Older surgical patients with preexisting cognitive impairment are at increased risk for developing postoperative delirium.1,2 Screening surgical patients for preexisting cognitive impairment, however, has yet to become routine clinical practice, in part because many cognitive screening tests are time-intensive and require special training to administer.

Although abbreviated cognitive screening tests exist, few have been studied in the preoperative setting.3 One of these brief cognitive screens is the animal fluency test (AFT). It requires patients to name as many animals as possible within 60 sec, with one point given for each unique animal. Scores < 15 points have demonstrated high sensitivity and specificity (88% and 96%, respectively) for the presence of Alzheimer dementia in the memory clinic setting.4 In addition to its brevity, the AFT has the advantage of requiring limited training to administer it correctly.

We designed a study to investigate the feasibility of administering the AFT before surgery and its effectiveness for predicting postoperative delirium. We hypothesized that patients who scored < 15 on the AFT immediately before surgery would be more likely to develop postoperative delirium than patients who scored ≥ 15.

Methods

The University of California, San Francisco Human Research Protection Program, Committee on Human Research approved the study in February 2013, and the requirement for written informed consent was waived. The study was conducted between July 1 and December 31, 2012 at a single, large university hospital. Inclusion criteria were consecutive patients > 65 yr of age who underwent hip, knee, or spine surgery and who required a postoperative stay of more than 24 hr. We chose patients undergoing these types of surgery because prior research has shown that they are typically older and have a high incidence of postoperative delirium.5 Exclusion criteria were non-English speakers, hearing or speaking impairment (e.g., preoperative endotracheal intubation), and preexisting neurological impairment (e.g., dementia, altered mental status).

Before study commencement, all anesthesia providers – residents in training, nurse anesthetists, and faculty anesthesiologists – were shown a video that demonstrated how to administer the AFT. The AFT was then administered prospectively by the anesthesia providers during the preoperative interview, which typically took place immediately before surgery. The AFT scores were entered into the electronic medical record. If the AFT was not administered preoperatively, the anesthesia providers were instructed to provide the reason(s) why. They reported clinician oversight or time constraint as reasons.

One investigator (L.S.L.) measured postoperative delirium using a previously validated chart-based tool published by Inouye et al.,6 after which the results were validated by a second investigator (J.M.L.), who was blinded to L.S.L.’s delirium designation. The two investigators resolved disagreements by reviewing the chart together to achieve consensus. The chart-based tool to measure delirium was previously validated with interviewer ratings using the Confusion Assessment Method in a large cohort of hospitalized older adults. Results indicated that it had a sensitivity of 74% and specificity of 83%. Finally, a third investigator (J.T.W.) determined which patients had taken the AFT preoperatively. Because AFT scores were not abstracted until delirium was diagnosed, investigators who made the diagnosis had been blinded to the AFT scores. The medical records were also abstracted to obtain data on demographics, length of hospital stay, intensive care unit (ICU) admission status, discharge disposition, and in-hospital mortality.

Bivariate analyses were conducted using Chi-square and t-tests. The final model was constructed using multiple logistic regression analysis. Statistical analysis was conducted using Stata® 8.0 software (College Station, TX, USA) or Microsoft Excel® for Mac 2011.

Results

During the six-month period, 432 patients were examined and 362 satisfied the study inclusion/exclusion criteria. Reasons for excluding patients were communication difficulty (n = 32/432, 7%), patient refusal or anxiety (n = 23, 5%), and neurological impairment (n = 15, 4%). A number of patients satisfied the inclusion/exclusion criteria but were not AFT screened because of time constraint (n = 14/362, 4%) or clinician oversight (n = 149, 41%). The remaining 199 patients (55%) formed the sample for this report (Fig. 1). The demographics of patients included in this report were similar to those who were excluded/not screened with respect to mean age, sex, American Society of Anesthesiologists Physical Status (ASA) classification, emergency case status, rate of ICU admission, and rate of postoperative delirium (see Table 1). These two groups differed only by surgery type.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Enrolment flowchart

Table 1 Characteristics of patients included in this report vs those who were excluded or not screened with the AFT

Of the 199 patients who were screened with the AFT, 57 (29%) scored < 15. The overall rate of postoperative delirium as measured by chart review was 19% (n = 38, 95% confidence interval [CI]: 14 to 25%). Patients with AFT scores < 15 were more likely to develop postoperative delirium than patients with scores ≥ 15 (54% vs 5%, P < 0.01) (Fig. 2). This finding was supported by receiver operating characteristic curve analysis, which yielded an area under the curve of 0.83 (95% CI: 0.76 to 0.90), suggesting that the AFT was able to identify patients at risk for delirium (Fig. 3). At the cutoff of < 15 used in this study, the sensitivity and specificity were 84% and 75%, respectively, and the positive and negative likelihood ratios were 3.39 and 0.21, respectively.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Number of patients with and without postoperative delirium at any given animal fluency test (AFT) score. The AFT scores (range 4-36) are shown on the horizontal axis. Numbers of patients with those AFT scores are shown on the vertical axis. Blue bars = patients who developed postoperative delirium; red bars = patients who did not develop delirium; bars to the left of the green dotted line = patients whose AFT scored was < 15. It is evident that patients with scores < 15 were more likely to develop delirium, as seen by the predminance of blue bars to the left of the green dotted line and red bars to the right of it

Fig. 3
figure 3

A receiver operating characteristic curve was generated to determine the sensitivity and specificity of the AFT for predicting postoperative delirium

Patients with AFT scores < 15 were more likely to be ≥ 80 yr of age (23% vs 11%, P = 0.02) and had a high ASA classification (III or IV) (68% vs 44%, P < 0.01). Multiple logistic regression analysis, with postoperative delirium as the dependent variable, identified an AFT score < 15 (crude odds ratio [OR] 19.8, 95% CI: 7.7 to 51.4; adjusted OR 20.1, 95% CI: 7.9 to 51.4) and high ASA classification (crude OR 3.3, 95% CI: 1.2 to 8.8; adjusted OR 3.5, 95% CI: 1.3 to 9.2) as independent predictors. Additional covariates not significant in the final model were age, sex, ICU admission, and emergency case status (see Table 2).

Table 2 Crude and adjusted odds ratios for predictor variables of postoperative delirium

Lastly, patients with postoperative delirium as measured by chart review had worse clinical outcomes than non-delirious patients. They had a higher ICU admission rate (42%, 95% CI: 26 to 59% vs 16%, 95% CI: 10 to 22%, P < 0.01), longer length of hospital stay (7.8 days, 95% CI: 5.1 to 6.1 vs 5.6 days, 95% CI: 6.2 to 9.4, P < 0.01), higher rate of institutionalization after discharge (92%, 95% CI: 79 to 98% vs 42%, 95% CI: 35 to 50%, P < 0.01), and higher mortality rate (5%, 95% CI: 1 to 18% vs 0.6%, 95% CI: 0 to 3%, P = 0.03).

Discussion

Our study shows that older patients who scored < 15 on the AFT before surgery were more likely to become delirious postoperatively, and that patients who became delirious had worse clinical outcomes overall. Our results are novel in demonstrating an association between a 60-sec preoperative cognitive screening test and the development of postoperative delirium as measured by chart review.

A previous small study was conducted on a group of patients aged 70 yr or older who were undergoing proximal femoral fracture repair and were free of known dementia and delirium preoperatively. Results showed that a brief cognitive screen – which included a three-word recall test, simplified clock drawing, and spatial orientation – predicted the risk of postoperative delirium in a univariate analysis and after adjustment for age.7 Our results are in agreement with the conclusion of that study: that a limited cognitive screen such as a one-minute fluency test can be prognostically useful for identifying patients at risk of developing postoperative delirium. One potential advantage of the AFT – in contrast to other cognitive screens, such as the Mini Cog8 – is that it can be administered in person or over the telephone. Tests that include a clock drawing, for example, require in-person testing, and the results may be difficult to store and retrieve.

Our study was not powered to examine the relative importance of all known risk predictors of postoperative delirium. In addition to AFT scores < 15, however, a high ASA physical status independently predicted the development of postoperative delirium, which was demonstrated previously.1 Although advanced age is also an established risk factor for postoperative delirium, we did not find an independent association in our analysis. This discrepancy may be explained by the selection bias of including only older patients, thereby truncating the age distribution and attenuating the effect of age on delirium.

A key aspect of this study was to examine the potential barriers of adding a cognitive screen to the routine preoperative evaluation. With a screening rate of 55%, our study suggests that the completion rate could be augmented in future, larger studies. Given that clinician oversight was the most common reason cited for not administering the AFT, education and reinforcement of this practice as part of a routine preoperative evaluation may improve its success. More importantly, investigators should seek better understanding of the barriers to administering a preoperative cognitive screen so strategies can be developed to overcome them.

The overall 19% rate of delirium reported in our study is comparable to that in previous studies of older surgical patients undergoing major surgery.1,9-11 The incidence of delirium has ranged from 10-62%, with higher rates reported in higher-risk patients, such as those with a hip fracture.12 In contrast, studies on more homogeneous patient populations, such as those undergoing elective orthopedic arthroplasty, have reported delirium rates of approximately 10%.10,13

There are limitations to our study. First, this one-minute cognitive screen may not be sensitive enough to identify mild cognitive impairment. Second, we investigated only the cutoff value of < 15. We did not consider other values, which need to be investigated in future larger studies. Third, the educational background and/or anxiety state of patients may influence test performance. Fourth, we included only patients undergoing spine surgery or arthroplasty, so the results may not be generalizable to patients undergoing other types of surgery. Fifth, a large number of patients were not screened with the AFT, mostly because of clinician oversight, which may have introduced a selection bias. Sixth, because we found that patients who developed delirium were more likely to be admitted to the ICU and had a longer length of hospital stay, it is possible their mental status was assessed more frequently, resulting in an observation bias. Finally, the choice of the chart-based tool to screen for incident delirium may result in misclassification. For example, a recent study demonstrated that a chart-based tool was more likely to miss patients with hypoactive delirium.14 However, the patients in our study who were deemed delirious as measured by chart review had worse postoperative outcomes than non-delirious patients, supporting the validity of the chart-based tool used for measuring delirium. Future studies with adequate sample size should include a standardized, validated tool to measure delirium actively and prospectively. It should be noted that this was an initial investigation into the feasibility of administering the AFT and its effectiveness for predicting delirium. Accordingly, we did not measure a large number of covariates for postoperative delirium. A future study with adequate sample size is needed that includes consideration of additional covariates for delirium development.

In conclusion, a simple, brief preoperative cognitive screening test, such as the AFT, is potentially useful when applied prior to surgery. Based on our data, the AFT was able to identify older surgical patients at risk for developing postoperative delirium as measured by chart review. Although limited participation by eligible participants in this study raises questions about the usefulness and feasibility of systematic administration of the AFT, our study adds to the growing “call for action” for anesthesiologists to incorporate cognitive assessment in the preoperative evaluation of older surgical patients.15