Introduction

Evidence for early humans utilizing fire as a tool—for processing food and raw materials, modifying their (micro)environment, and for social functions (for a comprehensive list of possible uses for fire and relevant sources, see Rolland 2000)—is largely sporadic until second half of the Middle Pleistocene (roughly 400–300 kya), with combustion traces beginning to appear with increased frequency right around the time the Neandertal morphotype begins to enter the scene (Roebroeks and Villa 2011a; Shimelmitz et al. 2014; Meyer et al. 2016; Zanolli et al. 2018; MacDonald et al. 2021). This upward trend carries on through to the Late Pleistocene, with fire use by Neandertals becoming a (semi-)regular practice up until their disappearance approximately 40 kya (Higham et al. 2014; Djakovic et al. 2022). It is thus not all that controversial to say that Neandertals were competent users of fire. There is debate, however, surrounding the degree and complexity of Neandertal fire use (see Sorensen 2019 and Allué et al. 2022 for an overview).

Beyond the relatively commonplace presence of simple combustion features or fire-affected artifacts at Middle Palaeolithic sites, there are a number of archaeological findings that attest to Neandertal pyrotechnical prowess. These includes the occasional construction of more complex pit hearths or hearths structured with stones (e.g., Goldberg and Bar-Yosef 2002; Meignen et al. 2007; Shahack-Gross et al. 2014; Monnier et al. 2016; Leierer et al. 2020), purposeful burning of the landscape to create conditions favourable for preferred food resources (Pop et al. 2016; Roebroeks et al. 2021), charring the surfaces of wooden tools to make them easier to shape with stone tools (Aranguren et al. 2018), and boiling (as opposed to simply roasting) their food (Henry et al. 2010), the latter possibly suggesting Neandertal use of perishable containers (Speth 2015) and suspension systems for cooking over a fire (Castro-Curel and Carbonell 1995). Another strong indicator of sophisticated use of fire by Neandertals is the series of artifacts suggesting they were producing birch bark tar, an adhesive used as a hafting material for stone tools and generally considered the first “synthetic” material produced by humans (Koller et al. 2001; Mazza et al. 2006; Wragg Sykes 2015; Niekus et al. 2019). The exact methods Neandertals may have use to synthesize birch bark tar are presently unknown, though researchers continue to experiment with different “low-tech” methods that would have been available to Neandertals, with varying levels of complexity and rates of success (Groom et al. 2013; Kozowyk et al. 2017; Schenck and Groom 2018; Schmidt et al. 2019; Koch and Schmidt 2022). Fire may have even served a ritual purpose among some Neandertals, as suggested by the discovery at Bruniquel Cave (France) of two enigmatic circles of broken and stacked stalagmites (~ 400) constructed on the floor of a chamber more than 300 m from the cave entrance, and upon which 20 fires were built (Jaubert et al. 2016).

Perhaps the most fundamental skill necessary for fire mastery among any groups of humans, however, is the ability to produce fire. Whether or not Neandertals possessed this skill remains a contentious issue, largely due to the heavy reliance on proxy evidence for this practice (i.e., the presence/absence or relative abundance of fire traces at Middle Palaeolithic archaeological sites), with some authors believing the evidence for fire use is sufficiently abundant to suggest Neandertals could make fire (Roebroeks and Villa 2011b; Shimelmitz et al. 2014; Sorensen 2017; Sorensen and Scherjon 2018), while others remain sceptical (Sandgathe et al. 2011a, b; Dibble et al. 2017, 2018; Sandgathe 2017; Abdolahzadeh et al. 2022). However, microwear analysis of lithic artifacts appears to provide a means by which to overcome some of these interpretive differences by identifying direct evidence of fire production in the archaeological record, i.e., the fire-making tools themselves—specifically, flint ‘strike-a-lights’ bearing use traces consistent with having been struck against the mineral pyrite to produce sparks (Collin et al. 1991; Stapert and Johansen 1999; Weiner and Floss 2004; Sorensen et al. 2014). A number of such tools have been identified from Middle Palaeolithic contexts in France, including a few dozen Mousterian of Acheulean Tradition (MTA) flint bifaces dating to at least 50 kya (Sorensen et al. 2018), and another possible earlier example (a Levallois point) dating to about 75–85 ka BP (Rots 2011; Sorensen and Rots 2014). While the origins, distribution and continuity of this technology remain nebulous, these findings lend support to the idea that at least some Neandertals were able to produce fire as needed.

MnO2: a pyrotechnological pigment?

Mineral pigments—specifically, the manganese dioxide (MnO2)-rich mineral pyrolusite—may provide an additional twist to the possibility and complexity of Neandertal fire production. These dark-gray to black-colored MnO2 blocks are sometimes found in abundance at various Middle Palaeolithic archaeological sites (Demars 1992; Soressi et al. 2008; Dayet et al. 2014, 2019; Pitarch Martí and d’Errico 2018). Some possess grinding facets or scoring traces that suggest these blocks were being powdered (Fig. 1), possibly for use as body paint (Soressi et al. 2008; Bodu et al. 2014; Pitarch Martí et al. 2019), while rounded edges and facets on softer fragments have been interpreted as their having been used as crayons on skin or hides (Bordes 1972; Soressi and d’Errico 2007). It is not universally accepted that the collection and processing of MnO2 is related to symbolic expression by Neandertals (Chase and Dibble 1987; Velo and Kehoe 1990), as this mineral likely has other practical uses, as well (see Supplementary Information (SI) 1 for a longer discussion of Neandertal pigment use and symbolic thinking).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Blocks of manganese dioxide (MnO2) from Pech de l’Azé I (Dordogne, France) with modified surfaces exhibiting scoring (a, b) and grinding (c) use-wear traces resulting from powder production

In an effort to test for possible wider applications of powdered mineral materials beyond use as pigments, a series of experiments were performed by Heyes and colleagues (2016) to explore their pyrotechnic properties. Various metal oxide powders (Mn, Fe, Zn, Ti, Al) mixed with beech wood turnings were combusted and monitored via infrared camera and thermogravimetric analysis (TGA). It was found that the addition of MnO2 powder (both commercially produced, and powder produced by grinding blocks recovered during archaeological excavation) uniquely reduced the temperature required for the combustion of the beech wood by ca. 80–180 °C (depending on the ratio of MnO2 powder to wood turnings) and provided a sevenfold increase in the rate of char combustion.

These findings help to elucidate the oxidizing and catalytic properties of MnO2 that promote combustion, thereby highlighting its potential utility for prehistoric fire lighting. However, Heyes and colleagues do not really delve into the real-world practicalities of this application. In some of the experiments performed by the authors, cotton wool or elm seed tinder were used to ignite wood turnings mixed with MnO2 powder, but these tinders were not themselves mixed with MnO2. Thus, in this setup, the wood turnings function more as a fine kindling than as a tinder. So while mixing MnO2 with fine kindling might also be useful, it could be argued that there is more benefit to be gained from instead mixing the MnO2 powder with the tinder. It is here that the most crucial moment in the fire-making process takes place: the initial capture and propagation of a spark into an ember.

Powdered charcoal and gunpowder meal (dust) have been similarly applied to tinder in the past by indigenous peoples (Ray 1885; Segers 1891; Murdoch 1892). This then begs the question: Does adding MnO2 powder to a tinder improve its ability to capture sparks produced using materials and methods available to prehistoric humans (i.e., flint and pyrite)? And if so, is the superior performance of treated over untreated tinder great enough to warrant targeted selection of MnO2 for this purpose by Neandertals?

In a subsequent pilot study, Sorensen (2020) conducted a limited number of actualistic fire-making experiments in a first attempt to test these questions. The results showed shredded tinder fungus (Fomes fomentarius) mixed with MnO2 powder performed noticeably better than untreated tinder, exhibiting increases in both the speed and the frequency at which ignitions were achieved. Given the encouraging results and limited scope of the initial study, it was determined that further exploration was necessary.

Materials and methods

The study presented here attempts to replicate the findings of Sorensen (2020) using an expanded experimental dataset of different tinder types of variable quality to further test the efficacy of MnO2 as a fire-starting aid. The experimental protocol described below closely follows that of the original pilot study.

Tinder types tested

Eight different types of tinder common to various habitats in temperate Northwest Europe were used to test the hypothesis posed by Heyes et al. (2016) that MnO2 powder was potentially used by Neandertals as a tinder additive to improve its fire production capacities. These included three species of tree-dwelling fungi, two terrestrial plant species, one (semi-)aquatic plant species, one tree species, and one species of moss. The fungus species included Fomes fomentarius (SI Fig. 1), Ganoderma adspersum (SI Fig. 2), and Daldinia concentrica (SI Fig. 3). The terrestrial plant species included Cirsium arvense (SI Fig. 4) and Tussilago farfara (SI Fig. 5). The (semi-)aquatic plant, tree and moss species tested were Typha latifolia (SI Fig. 6), Populus nigra (SI Fig. 7) and Brachythecium sp. (SI Fig. 8), respectively. All of the tinders tested in this study were collected in the Netherlands and will hereafter be referred to by their genus names. This selection of tinders is by no means exhaustive, but it does provide a cross-section of easily obtainable tinder materials with different physical attributes and qualities. Moreover, these specific tinders (or those with comparable properties) would likely have been available in Northwest Europe in the past under both warmer interglacial and colder glacial conditions. For more detailed physical and ecological descriptions of the tinders used here (and of tinder, in general), please refer to SI 1.

Tinder preparation

The majority of the tinders selected for this study required little to no processing and could be used as is after being allowed to air-dry at room temperature—in this case, for a number of weeks, but in practice, many of these tinders could have been used shortly after harvesting (SI Figs. 1–8). Most of the residual stems and seed capsules incorporated into the collected mass of Populus seed tufts were removed and discarded. Only the fungi species required minor processing prior to use. While intact fragments of Daldinia are typically used by modern primitive technologists/bushcrafters to make fire, for the sake of consistency and measurability between experiments, the interior portion of the fruiting body was ground into a homogenous powder in a mortar and pestle. For both the Fomes and Ganoderma, the fruiting bodies were bisected with a knife while still fresh to exposed in the inner, velvety trama layers and then allowed to dry (SI Fig. 1). These were then scraped with a flint flake to produce the fluffy masses used as tinder.

The dried and processed tinders were weighed and placed into six Minigrip® bags per tinder type, each containing 0.3 g of material. Three of the six bags (per tinder type) were left as is to serve as the control group. For the experimental group, 0.1 g of commercially manufactured MnO2 powder (specifically, pyrolusite; Sigma-Aldrich, product reference number 310,700) was added to each of the remaining three bags and mixed thoroughly.

Fire-making experiments

Fire-making experiments were conducted using an actualistic approach, using only materials that would have been available to Neandertals. However, various measures were taken to limit variability between experiments. All fire-making experiments were performed by the author indoors at the Leiden University Faculty of Archaeology Laboratory for Artefact Studies to ensure a level of consistency in the environmental conditions between experiments, thus avoiding the variability introduced outdoors by shifts in temperature, relative humidity and wind speeds that can have significant effects on the fire production process and potentially skew the results. Moreover, the ability to maintain relatively low lighting conditions allowed for easier observation of the sparks produced.

For each experiment, one bag of prepared tinder was emptied into a small ceramic bowl (diameter: 4.5 cm; depth: 2.2 cm) to control for the surface area exposed to the sparks (Fig. 2). The tinder was tamped down into the bowl to form a continuous and more or less uniformly thick bed (3–5 mm) to increase connectivity between the tinder fibers and ensure a level consistency between experiments.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Materials and gesture used for the fire-making experiments. The flint crested blade was pressed firmly against the broken surface of the pyrite nodule and then pulled upwards in a scraping motion to produce sparks directed into the ceramic bowl containing the tinder

The same fire-making set was used throughout the study and consisted of a crested blade strike-a-light crafted from Grand-Pressigny flint (source in Central France) and a halved pyrite nodule (collected from a beach near Cap Gris-Nez, Northwest France). To provide greater manual control, a strong friction gesture was employed in lieu of a percussive striking gesture, whereby the tip of the strike-a-light was forcefully rubbed/scraped in an upwards motion against the broken surface of the pyrite module fragment to produce sparks that were directed into the bowl of tinder (Fig. 2). It should be noted that both elements of the fire-making set had been used previously by the author to make fire. This was done to ensure that the rounded and flattened morphologies of the respective strike-a-light and pyrite contact surfaces would remain largely similar between the first and final experiments. The experiments were conducted over the course of a couple of weeks to ensure fatigue did not create disparities between experiments performed earlier and later in the fire-making sessions.

Experimental program

Six experiments were performed for each tinder type, three treated with MnO2 and three controls, yielding a total of 48 experiments performed for this study. Since not all scrapes of the strike-a-light against the pyrite (hereafter, “strokes”) will necessarily produce sparks, an effort was made to construct an experimental design that would ensure each tinder experiment was exposed to a comparable number sparks. Therefore, each experiment was comprised of 100 spark-producing strokes (i.e., where sparks were observed making contact with the tinder) subdivided into 20 sets of five strokes. The total number of strokes required to achieve this target per set was also recorded but is not particularly important for the analyses (e.g., it may require eight strokes in total to produce sparks five times). The total number of times a spark was captured by the tinder (designated here as “ignitions”) was recorded for each set of five spark-producing strokes. To gauge how the ignition frequency might change over the course of an experiment, the sets were grouped into four groups of five sets and the probability of achieving at least one ignition within each group calculated (P(Ignition)). Table 1 provides a visual representation of the experimental setup and how data was recorded. These data were analyzed and plotted using Microsoft Excel. Figures were produced using Microsoft Excel and Adobe Illustrator.

Table 1 Experimental design and data format overview

Results

The addition of MnO2 powder to tinder greatly improves its ability to capture sparks, both in terms of reliability (total ignitions over the course of the experiments) and speed (point of first ignition). Looking at the average results across all tested tinder types for both these parameters, tinder mixed with MnO2 powder proved to be around twice as effective as untreated tinder. A full accounting of the experimental data for the individual tinders tested, along with a detailed comparison of their performance, can be found in SI 1 and SI Table 1. It should be noted that the results obtained in this or similar studies are likely contingent upon the proficiency and experience of the individual conducting the fire-making experiments, as well as the nature and quality of the fire-making tools utilized. Nevertheless, it is reasonable to posit that the relative difference in efficacy between a particular tinder with and without added MnO2 would exhibit consistency across different experimenters.

In terms of total ignitions achieved, the control tinder group produced an average of 8.92 ignitions per 100 spark-producing strokes (range: 1–27.67 ignitions for Brachythecium and Fomes, respectively), while the experimental group produced an average of 17.34 (range: 2.67–46.67 ignitions for Brachythecium and Fomes, respectively), a 1.94x increase in ignitions overall (Fig. 3a), with different tinder types showing increases ranging from 1.57x (Typha) and 2.67x (Brachythecium) (SI Fig. 10). In a normal fire-making scenario, however, it is unlikely that anyone would proceed beyond the first ignition, assuming the attempt at producing flames was ultimately successful. Therefore, it is important to discuss the influence of MnO2 on achieving this crucial first ignition. In this case, the first ignition for the control group was attained after 11.29 sets, or approximately 55 sparks (range: 7.33–17.67 sets for Fomes and Brachythecium, respectively), while the experimental group achieved ignition after 5.33 sets, or approximately 26.67 sparks (range: 1–11.33 sets for Fomes/Ganoderma and Brachythecium, respectively), meaning sparks can be captured, on average, 2.12x faster using MnO2 (Fig. 3b). For the individual tinders tested, the increase in the rate of ignition stemming from the addition of MnO2 varies between 1.32x (Brachythecium) and 9.33x (Ganoderma) (SI Fig. 11).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Comparison of ignition effectiveness between tinder treated with manganese dioxide (MnO2) and untreated tinder. The bar graphs present combined average values for all tinder types tested in terms of (a) the total number of successful ignitions, and (b) the point at which ignition was first achieved (1 Set = 5 spark-producing strokes)

An additional finding of these experiments was that the tinders appeared to become more effective at capturing sparks the longer they were used. This progressive increase in ignition frequency is clearly indicated by the increasing slope of trendlines (line graphs) and by increases in ignition probability (bar graphs) over the course of the experiments, for both the experimental groups treated with MnO2 and the untreated control groups (average of all tinders, Fig. 4; averages of individual tinders, SI Fig. 9). A limited series of supplemental experiments were performed in an effort to explain this phenomenon.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Combined average results of fire-making experiments testing the effects of adding manganese dioxide powder (MnO2) to tinder for all tinder types tested. (a) The line graph plots the average number of successful ignitions per 5 spark-producing strokes (20 sets, 100 strokes per experiment), with trendlines indicating change in ignition frequency over time. (b) The bar graph provides an alternative visualization of these changes in ignition frequency, expressed as the probability of achieving at least 1 ignition within each grouping of 5 sets

Pyrite powder experiments

The increased rate of ignitions over the course of the experiments was presumed to be related to the gradual accumulation of pyrite dust onto the surface of the tinder, based both on personal experience, but also following observations made by Collin and colleagues (1991) and Roussel (2005). However, it could also be due to an increased presence of charred areas in the tinder from previous ignitions, these likely being more accepting of sparks than uncharred material. In an effort to test both of these hypotheses simultaneously, two additional sets of experiments were performed using Fomes and Typha, where pyrite (FeS2) powder (produced by scraping the same pyrite nodule used to produce sparks) was mixed with prepared tinder using the same protocol as for the MnO2 experiments (0.1 g of powder to 0.3 g of tinder).

The experiments appear to show pyrite powder added to tinder indeed improves the fire-making process, often performing even slightly better than tinder treated with MnO2 (Fig. 5; SI Table 1). There was a clear increase in average total ignitions over the controls for both the Fomes (n = 48.67, a 1.76x increase) and Typha (n = 19.33, a 2.76x increase) experiments (Fig. 5a). The point of first ignition was also achieved more quickly by pyrite-treated tinder compared to untreated controls (Fig. 5b), with Fomes only requiring around 1.67 sets (~ 8.33 sparks) and Typha 5.67 (~ 28.35 sparks), while corresponding controls required 7.33 sets (~ 36.67 sparks) and 11 sets (~ 55 sparks). This translates to a 7.33x and 1.94x reduction in the number of sparks needed to start a fire, respectively.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Comparison of ignition effectiveness between tinder treated with powdered pyrite (FeS2), manganese dioxide (MnO2), and untreated tinder. The bar graphs present the average values for two tinder types tested (Fomes fomentarius and Typha latifolia) in terms of (a) the total number of successful ignitions, and (b) the point at which ignition was first achieved (1 Set = 5 spark-producing strokes)

The trend lines and ignition probabilities presented in Fig. 6 demonstrate the rates of ignition again improved over the course of the pyrite experiments. This may provide some support to the idea that the increase in charred surface area in a tinder at least partially contributes to the improved chances for spark capture over time. However, the fact that treated tinder achieved first ignition more quickly prior the accumulation of any charred material strongly suggests the addition of pyrite powder is the main factor influencing the improvement in the tinder performance, both initially and progressively during the fire-making process.

Fig. 6
figure 6

Results of supplemental fire-making experiments testing the effects of adding pyrite powder (FeS2) to tinder (Fomes fomentarius and Typha latifolia), compared to their respective experimental manganese dioxide (MnO2) and control groups. The line graphs plot the average number of successful ignitions per 5 spark-producing strokes (20 sets, 100 strokes per experiment), with trendlines indicating change in ignition frequency over time. The bar graphs provide an alternative visualization of these changes in ignition frequency, expressed as the probability of achieving at least 1 ignition within each grouping of 5 sets

Discussion

In agreement with those produced during an earlier pilot study (Sorensen 2020), the findings of this study confirm the model laid out by Heyes and colleagues (2016) by demonstrating that the addition of MnO2 to a tinder material greatly improves its combustion potential by making it much more effective at quickly and consistently capturing sparks and allowing them to propagate, regardless of tinder type (Figs. 3 and 4; SI Fig. 9). The variability in effectiveness observed between tinder types likely reflects the relative thickness of the individual fibers comprising each tinder type (see SI 1). Coarser tinders require greater and/or longer heat inputs to overcome their respective ignition thresholds, while finer tinder fibers also allow for a greater degree of compactibility, which in turn provides increased connectivity between fibers (i.e., reduced porosity), facilitating ember propagation. In addition to the chemical benefits offered by MnO2, as documented by Heyes and colleagues—namely, a ca. 80–180 °C reduction in the temperature required for combustion and a sevenfold increase in the rate of char combustion—it is suggested here that the powder itself can act as a physical bridge between individual tinder fibers by infilling void space, thereby increasing the connectivity between fibers and allowing an ember to propagate more effectively. Interestingly, this catalytic effect was similarly leveraged in the 19th century to improve the lighting efficiency of cotton lighter wicks, which were impregnated with MnO2 to provide a safer alternative to wicks traditionally impregnated with highly toxic lead chromate (Monier 1876; Moser-Gautrand 1984).

Another finding of the current study was that all of the tinder experiments—with and without added MnO2—showed increased spark capturing capacities as the experiment progressed (Fig. 4; SI Fig. 9). Additional experiments using a subsample of tinders mixed with pyrite powder appear to confirm the hypothesis that this phenomenon is primarily related to the gradual and incidental production of pyrite dust during the fire-making process, which appears to improve the effectiveness of a tinder through physical and possibly chemical means similar to those described above for MnO2 (Figs. 5 and 6). Replicating the experiments and analyses performed by Heyes and colleagues (2016) using pyrite powder would likely shed further light on the chemical mechanisms underlying this process. Localized ignition of the surficial accumulation of pyrite dust appears to extend the life of a spark to the point that it can overcome the initial energy required to ignite the underlying tinder material, as has been surmised in previous fire-making studies (Collin et al. 1991; Roussel 2005) and noted during my own fire-making experience. But to my knowledge, this phenomenon has not been empirically demonstrated until now. Thus, the possible implications of these findings with regard to Neandertal use of MnO2 for fire making are discussed in more detail below.

The results from this study appear to run counter to the recent findings of Langley and Needham (2021), who found that the addition of MnO2 to tinders comprised of birch bark (strips and powdered) or powdered charcoal failed to improve their respective abilities to capture sparks produced by a modern ferrocerium fire-starting rod, which produces sparks with temperatures around 3,000\(\:^\circ\:\)C when scraped with a steel edge—much hotter than those produced by pyrite, estimated to be between ca. 680–780 °C (Weiner 2012) and 760–870 °C (Bois 2004). However, their findings are limited by their lack of success in getting the birch bark and charcoal tinders to ignite at all (both with and without added MnO2). This may be due to poor experimental design and execution. Ethnographic data alludes to the addition of powdered charcoal to tinders like moss, willow catkins or downy bird feathers to improve their efficacy (Ray 1885; Segers 1891; Murdoch 1892), but not charcoal used on its own. Anecdotally, a cursory survey of fire-making videos on YouTube did not yield any instances where charcoal powder was used as a tinder, only occasional examples where whole fragments of charred punk wood were used to capture sparks from a fire-steel or ferrocerium rod. Furthermore, Langley and Needham state they used “ground” or “powdered” birch bark in some of their experiments, but it is unclear how exactly this was produced. Bushcrafters often use with great success the finely shaved/shredded outer, more papery layers of birch bark to start fires with ferrocerium rods, usually achieving ignition within a few strokes. That the authors only managed one ignition out of 30 strokes might speak to either improper preparation of the birch bark tinder (i.e., too coarse), or a lack of experience with the method; a more detailed description of the fire-making gesture used might have shed more light on this issue. Additionally, the relative coarseness of the birch bark and charcoal tinders (at least compared to those tested in this study) may largely negate the physical benefits conferred by MnO2 powder. So in the end, this study does little to validate or refute the model put forward by Heyes and colleagues (2016).

The above findings are applicable to any region or period where MnO2 was being collected by prehistoric peoples (e.g., during the Upper Palaeolithic in Southwest France, see Pitarch-Martí and d’Errico, 2018). However, the archaeological focus of this and previous studies on the Middle Palaeolithic is primarily due to the higher prevalence of MnO2 collection by Neandertals and the ongoing debate surrounding their use of fire (see above). Indeed, the idea that MnO2 may have been collected by Neandertals to aid in fire starting has been challenged by Dibble and colleagues (2018). In their study, the authors compare the relationship between the relative abundance of fire proxy evidence (i.e., heated lithic artifacts) and MnO2 fragments present in different archaeological layers at the Middle Palaeolithic site of Pech de l’Azé IV (Dordogne, France). Figure 8 from this study shows an inverse relationship between heated lithics and the number of MnO2 fragments recovered per layer, with the prevalence of MnO2 rising in layers deposited under colder climatic conditions as the percentages of heated lithics steadily decline. These authors interpret this negative correlation as evidence that the presence of MnO2 is unrelated to fire making, as one would expect a greater prevalence of fire evidence in layers where fires were being produced.

However, this evidence remains open to interpretation and may not be as straightforward as Dibble and colleagues suggest. For starters, it should be mentioned that the majority of the Middle Palaeolithic archaeological layers from sites in Southwest France known to contain MnO2 fragments also exhibit at least some evidence of fire having been used on site (see SI 1 Table 1 in Heyes et al. 2016, and references therein). The trend observed at Pech de l’Azé IV, and similar increases in the prevalence of MnO2 in Quina and Ferrassie Mousterian layers deposited during Marine Isotope Stage (MIS) 4 at Combe Grenal and Caminade (Demars 1992; Dayet et al. 2019), suggest an increased reliance on this material during this colder climatic period that could be interpreted as evidence in support of fire making at these sites using MnO2. Increased pressure to reliably and quickly produce fire during the MIS 4 cold stage could account for the increased presence of MnO2 in these layers. Moreover, limited access to wood fuel may have made fire-making activities more frequent (i.e., due to the reduced ability to retain one’s flame simply through continuously feeding a fire), while fire signals would have nevertheless become weaker through the use of shorter term, task specific fires for the sake of fuel economization (Sorensen 2017). Thus, the apparent relationship between cold climate and MnO2, while not absolute, could provide tacit support for the hypothesis that Neandertals used MnO2 to aid in fire production.

The archaeological focus of the study by Heyes and colleagues (2016) was Layer 4 at Pech de l’Azé I (Dordogne, France), which was deposited under more temperate climatic conditions ca. 50 kya and contains an artifact assemblage attributed to the Mousterian of Acheulean Tradition, or MTA (Soressi 2002; Soressi et al. 2008). This layer not only produced a large number of MnO2 blocks, but also strong fire evidence, including multiple combustion features, an abundance of heated bone fragments and charcoal (Soressi et al. 2008), but interestingly, only a small number of heated lithics (M. Soressi, personal communication), and at least one biface bearing percussive and frictive microwear traces consistent with use as a strike-a-light (Sorensen et al. 2018). This association between the abundance of MnO2 fragments and strong signals of fire use, which runs counter to the apparent inverse relationship between these lines of evidence presented by Dibble and colleagues (2018) for Pech de l’Azé IV, may or may not be related. The high abundance of MnO2—much more than one would need for fire making alone—recovered from this and other MTA layers in Southwest France (e.g., at Le Moustier, Pech de l’Azé IV; Soressi et al. 2008; Pitarch Martí et al. 2019), could signal a retention of this practice by later Neandertal groups, or that these sites may simply have been close to ancient sources of MnO2, allowing for easy aggregation prior to being carried outwards into the landscape.

An alternative and more probable explanation likely lies in the versatility of MnO2, with its ability to aid in fire making being only one of a number of potential uses for this highly valued resource. In addition to the various symbolic and utilitarian functions outlined in the introduction and SI 1, there are two other possible practical applications for MnO2 powder that may have benefitted Neandertals during glacial periods (or under winter conditions, more generally). The first is as an eye-blacking agent, similar to those used by professional athletes today. When smeared under or around the eyes (ideally mixed with animal fat/grease to improve adhesion and spreadability), eye black would have helped to mitigate (snow) glare and improve contrast sensitivity (de Broff and Pahk 2003; Powers 2005), thereby enhancing one’s ability to track moving objects (e.g., prey) on sunny winter days. Such a glare-reduction strategy (but instead, using charcoal) has been recommended for arctic or desert survival situations when suitable eye protection is not available (Wiseman 2009). In a similar vein, smearing the same MnO2 and grease mixture over the entirety of the face could have served the dual purpose of protecting against both sunburn (Rifkin et al. 2015a; b) and chapped skin caused by moisture loss in cold, windy conditions, in the very least, while perhaps even providing a thin layer of insulation against the elements when additionally applied to other unclothed parts of the body (for ethnographic analogues, see Moore 1842; Densmore 1929; Gusinde 1931; Gusinde and Schütze 1937; Chapman 1982).

There are a number of other factors to consider when evaluating the likelihood that MnO2 powder was at least an occasional addition to the Neandertal fire-making kit. Assuming Neandertals did in fact use MnO2 for this purpose, reduced access to higher quality tinder during cold stages may have been a driving factor in the collection of MnO2 during these periods, allowing Neandertals to improve the reliability of less suitable tinders. However, since MnO2 is not a requisite element of the fire-making toolkit—indeed, all tinders tested in this study, even those of poor quality, were able to capture sparks eventually without the addition of MnO2 powder (see SI Fig. 9)—its use for this task may only have been employed when readily available in the landscape, or already on hand from collection for other uses. Moreover, the experimental findings from this study have shown that adding pyrite powder to tinder (either purposefully or incidentally through use) can also expedite the fire-making process, perhaps providing an argument against the need to collect MnO2 for this purpose. Nevertheless, it should be pointed out that pyrite, which is rated 6–6.5/10 on the Moh’s scale of mineral hardness (Mindat 2022a), is much harder and therefore more difficult to grind into powder than the softer, fine-grained blocks of MnO2 usually collected by Neandertals, with pyrolusite in its massive (as opposed to crystalline) form rating ~ 2/10 on the Moh’s scale (Mindat 2022b). MnO2-bearing deposits, of which there are numerous known occurrences in Southwest France, can sometimes yield large quantities of material see Pitarch Martí et al. 2019). Given the necessity of pyrite in the fire-making process and its relative sparseness in some landscapes, grinding MnO2 over tinder may have allowed Neandertals to limit attrition to their pyrite by reducing the number of strikes needed to capture a spark, thereby extending the use-life of the pyrite.

Conclusion

To sum up, previous studies have provided evidence suggesting the following:

  1. 1)

    Neandertals were regular and proficient users of fire;

  2. 2)

    they were occasional and, at times, avid collectors of MnO2-rich rocks;

  3. 3)

    some of these MnO2-rich rocks exhibit use wear traces indicating they had been ground or scraped to produce powder;

  4. 4)

    the addition of MnO2 powder to a tinder material accelerates its combustion under controlled laboratory conditions;

  5. 5)

    Neandertals were capable of producing fire using flint and pyrite.

The aim of this article has been to explore the next logical question in this sequence: How effective is MnO2-treated tinder at capturing sparks during actualistic percussive fire-making experiments? The results of these experiments definitively show MnO2 powder improves the spark capturing efficiency of treated over untreated tinder, regardless of the type and relative quality of the tinder, thereby reducing the time and energy required to produce fire. It was also observed that both treated and untreated tinders became more receptive of sparks as the experiment progressed, a phenomenon presumed to be related to pyrite dust being gradually incorporated into the tinder. This was confirmed through additional experiments that showed pre-mixing pyrite powder into tinder also greatly facilitated spark capture and propagation. While this finding could be used to argue against fire making being the primary motivation for collecting MnO2, its relative softness compared to pyrite greatly facilitates powdering, making it an attractive (and effective) alternative for this purpose. Moreover, the addition of powdered materials to tinder to facilitate combustion (e.g., charcoal, gunpowder meal) is a known strategy for increasing fire-making success among various indigenous groups, lending tacit support to the idea that MnO2 may have been used similarly in the past. At the moment, however, it remains difficult to say definitively that Neandertals used MnO2 powder as a tinder-enhancing agent based on currently available archaeological data. Indeed, it is difficult to surmise what specific set of evidences or conditions would be required to do so. But given the pyrotechnical prowess of Neandertals, their long history of collecting and powdering MnO2-rich rocks, and the intersection of these two aspects of Neandertal daily life at numerous Middle Palaeolithic sites, it seems entirely reasonable to presume that they cultivated through experimentation and observation an intimate knowledge of the various applications of MnO2, including its use as a tinder additive that improved their chances for quickly and easily producing fire.