resources across the two AB stimuli. In support of this, an exploratory single trial analysis of the posterior-N2 GFP showed that correct identification of short interval T2 stimuli was associated with a smaller posterior-N2 GFP time-locked to T1, suggesting that when fewer attention resources were devoted to processing T1, T2 could be more accurately identified. As such, although the meditation group did not show higher task performance overall, their neural activity averaged within each condition showed the same pattern that was associated with higher performance.

It is not clear, however, why our study detected differences in the posterior-N2 rather than the P3b, given the findings reported by Slagter et al. (2007) of a difference in the P3b. This inconsistency might be explained by a progressive change of neural activity during the AB task with more intensive meditation experience. The sample tested by Slagter et al. (2007) underwent a 3-month intensive retreat, while our participants were experienced meditating members of the lay public (although with an average of 6 years of meditation experience, and an average of approximately 7 hr per week of practice at the time of the study). However, if the difference in meditation experience explains the conflict between our finding of a difference in the posterior-N2 compared to the finding reported by Slagter et al. (2007) of a difference in the P3b, it is not clear why the less experienced meditators in our study would show altered T1 processing at a shorter delay following T1 presentation than the sample tested by Slagter et al. (2007) of more experienced meditators. Despite the ambiguities in interpreting our results, the characterisation of meditators showing more equal distribution of posterior-N2 amplitudes between rapidly presented stimuli that compete for attentional resources aligns with previous research demonstrating mindfulness enhances the distribution of attentional resources (Bailey et al., 2018; Slagter et al., 2007, 2009; Wang et al., 2020).

Our primary analysis of TPS to short interval T2 trials showed no difference between meditators and controls. In contrast, our exploratory test of TPS showed strong Bayesian evidence of an interaction between TPS and long/short interval trial condition. This interaction indicated that while controls showed higher TPS to T2 for long interval trials than short interval trials, meditators showed similar TPS to T2 for both short and long interval trials. Strong Bayesian evidence also indicated that meditators showed a more even distribution of TPS between the first and second target in short interval trials, in comparison to controls who showed higher TPS following the first target. Multiple validation checks of this test demonstrated the same result. These validation checks included single electrode analyses averaged within our a priori time window of interest, and a repeat of the test that excluded participants who provided fewer epochs (ensuring the test possessed maximal validity). These results align with the interpretation proposed by Slagter et al. (2009) that theta synchronisation reflects increased consistency of neural processes, allowing increased attention as a result of meditation training. Our results also support this interpretation, indicating that theta synchronisation was higher following T2 in long interval trials than short interval trials (suggesting theta synchronisation to T2 is disrupted by T1 processing in short interval trials) and that higher theta synchronisation was related to performance.

However, despite the association between increased theta synchronisation and performance and our finding of a higher TPS in our meditation group, we found evidence against increased AB task accuracy in our meditation group. Our significant result also only overlapped with the first half of the window in which Slagter et al. (2009) detected increased TPS in their meditators after the retreat, and unlike Slagter et al. (2009), our TPS result was not present when the analysis was focused specifically on the difference between meditators and controls in TPS following short interval T2 trials. This may suggest that while typical community-meditation is associated with an effect on theta synchronisation attentional mechanisms, the theta synchronisation after stimulus presentation is not as prolonged as in post-intensive-retreat meditators. Additionally, the effect may be weaker, only appearing relative to the non-short interval T2 conditions (in which theta synchronisation is perhaps less vital for task performance than it is in the commonly attentional blinked short interval T2 condition). However, the more equal distribution of TPS to short interval T2 stimuli in meditators in our study may suggest that the meditation group is distributing limited attentional resources to better encode the T2 stimuli, as suggested by Slagter et al. (2009). The efficacy of this neural strategy seems to be reflected in the correlation between higher TPS and higher accuracy at accurately identifying short interval T2 stimuli. However, when correlations between TPS and performance were conducted within each group separately, only the correlations between TPS locked to T1 stimuli and short interval T2 accuracy remained significant. Additionally, these correlations were only significant within the control group. As such, it may be that TPS reflects a general mechanism enabling attentional focus on the task in the control group (with higher TPS to T1 reflecting an increase in overall attentional focus on the task, rather than accurate identification of T2 depending on TPS specifically locked to T2). In contrast, the relationship between TPS to a single target stimuli and performance in the meditation group may have been weakened, perhaps due to an alteration in the relationship between TPS to both stimuli (with meditators showing a more equal distribution of TPS across both T1 and T2 stimuli), or the influence of the posterior-N2 and alpha activity differences in the meditation group. As such, the functional interpretation of this result is not clear, more research is required to elucidate the finding, and the result should be interpreted with caution, as we note that these within-group correlations had reduced statistical power compared to the correlations across both groups, and that the confidence intervals for the correlation strengths from the two groups overlapped.

Our exploratory analysis of the distribution of TPS also indicated that meditators showed more TPS in occipital electrodes prior to T1 stimuli than controls. There was also a more consistent topographical distribution of activity within the meditation group than within the control group, perhaps indicating a consistent synchronisation of oscillations to the target stream in a functionally relevant brain region in preparation for the detection of the relevant stimuli. Similar to our findings for the posterior-N2, the pattern demonstrated where meditators showed a more equal distribution of theta activity between rapidly presented stimuli that compete for attentional resources provides further support for research that has indicated mindfulness enhances the functional allocation of attentional resources (Bailey et al., 2018; Slagter et al., 2007, 2009; Wang et al., 2020). However, if this interpretation is correct, it is not clear why the meditation group did not show higher accuracy than the control group. As such, our exploratory results should be viewed with caution, and require replication. It may be that ultimately research will show there is no significant difference in TPS between meditators and non-meditators.

The current study did not find a significant difference in our primary analyses focused on specific time windows within which we analysed alpha-power and alpha phase synchronisation (with time windows of interest derived from Slagter et al., 2009). However, in our exploratory analysis, the meditation group showed a larger reduction in the level of ongoing alpha-power from 475 to 685 ms following T1 stimuli (relative to the alpha-power across the rest of the epoch). Higher alpha-power has been associated with the inhibition of non-relevant brain regions during attention tasks, with the suggestion that this allows the brain to prioritise processing in brain regions that are relevant to the task, without the relevant brain regions being “distracted” by processing in non-relevant regions (Klimesch et al., 2007). In contrast, lower alpha-power is found in brain regions where active processing is required to complete the task, such that alpha-power can be increased to inhibit processing or decreased to enable processing in specific brain regions (Klimesch et al., 2007). In support of this interpretation of the function of alpha-power, previous research has shown higher levels of brain region–specific alpha-power modulation in experienced-meditators when attention is required to either tactile oddball or visual working memory stimuli (Wang et al., 2020). Results in that study indicated that alpha-power increased or decreased in specific task-relevant regions dependent on the specific task demands, and that meditators produced stronger task-relevant increases or decreases (Wang et al., 2020). The results also indicated that alongside the differences in alpha-power, meditators performed the task more accurately (Wang et al., 2020). The current study provides further support for the interpretation of alpha as an inhibitory mechanism, with alpha-power remaining high during distractor stimuli presentation but decreasing (releasing inhibition) earlier in short interval trials in alignment with short interval T2 processing, and decreasing later in long interval trials, in alignment with long interval T2 processing (see Figs. S8 and S9 in the Supplementary Materials Section 3e for a complete explanation and evidence in support of this point). This decrease in alpha-power during short interval T2 stimuli processing and increase in alpha within long interval trials during the same time period likely reflects a “gating” mechanism. In particular, the decrease in alpha power might reflect a release of inhibition to process target stimuli, while the increase in alpha power might reflect an increase of inhibition to reduce distractor processing. Indeed, lower alpha-power RMS within a 685 to 1050 ms window was strongly associated with short interval T2 correct responses (Supplementary Materials, Fig. S12).

As such, the results of the current study might suggest that the reduction in alpha-power immediately following the timing of the presentation of short interval T2 stimuli in the meditation group reflects an attentional mechanism. This attentional mechanism might enable increased neural processing during the period where processing of the short interval T2 stimuli would be required. This appears to occur regardless of whether the short interval T2 stimuli was presented or not presented, perhaps reflecting the fact that participants were unable to determine if the trial would be a short or long interval trial at the time they would have to engage this attentional mechanism (so engaged the mechanism regardless of the trial type). Two possible interpretations of the fact that meditators showed this prolonged alpha-power reduction to enable short interval T2 processing even for long interval trials are possible. The first is that it may reflect a neural activity pattern prioritising awareness in general. The second is that it may reflect increased carefulness. The increased processing of stimuli, regardless of whether they might be task-relevant, might reflect increased general awareness. Alternatively, the increased processing of the time period during which T2 might be present may indicate increased carefulness in anticipation of a potential T2 stimuli being presented. Some previous research has reported results that suggest the “increased awareness” interpretation is more likely — research using mathematical modelling of performance in a behavioral task has suggested that the improved attention function from mindfulness is related to enhancements in an individual’s ability to extract higher information quality during a working memory task rather than increased caution in responding (Van Vugt & Jha, 2011), a finding supported by neuroimaging research showing earlier activation of working memory-related brain regions in meditators (Bailey et al., 2020). Our task did not require participants to respond quickly, so it did not provide the ability to assess reaction times. However, previous results indicated meditators have shown increased performance without reaction time slowing (Van Vugt & Jha, 2011) and increased accuracy across both fast and slow reaction times (van den Hurk et al., 2010). In contrast, other research has indicated that meditators perform better in a movement task when the action required to meet the task goals is ambiguous and changing, and that they achieve this by performing a speed-accuracy trade-off for slower but more accurate responses (Naranjo & Schmidt, 2012). Trait mindfulness has also been shown to reduce the accelerating but accuracy-reducing effects of worry on performance (Hallion et al., 2020), supporting the “increased carefulness” interpretation. Further research may be able to elucidate the reasons for this pattern further.

While this pattern whereby meditators may have shown prolonged alpha-power reduction to enable short interval T2 processing even for long interval trials and our suggested interpretations of the pattern would have had no effect on task-relevant stimulus perception and, therefore, could not lead to improved task performance, the pattern does align with the “non-judgemental” aspect of mindfulness practice — maintaining awareness of the present moment as it is, without evaluation. This contrasts with the pattern shown by the controls, which indicates they reduced the processing of non-target distractor stimuli within the short interval T2 period, eliminating the distractor stimuli from awareness. As might be expected, given the lack of relevance to task performance of this neural strategy, across all participants, averaged alpha-power within the time window where meditators showed reduced alpha activity did not correlate with the accuracy of short interval T2 detection. In fact, our exploratory analysis indicated that incorrect responses on short interval trials were associated with slightly, but significantly, lower alpha-power within this window than correct responses (Supplementary Materials Section 3e, S11). This might provide support for a conjecture that the careful or non-judgemental neural strategy of the meditators prioritised present moment awareness at the expense of accurate task performance. However, alpha-power RMS was also strongly correlated between the earlier (during-T2 processing) and later (post-T2 processing) alpha power time periods. This relationship was also stronger within incorrect trials than for correct trials. As such, it may be that the alpha-power reduction during the earlier (during-T2 processing) period might reflect a preparatory mechanism that attempted to engage attention when attention had drifted, so that the neural activity required for successful task performance in the later (post-T2 processing) window would be present. We note that at this stage, these explanations are conjecture. Alternatively, it may simply be that the lower alpha-power in meditators during the earlier (during-T2 processing) period reflects a non-optimal neural activation in the context of the task. Further research is required to test whether our exploratory results can be replicated, and to determine which explanation is correct.

Similar to the alpha-power results, our study did not find a significant difference in our primary analysis focused on specific time windows, within which we analysed alpha phase synchronisation in replication of the results reported by Slagter et al. (2009). However, in contrast with the lower alpha-power during the short interval T2 stimuli time window, the meditation group showed a prolonged period of higher alpha synchronisation to T1. Meditators also showed a different scalp distribution of alpha synchronisation to T1, with more parietal and frontal APS than controls. While alpha-power has been associated with the inhibition of brain regions that are not relevant for processing the current attention task (Klimesch et al., 2007), the same relationship has not been reported for APS. Indeed, the correlation between APS and task performance in our study, along with the more occipital distribution in the meditation group, suggests that inhibition of non-relevant brain regions (in our visual task) is not likely to be the explanation for the higher APS in our meditation group. Instead, we suspect the increased APS in our meditation group reflects synchronisation to the ongoing stream of stimuli presentation timing (as stimuli were presented at 10 Hz, within the alpha frequency). Previous research has suggested that the synchronisation of ongoing endogenous neural oscillations to external stimuli may increase the likelihood of neurons firing in response to those stimuli, which is then related to the increased encoding of those stimuli into working memory (Buzsáki & Moser, 2013; Fujisawa & Buzsáki, 2011; Lisman & Buzsáki, 2008; O’Neill et al., 2013). This process is likely to reflect a mechanism underlying attention function, and a similar phenomenon may underlie the alpha synchronisation to stimuli in the current study. As such, it may be that the attentional training the meditation group had undertaken increased their ability to time lock their alpha oscillations to stimuli in occipital regions responsible for processing the visual stimuli, and frontal regions responsible for attending to the stimuli. We note here that it might be valuable to analyse connectivity between these regions in future research.

While our results suggest differences in neural activity in meditators that align with improved attention function, the meditator and control groups did not differ in task performance. There are a number of potential explanations for this null result, as well as the null results for our primary analyses. For the sake of brevity, these are summarised here, and explained in full in the Supplementary Materials (Section 4). Firstly, the behavioral effects of meditation in the AB task may be dependent on a meditation-induced mindful state, or particular types of meditative practices, which may not have been sampled in our study. Secondly, it may be that more meditation experience is required before differences in AB task performance are detected, or that the AB task we used was not sensitive enough to detect differences between our groups. On this point, we note that the effects of meditation on attention function reported in meta-analyses are small (Sumantry & Stewart, 2021), so may be easily “washed out” by variations in context, such as the use of a task with lower sensitivity, a factor that may explain the not uncommon null results reported by studies of mindfulness and attention (Bailey et al., 2018; Osborn et al., 2022; Payne et al., 2020). Age may have also been a factor — perhaps meditation protects against age-related decline in AB performance, and our young meditation group had not aged enough to show this effect. Indeed, Slagter’s participant’s median age was 41, whereas the median age of our meditation group was 35, and ERP latency is known to increase with age (Polich, 1997). However, these explanations seem unlikely given our meditators were more experienced than those included in many studies, our task replicated a number of previous AB task studies that did detect differences, and some research has indicated older meditators showed improved AB task performance compared to both age-matched controls and a younger control group (van Leeuwen et al., 2009). Next, our study design differed from Slagter et al. (2007, 2009) — most notably in that their study involved the repetition of the AB task before and after an intensive retreat whereas our study focused on community-meditators. It may be that MM is not associated with generalised better performance in the AB task, but rather an increased ability to learn the task and as a result, increased performance on the second repetition of the task following meditation practice. This feature meant that the within-subject design used by Slagter et al. (2007, 2009) controlled for interindividual variability, while our between-groups study did not. Overall, there are a number of potential explanations for our null result with regard to our behavioral measures, and it may be useful for future research to systematically explore variations in task parameters, participant ages, test-retest performance, and other factors to determine the parameters under which meditators do show improved AB task (or attention task) performance.

Our study also included updated EEG analysis methods from Slagter et al. (2007, 2009). Most notably, the current study used a high pass filter of 0.25 Hz, whereas Slagter et al. (2007) used a high pass filter of 1 Hz. The amplitude of ERPs, including the P3b, has been shown to be produced at least in part by < 1 Hz activity, and are adversely affected by high pass filtering out < 1 Hz data (Rousselet, 2012; Tanner et al., 2016). As such, the P3b data Slagter et al. (2007) analysed may have had considerable signal removed from the P3b, and their analysis may have been adversely affected. Lastly, it may be that either our result or the results reported by Slagter et al. (2007, 2009) are spurious, reflecting a sampling bias, chance-like effect, or similar “non-effect of interest.” However, we note that a spurious chance-like result is less likely in studies with a larger sample size, as per the current study, according to Stevens (2017).

As such, our results indicate that the specific alterations detected by previous research, including those to the P3b (within a specific window of interest), increased T2-locked TPS, and improved performance on short interval AB trials, are not necessarily markers of regular mindfulness meditation practice. Despite the potential explanations outlined in the previous sections for the differences between the meditator and control group in our study, these findings were exploratory and were not controlled for experiment-wise multiple comparisons. As such, it is possible that there are simply no differences between groups and that ultimately, previous mindfulness experience may not result in behavioral improvements in the AB task (although unlikely given the number of positive findings, even if the findings were exploratory). Although our EEG findings are uncertain, our behavioural results provide confidence in the null result for differences in task performance. This was surprising as it conflicted with previous findings (Slagter et al., 2007; Slagter et al., 2009). It was especially surprising considering that the meditators in the current study reported at least 2 years of meditative practice, which we expected would be sufficient to produce differences in attention performance if MM did indeed affect attention. From our perspective, the most likely explanation for the difference between our results and those of Slagter et al. (2007, 2009) is that our participants were regular meditators, whereas theirs were tested before and after a 3-month retreat. As such, when viewing both studies together, our results suggest that differences in AB performance among meditators may be exclusively present following intensive meditation interventions.

It may be that the type of attention captured by the AB task is less relevant to the attention trained through mindfulness meditation practice. This interpretation is supported by our alpha-power findings, which suggested meditators may not have engaged alpha to inhibit distractor processing when short interval T2 stimuli were absent as strongly as the controls. Other EEG markers or neuroimaging methods using different attention tasks may be better suited to detect differences between meditation and control groups, and the null results for behavioral analyses in the current study may help refine our understanding of exactly which mechanisms are altered (and which are not altered) by meditation practice. With AB literature suffering from a lack of published replications, the present study also underscores the importance of replication studies in different populations and contexts, as some of the effects of meditation may be specific to certain populations only (Bailey, Raj, et al., 2019b; Osborn et al., 2022; Vago et al., 2019; Van Dam et al., 2018). Slagter et al. (2007) have been cited over 1000 times, yet this is the first even partial replication attempt, which, despite using a larger sample size, revealed null results for our replication of the outcome measures reported by Slagter et al. (2007, 2009).

Limitations and Future Directions

The most obvious limitation of our study is that it utilised a cross-sectional design. A longitudinal approach, assessing participants before and after meditation practice, may allow for the determination of causality. However, we note that this is difficult to achieve with the level of meditation experience tested in the current study. Another limitation of this study was that it utilised a broad definition of meditation (Kabat-Zinn, 1994) and included both “focused attention” and “open monitoring” practitioners. Meditation literature is unclear on the direct impact of different varieties of meditation practice on AB performance, with research suggesting both focused attention and open monitoring meditation affect AB performance (van Leeuwen et al., 2009), other research suggesting AB performance is exclusively impacted by open monitoring meditation (Colzato et al., 2015), and some studies suggest neither practice affects AB performance (Sharpe et al., 2021). While delineating between the different MM practices and their potential impacts may be valuable, the conclusions that can be drawn from our broad sample may be more reflective of everyday mindfulness meditators in the community. It would also be interesting to assess the potential dose-response relationship between mindfulness practice and the differences in neural activity we have reported. Unfortunately, our sample size was likely too small to provide a good test of a potential dose-response relationship, and the measures of meditation experience we obtained are not likely to provide a robust assessment of meditation experience, so we did not conduct this analysis in our study. It would be interesting for future research to consider potential dose-response relationships. Finally, it is important to emphasize that the significant results detected in our study were only from our exploratory analyses, and our primary analyses replicating the effects demonstrated by Slagter et al. (2007, 2009) did not show significant results. Furthermore, there was no difference in behavioral accuracy between the groups, and this was unlikely to be due to a ceiling effect (with a mean short interval T2 accuracy of 67.1% for meditators and 63.9% for controls). As such, it is not clear the potential meditation-related differences in neural activities are meaningful, and replication is required to test our interpretations of the potential functional relevance of differences in neural activity in our meditator group (for additional strengths and limitations of the study, see the Supplementary Materials).