Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Psychological Pathways Through Which Social Norms and Social Identity Influence Eating Behavior: Testing a Conceptual Model

  • Full length manuscript
  • Published:
International Journal of Behavioral Medicine Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Background

Although interventions frequently promote healthy eating, failing to consider psychosocial factors, such as social norms, may limit the effectiveness of these efforts. Perceived social norms are a well-documented determinant of eating behavior; however, there is limited understanding of the processes through which, and for whom, this relationship emerges. Using identity-based motivation as a theoretical framework, we present a conceptual model identifying one route through which descriptive social norms—beliefs about how others behave—predict eating behavior, and test whether this process varies across social identities (e.g., self-perceived weight status).

Method

Structured telephone interviews were conducted for a national sample of non-diabetic adults who identified as non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, or Mexican American (n = 990).

Results

Multigroup SEM analysis comparing individuals who self-identified as overweight (versus “about the right weight” and underweight) demonstrated that perceiving descriptive social norms that people do not eat healthy foods predicted greater perceived barriers to eating healthy foods. Perceived barriers, in turn, predicted stronger beliefs that body weight is uncontrollable, and this relationship was stronger for participants who self-identified as overweight (relative to participants who did not identify as overweight). These beliefs subsequently predicted greater self-reported consumption of unhealthy foods (e.g., sweets), but did not predict consumption of fruits or vegetables.

Conclusions

This study extends our understanding of a psychosocial process that predicts consumption of unhealthy foods and underscores the importance of social identities for shaping responses to perceived norms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Availability of Data and Materials

Data is available from the corresponding author upon request.

Notes

  1. Although the survey included two additional food categories that are not reported in the main text (fruit drinks and bread-like foods, such as tortillas), we focused our analyses on food categories that have high consensus about being (un)healthy or (un)processed (e.g., although there is high consensus that sweets are unhealthy, there is relatively less consensus regarding the healthiness of bread-like foods) [50].

  2. Although this is a conceptual measure and the single items may not be highly correlated, using the index is a more powerful predictor of the category (i.e., unhealthy foods). Thus, we collapsed across food categories given our interest in unhealthy eating broadly (rather than a specific type of behavior). Analyses measuring each type of food separately are reported in the online supplement.

  3. Although previous research assesses self-perceived weight using these labels, we acknowledge that the term “overweight” is pejorative. However, to maintain consistency with how participants responded to this measure, participants will be described as individuals who (do not) “self-identify as overweight” throughout the paper.

  4. Although perceived barriers were transformed into a count variable, it was not estimated with a Poisson distribution because this distribution cannot be used with negative values (as generated by our standardized variables). Therefore, we tested our model without Poisson distribution to avoid issues with interpretability caused by having both standardized and unstandardized coefficients in the model. Use of the Poisson distribution did not change the reported results, and this model is reported in the online supplement for interested readers.

  5. When completing the survey, some participants volunteered responses, particularly on the eating outcomes, that we retained in the dataset to mitigate a substantial loss of statistical power (e.g., 30% of the sample volunteered a response of “never” in response to drinking regular soda). However, exclusion of these responses showed no significant impact on the pattern of reported results. These analyses are reported in the online supplement (Table S4).

References

  1. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. State indicator report on fruits and vegetables, 2018. Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. 2018. Retrieved from: https://www.cdc.gov/nutrition/downloads/fruits-vegetables/2018/2018-fruit-vegetable-report-508.pdf

  2. Krebs-Smith SM, Guenther PM, Subar AF, Kirkpatrick SI, Dodd KW. Americans do not meet federal dietary recommendations. J Nutr. 2010;140(10):1832–8. https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.110.124826.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Steele EM, Baraldi LG, da Costa Louzada ML, Moubarac J, Mozaffarian D, Monteiro CA. Ultra-processed and added sugars in the US diet: evidence from a nationally representative cross-sectional study. BMJ Open. 2016;6(3):e009892. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2015-009892.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Poor nutrition. 2019. Retrieved from: https://www.cdc.gov/chronicdisease/resources/publications/factsheets/nutrition.htm

  5. Jacka FN, Pasco JA, Mykletun A, Williams LJ, Hodge AM, O’Reilly SL, Nicholson GC, Kotowicz MA, Berk M. Association of Western and traditional diets with depression and anxiety in women. Am J Psychiatry. 2010;167(3):305–11. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060881.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. National Institutes of Health. Managing overweight and obesity in adults: systematic evidence review from the Obesity Expert Panel. 2013. Retrieved from: https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/managing-overweight-obesity-in-adults/

  7. Sherif M. The psychology of social norms. Oxford, England: Harper. 1936.

  8. Ajzen I, Fishbein M. Understanding attitudes and predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Hochbaum G, Rosenstock I, Kegels S. Health belief model. Washington, DC: United States Public Health Service. 1952.

    Google Scholar 

  10. Griffith DM, King A, Ober Allen J. Male peer influence on African American men’s motivation for physical activity: men’s and women’s perspectives. Am J Mens Health. 2013;7(2):169–78. https://doi.org/10.1177/1557988312465887.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Higgs S. Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite. 2015;86:38–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Park HS, Klein KA, Smith S, Martell D. Separating subjective norms, university descriptive and injunctive norms, and U.S. descriptive and injunctive norms for drinking behavior intentions. Health Commun. 2009;24(8):746–751. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410230903265912.

  13. Pelletier JE, Graham DJ, Laska MN. Social norms and dietary behaviors among young adults. Am J Health Behav. 2014;38(1):144–52. https://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.38.1.15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Robinson E, Thomas J, Aveyard P, Higgs S. What everyone else is eating: a systematic review and meta-analysis of the effect of informational eating norms on eating behavior. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2014;114(3):414–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jand.2013.11.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Cialdini RB, Reno RR, Kallgren CA. A focus theory of normative conduct: recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;58(6):1015–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.58.6.1015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Smith JR, Louis WR, Terry DJ, Greenaway KH, Clarke MR, Cheng X. Congruent or conflicted? The impact of injunctive and descriptive norms on environmental intentions. J Environ Psychol. 2012;32(4):353–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2012.06.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Lally P, Bartle N, Wardle J. Social norms and diet in adolescents. Appetite. 2011;57(3):623–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.07.015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Mollen S, Rimal RN, Ruiter RA, Kok G. Healthy and unhealthy social norms and food selection. Findings from a field-experiment. Appetite. 2013;65:83–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2013.01.020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Robinson E, Fleming A, Higgs S. Prompting healthier eating: testing the use of health and social norm-based messages. Health Psychol. 2014;33(9):1057–64. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Staunton M, Louis WR, Smith JR, Terry DJ, McDonald RI. How negative descriptive norms for healthy eating undermine the effects of positive injunctive norms. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2014;44(4):319–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Zou X, Savani K. Descriptive norms for me, injunctive norms for you: using norms to explain the risk gap. Judgm Decis Mak. 2019;14(6):644–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Carpenter CJ. A meta-analysis of the effectiveness of health belief model variables in predicting behavior. Health Commun. 2010;25(8):661–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2010.521906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Fishbein M, Ajzen I. Predicting and changing behavior: the reasoned action approach. New York, NY: Psychology Press. 2011.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  24. Murphy CC, Vernon SW, Diamond PM, Tiro JA. Competitive testing of health behavior theories: how do benefits, barriers, subjective norm, and intention influence mammography behavior? Ann Behav Med. 2013;47(1):120–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-013-9528-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Sparkman G, Walton GM. Witnessing change: dynamic norms help resolve diverse barriers to personal change. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2019;82:238–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Fishbein M, Azjen I. Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: an introduction to theory and research. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Rosenstock IM. The health belief model and preventive health behavior. Health Educ Behav. 1974;2(4):354–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/109019817400200405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Salmon J, Owen N, Crawford D, Bauman A, Sallis JF. Physical activity and sedentary behavior: a population-based study of barriers, enjoyment, and preference. Health Psychol. 2003;222:178–88. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.22.2.178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Strecher VJ, DeVellis BM, Becker MH, Rosenstock IM. The role of self-efficacy in achieving health behavior change. Health Educ Q. 1986;13(1):73–92. https://doi.org/10.1177/109019818601300108.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Shen L, Condit CM, Wright L. The psychometric property and validation of a fatalism scale. Psychol Health. 2009;24(5):597–613. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870440801902535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Dar-Nimrod I, Cheung BY, Ruby MB, Heine SJ. Can merely learning about obesity genes affect eating behavior? Appetite. 2014;81:269–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.06.109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Drew EM, Schoenberg NE. Deconstructing fatalism: ethnographic perspectives on women’s decision making about cancer prevention and treatment. Med Anthropol Q. 2011;25(2):164–82. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1548-1387.2010.01136.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Niederdeppe J, Levy AG. Fatalistic beliefs about cancer prevention and three prevention behaviors. Cancer Epidemiol Biomarkers Prev. 2007;16(5):998–1003. https://doi.org/10.1158/1055-9965.EPI-06-0608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. San Diego ERN, Merz EL. Diabetes knowledge, fatalism and type 2 diabetes-preventive behavior in an ethnically diverse sample of college students. J Am Coll Health. 2020;1–10. https://doi.org/10.1080/07448481.2020.1751175.

  35. Walker RJ, Smalls BL, Hernandez-Tejada MA, Campbell JA, Davis KS, Egede LE. Effect of diabetes fatalism on medication adherence and self-care behaviors in adults with diabetes. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2012;34(6):598–603. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2012.07.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Inzlicht M, Good C. How environments threaten academic performance, self-knowledge, and sense of belonging. In: Levin S, van Laar C, editors. Stigma and group inequality: social psychological approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 2006;129–50.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Guendelman MD, Cheryan S, Monin B. Fitting in but getting fat: identity threat and dietary choices among U.S. immigrant groups. Psychol Sci. 2011;22(7), 959–967. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611411585.

  38. Liu J, Thomas JM, Higgs S. The relationship between social identity, descriptive social norms and eating intentions and behaviors. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2019;82:217–30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2019.02.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Hunger JM, Major B, Blodorn A, Miller CT. Weighed down by stigma: how weight-based social identity threat contributes to weight gain and poor health. Soc Pers Psychol Compass. 2015;9(6):255–68. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Steele CM, Spencer SJ, Aronson J. Contending with group image: the psychology of stereotype and social identity threat. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 2002;34:379–440. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(02)80009-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Oyserman D. Social identity and self-regulation. In: Kruglanski AW, Higgins ET, editors. Social psychology: handbook of basic principles. The Guilford Press. 2007;432–53.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Oyserman D, Fryberg SA, Yoder N. Identity-based motivation and health. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2007;93(6):1011–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.6.1011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Oyserman D, Smith GC, Elmore K. Identity-based motivation: implications for health and health disparities. J Soc Issues. 2014;70(2):206–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Eikenberry N, Smith C. Healthful eating: perceptions, motivations, barriers, and promoters in low-income Minnesota communities. J Am Diet Assoc. 2004;104(7):1158–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jada.2004.04.023.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Stankevitz K, Dement J, Schoenfisch A, Joyner J, Clancy S, Stroo M, Ostbye T. Perceived barriers to healthy eating and physical activity among participants in a workplace obesity intervention. J Occup Environ Med. 2017;59(8):746–51. https://doi.org/10.1097/JOM.0000000000001092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. MacCallum RC, Zhang S, Preacher KJ, Rucker DD. On the practice of dichotomization of quantitative variables. Psychol Methods. 2002;7:19–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Preston CC, Colman AM. Optimal number of response categories in rating scales: reliability, validity, discriminating power, and respondent preferences. Acta Psychol. 2000;104(1):1–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0001=6918(99)00050-5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  48. O'Connell JK, Price JH. Development of a heart disease locus of control scale. Psychol Rep. 1985;56(1):159–164. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1985.56.1.159, https://doi.org/10.1111/josi.12056.

  49. Ley SH, Hamdy O, Viswanathan M, Hu FB. Prevention and management of type 2 diabetes: dietary components and nutritional strategies. Lancet. 2014;383(9933):1999–2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(14)60613-9.

  50. US Department of Health and Human Services and US Department of Agriculture. 2015;2015-2020 Dietary guidelines for Americans. Retrieved from: http://health.gov/dietaryguidelines/2015/guidelines/.

  51. Sonneville KR, Thurston IB, Milliren CE, Gooding HC, Richmond TK. Weight misperception among young adults with overweight/obesity associated with disordered eating behaviors. Int J Eat Disord. 2016;49(10):937-46. https://doi.org/10.1002/eat.22565

  52. Thurston IB, Sonneville KR, Milliren CE, Kamody RC, Gooding HC, Richmond TK. Cross-sectional and prospective examination of weight misperception and depressive symptoms among youth with overweight and obesity. Prev Sci. 2017;18(2):152–63. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121-016-0714-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Major B, Eliezer D, Rieck H. The psychological weight of weight stigma. Soc Psychol Personal Sci. 2012;3(6):651-8.https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611434400.

  54. Pfeffermann D. The use of sampling weights for survey data analysis. Stat Methods Med Res. 1996;5(3):239–61. https://doi.org/10.1177/096228029600500303.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  55. Chen Q, Elliott MR, Haziza D, Yang Y, Ghosh M, Little RJ, Sedransk J, Thompson M. Approaches to improving survey-weighted estimates. Stat Sci. 2017;32(2):227–48. https://doi.org/10.1214/17-STS609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Haziza D, Beaumont JF. Construction of weights in surveys: a review. Stat Sci. 2017;32(2):206–26. https://doi.org/10.1214/16-STS608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Battaglia MP, Hoaglin DC, Frankel MR. Practical considerations in raking survey data. Surv Pract. 2009;2(5):1–12. https://doi.org/10.29115/SP-2009-0019.

  58. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. (3rd ed.). Guilford Press. 2011.

  59. Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1991;50(2):179–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Puhl RM, Moss-Racusin CA, Schwartz MB. Internalization of weight bias: implications for binge-eating and emotional well-being. Obesity. 2012;15(1):19–23. https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2007.521.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Versluis I, Papies EK. The role of social norms in the portion size effect: reducing normative relevance reduces the effect of portion size on consumption decisions. Front Psychol. 2016;7:756. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Stok FM, Verkooijen KT, de Ridder DTD, de Wit JBF, de Vet E. How norms work: self-identification, attitude, and self-efficacy mediate the relation between descriptive social norms and vegetable intake. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. 2014;6(2):230–50. https://doi.org/10.1111/aphw.12026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Phua JJ. The reference group perspective for smoking cessation: an examination of the influence of social norms and social identification with reference groups on smoking cessation self-efficacy. Psychol Addict Behav. 2013;27(1):102–12. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029130.

  64. Persky S, Bouhlal S, Goldring MR, McBride CM. Beliefs about genetic influences on eating behaviors: characteristics and associations with weight management confidence. Eat Behav. 2017;26:93–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2017.02.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Straughan PT, Seow A. Fatalism reconceptualized: a concept to predict health screening behavior. J Gend Cult Heal. 1998;3(2):85–100. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023278230797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Higgs S, Ruddock H. Social influences on eating. In H. Meiselman (Eds.), Handbook of eating and drinking: interdisciplinary perspectives. 2020;277–291. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-14504-0_27.

  67. Cruwys T, Platow MJ, Angullia SA, Chang JM, Diler SE, Kirchner JL, Lentfer CE, Lim YJ, Quarisa A, Tor VWL, Wadley AL. Modeling of food intake is moderated by salient psychological group membership. Appetite. 2012;58(2):754–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Hendrinkse JJ, Cachia RL, Kothe EJ, McPhie S, Skouteris H, Hayden MJ. Attentional biases for food cues in overweight and individuals with obesity: a systematic review of the literature. Obes Rev. 2015;16(5):424–32. https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Puhl RM, Heuer CA. Obesity stigma: important considerations for public health. Am J Public Health. 2010;100(6):1019–28. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2009.159491.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Incollingo Rodriguez AC, Heldreth CM, Tomiyama AJ. Putting on weight stigma: a randomized study of the effects of wearing a fat suit on eating, well-being, and cortisol. Obesity. 2016;24(9):1892–8. https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.21575.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  71. Lee KM, Hunger JM, Tomiyama AJ. Weight stigma and health behaviors: evidence from the Eating in America Study. Int J Obes. 2021;45(7):1499–509. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41366-021-00814-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Major B, Hunger JM, Bunyan DP, Miller CT. The ironic effects of weight stigma. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2014;51:74–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.11.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Lewis Jr NA, Kougias DG, Takahashi KJ, Earl A. The behavior of same-race others and its effects on Black patients’ attention to publicly presented HIV-prevention information. Health Commun. 2021;36(10):1–8. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2020.1749369.

Download references

Funding

This project was funded by the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases of the National Institutes of Health under award number R0IDK083347 (to T.E.J.). Additional support was provided by the Michigan Center for Diabetes Translational Research under award number P30DK092926 (to T.E.J.) from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases. This publication was made possible with support from the Indiana Clinical and Translational Sciences Institute, which is funded in part by Award Number KL2TR002530 from the National Institutes of Health, National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences, Clinical and Translational Sciences Award. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Veronica Derricks.

Ethics declarations

Ethics Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary file1 (DOCX 109 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Derricks, V., Earl, A., Carmichael, A.G. et al. Psychological Pathways Through Which Social Norms and Social Identity Influence Eating Behavior: Testing a Conceptual Model. Int.J. Behav. Med. 30, 7–18 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-022-10064-y

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-022-10064-y

Keywords

Navigation