Skip to main content
Log in

Interaction with a Moving Object Affects One’s Perception of Its Animacy

  • Published:
International Journal of Social Robotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Sometimes we regard just an artifact as a lifelike one and other times not; it is considered to depend on how we deal and interact with the artifact. We experimentally examined whether differences in the manner of interacting with a moving robot (operating it or only observing its movements) influenced one’s perception of the robot’s animacy and, if so, whether the strength of this influence depended on the apparent goal-directedness of the robot’s movements. We found that people only observing the robot perceived it most animated when its movements seemed most goal-directed but that people controlling the robot perceived it more animated when 1/f noise made its movements seem less goal-directed. Our perception of a moving object’s animacy thus depends on whether we interact with the object or just observe it while someone else interacts with it. This result suggests that robotics researchers should design how a robot interacts with its users, in order to elicit higher degree of animacy perception for the robot.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  1. Michotte A (1963) The perception of causality. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  2. Premack D (1990) The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition 36:1–16

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Baron-Cohen S (1995) Mindblindness. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  4. Heider F, Simmel M (1944) An experimental study of apparent behavior. Am J Psychol 57:243–249

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Tremoulet PD, Feldman J (2000) The influence of spatial context and the role of intentionality in the interpretation of animacy from motion. Perception 68:1047–1058

    Google Scholar 

  6. Tremoulet PD, Feldman J (2006) The influence of spatial context and the role of intentionality in the interpretation of animacy from motion. Percept Psychophys 68:1047–1058

    Google Scholar 

  7. Dittrich W, Les S (1994) Visual perception of intentional motions. Perception 23:253–268

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Poulin-Doubois D, Lepage A, Ferland D (1996) Infants concept of animacy. Cogn Dev 11:19–36

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Legerstee M (2000) Precursors to the development of intention at 6 months: understanding people and their actions. Dev Psychol 36:627–634

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Arita A, Hiraki K, Kanda K, Ishiguro H (2005) Can we talk to robots? Ten-month-old infants expected interactive humanoid robots to be talked to by persons. Cognition 95:B49–B57

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Bartneck C, Kulic D, Croft E, Zoghbi S (2009) Measurement instruments for the anthropomorphism, animacy, likeability, perceived intelligence, and perceived safety of robots. Int J Soc Robot 1:71–81

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Lee KM, Park NH, Song H (2005) Can a robot be perceived as a developing creature? Hum Commun Res 31:538–563

    Google Scholar 

  13. Bartneck C, Kanda T, Mubbin O, Mahmud A (2009) Does the design of a robot influence its animacy and perceived intelligence? Int J Soc Robot 1:195–204

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Okita SY, Schwartz DL (2006) Young children’s understanding of animacy and entertainment robots. Int J Hum Robot 3:393–412

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Kozima H, Michalowski MP, Nakagawa C (2009) Keepon: a playful robot for research, therapy, and entertainment. Int J Soc Robot 1:3–18

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Shaw-Garlock G (2009) Looking forward to sociable robots. Int J Soc Robot 1:249–260

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Wagenmakers EJ, Farrell S, Ratcliff R (2005) Human cognition and a pile of sand: a discussion on serial correlations and self-organized criticality. J Exp Psychol Gen 134:108–116

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Gilden DL (2001) Cognitive emissions of 1/f noise. Psychol Rev 108:33–56

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (2006) e-puck educational robot. www.e-puck.org

  20. Opfer JE (2002) Identifying living and sentient kinds from dynamic information: the case of goal-directed versus aimless autonomous movement in conceptual change. Cognition 86:97–122

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Richards DD, Siegler RS (1986) Children’s understandings of the attributes of life. J Exp Child Psychol 42:1–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Braitenberg V (1984) Vehicles: experiments in synthetic psychology. MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  23. Wheatley T, Milleville SG, Martin A (2007) Understanding animate agent: distinct roles for the social network and mirror system. Psychol Sci 18:469–474

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Keysers C, Perrett DI (2004) Demystifying social cognition: a Hebbian perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 8:501–507

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Schultz J, Friston KJ, O’Doherty J, Wolpert DM, Frith CD (2005) Activation in posterior superior temporal sulcus parallels parameter inducing the percept of animacy. Neuron 45:625–635

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Schultz J, Imamizu H, Kawato M, Frith CD (2004) Activation of the human superior temporal gyrus during observation of goal attribution by intentional objects. J Cogn Neurosci 16:1695–1705

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Blakemore SJ, Boyer P, Pachot-Clouard M, Meltzoff A, Segebarth C, Decety J (2003) The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. Cereb Cortex 13: 837–844

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Adolphs R (2003) Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci 1:4165–4178

    Google Scholar 

  29. Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:169–192

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau M-C, Mazziotta JC, Lenzi GL (2003) Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci, USA 100:5497–5502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Gallese V, Goldman A (1998) Mirror neurons and simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends Cogn Sci 2:493–501

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Johnson S, Booth A, O’Heam K (2001) Inferring the goals of nonhuman agent. Cogn Dev 16:637–656

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Miyashita T, Ishiguro H (2004) Human-like natural behavior generation based on involuntary motions for humanoid robots. Robot Auton Syst 48:203–212

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Moss F, Wiesenfeld K (1995) The benefits of background noise. Sci Am 273:50–53

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Nozaki D, Mar D, Grigg P, Collins JJ (1999) Effect of colored noise on stochastic resonance in sensory neurons. Phys Rev Lett 82:2402–2405

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Kiss LB, Gingl Z, Marton Z, Kertesz J, Moss F, Schmera G, Bulsara A (1993) 1/f noise in systems showing stochastic resonance. J Stat Phys 70:451–462

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Ishiguro I (2005) Android science: toward a new cross-interdisciplinary framework. Proc Cogsci2005, workshop, pp 1–6

  38. Mori M (1970) Bukimi no tani (uncanny valley). Energy 7:33–35

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Haruaki Fukuda.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Fukuda, H., Ueda, K. Interaction with a Moving Object Affects One’s Perception of Its Animacy. Int J of Soc Robotics 2, 187–193 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-010-0045-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12369-010-0045-z

Keywords

Navigation