Skip to main content
Log in

Me, Myself, and Semiotic Function: Finding the “I” in Biology

  • Published:
Biosemiotics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This essay argues that stable, heritable, habituated semiotics on one scale of life allows for opportunism, origination, and the solving of novel problems on others. This is grounded in how interpretation is neither caused nor determined by its object, such that success at interpretation simply cannot be defined by any comparison between an interpretation and its object. Rather, an interpretation is a reciprocated incorporation of a living thing and its environment, and successful if it furthers the living, interpreting thing. By applying biosemiotic theory to seemingly disparate studies of parasitic infections (Jaroslav Flegr), autonomic nervous systems (Stephen Porges), and social change (Charles Tilly) as well as the classical pragmatic notion that biology, psychology and sociology are disparate approaches to the singular, radically continuous, and perennial question of who (or what) am I (Dewey, James, Mead). I argue that the distinction (e.g.,) between voluntary and autonomic behavior is but a ghost of older dualisms, the pseudo-contradictions of matter v. mind, body v. soul, but also self v. not self. Moreover, all such pseudo-contradictions (individual v. social, sensation v. response, parasite v. host, and etc.) are resolved as scale thick, self-similar examples of semiotic transaction wherein degeneration or habituation on one scale of life allows for generative or novel interaction on another.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Richard Burger in Hitt (2005)

  2. To steal a phrase from Kauffman (2002)

  3. Havel (1996): distinct scales appear in nature when the assessing of a phenomena requires a separate set of metrics: a thing is scale thick when it can be measured by multiple sets of metrics and is consequential on multiple scales of being, and scale thin when it is not.

  4. Lhotský et. al. (Publication pending) Symbioses from the point of reciprocal re-forming

  5. Ostdiek (2012, 2016), Tilly (1998), and Nöth (2013)

  6. Flegr (2007)

  7. Morgan (1903). p. 59.

  8. Darwin (1859). P. 490

  9. Stilling (2013) and Mazmanian (2013)

  10. Flegr (2002, 2007).

  11. James (1956) pg. 216–254

  12. Flegr (2007)

  13. Flegr (2007)

  14. Ingram et al. (2013)

  15. Porges (2003)

  16. Porges (1995)

  17. Portes (2009)

  18. E.g., Langley (1921)

  19. Austin et al. (2007)

  20. Ostdiek (2012, 2016), and Nöth (2013).

  21. Peirce (1998) EP 2.222, italics added

  22. Tilly (1998b), pg. 456

  23. Tilly (1996), pg. 7

  24. Tilly (2000)

  25. James (1956) pg. 216–254

  26. Santayana, G. (1980/1905) pg. 284

  27. Santayana (1920), pgs. 7, 53–4, 64–96, Santayana held that James was ‘irresponsible’ towards the past (which he ‘raided’ philosophically), and that he belied progress by begetting forgetfulness. And yet, Santayana also offered James the highest praise available within his weltenshauung, that he had “the temperament of an artist.” Theirs was a complicated relationship.

  28. James (1904a), pg. 533

  29. To be more exact, we must differentiate between genuine and degenerate experience: here, I speak of the first, which involves sentiency, Peircean Firstness, and the opening of possibility, and not the second, wherein consequence is determined by object and interaction, and which involves probabilities but not possibilities. See Ostdiek (2014)

  30. For the original idea, see Peirce e.g., (1977/1908) S.S. p. 80; for its development as a biology of minding, see Dewey (1934) chapters 1 and 3, and by biosemiotics as a philosophy of science, see Ostdiek in Favareau et al. (2012) pgs 263–5

  31. Ostdiek (2011) and Flegr (2010)

  32. E.g., Dewey et al. (1903) and James (1904b)

  33. James (1907) Chapter 2

  34. Dewey (1896)

  35. This being a central tenant of Pragmatism, e.g., James (1956), pg. 131 “Man's chief difference from the brutes lies in the exuberant excess of his subjective propensities”

  36. This claim is a) the central proposition of this essay, b) a corollary of the immediately previous proposition, and c) valid—based on evidence offered above.

  37. Savage (2010)

  38. O’Dwyer (2015)

  39. James (1890) Vol. 1 pg. 291 and Ostdiek (2016)

  40. Mead (1913) and also Ostdiek (2012)

  41. Whitman (1892/1855): e.g., section 51, lines 1324–6

  42. Bruni and Giorgi (2015)

References

  • Austin, M., Riniolo, T., & Porges, S. (2007). Borderline personality disorder and emotional regulations: insights from the polyvagal theory. Brain Cognition, 65(1), 69–76.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bruni, L., & Giorgi, F. (2015). Towards a heterarchical approach to biology and cognition. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular biology, 119(3), 481–492.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1859). The origin of species. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex Arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3, 357–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J., et al. (1903). Studies in logical theory (The decennial publications, second series, volume XI). Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Favareau, D., et al. (2012). A more developed sign: Interpreting the work of Jesper Hoffmeyer. Tartu: Tartu University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flegr, J. (2002). Increased risk of traffic accidents in subjects with latent toxoplasmosis: a retrospective case-control study. BMC Infectious Diseases, 2, 11. doi:10.1186/1471-2334-2-11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Flegr, J. (2007). Effects of toxoplasma on human behavior. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33(3), 757–760.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Flegr, J. (2010). Elastic, not plastic species: frozen plasticity theory and the origin of adaptive evolution in sexually reproducing organisms. Biology Direct, 5, 2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Havel, I. M. (1996). Scale dimensions in nature. International Journal of General Systems, 24(3), 295–679.324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hitt, J. (2005). Mighty white of you, Harper’s magazine. July, 2005. Online at: www.middlebury.edu/media/view/157971.

  • Ingram, W., et al. (2013). Mice infected with low-virulence strains of Toxoplasma gondii lose their innate aversion to cat urine, even after extensive parasite clearance. PLoS One, 8(9), e75246. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0075246.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1904a). A world of pure experience. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 1(533–543), 561–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1904b). The Chicago school. Psychological Bulletin, 1, 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1907). Pragmatism. New York: Longman, Green, and Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1956/1897). The will to believe. New York: Dover Publications.

  • Kauffman, S. (2002). Self-organization in biological systems. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langley, J. N. (1921). The autonomic nervous system. Cambridge: Heffer & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lhotský, et. al., (n.d.). Symbioses from the point of reciprocal re-forming. (publication pending).

  • Mazmanian, S. (2013). Microbiota modulates gut physiology and behavioral abnormalities associated with autism. Cell, 155(7), 1451–1463.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mead, G. H. (1913). The social self. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology, and Scientific Methods, 10, 374–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C. L. (1903). An introduction to comparative psychology (2nd ed.). London: W. Scott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nöth, W. (2013). The life of symbols and other legisigns. In V. Romanini (Ed.), Peirce and biosemiotics (pp. 171–182). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Dwyer, D. (2015). Three layers, two sides and one vote. A Medium Corporation: 22.5.2015. Online.

  • Ostdiek, G. (2011). Cast in plastic: semiotic plasticity and the pragmatic reading of Darwin. Biosemiotics, 4(1), 69–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostdiek, G. (2012). The self as social artifice: some consequences of Stanislavski. Biosemiotics, 5, 161–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostdiek, G. (2014). The manufacture of chance. Biosemiotics, 7(3), 361–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ostdiek, G. (2016). Towards a post-biotic anthropology. In K. Pauknerová, et al. (Eds.), Non-humans and after in Social Science. Červený Kostelec: Pavel Mervart Publishing.

  • Peirce, C. (1977). In C. S. Hardwick & J. Cook (Eds.), Semiotic and significs. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. (SS).

  • Peirce, C. (1998). The essential peirce: Selected philosophical writings, Vol. 2, Peirce Edition Project. Bloomington: Indiana University Press. (EP).

  • Porges, S. (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: Mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage. A Polyvagal theory. In Psychophysiology 3, pgs. 301–318. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Porges, S. (2003). Social Engagement and attachment: a phylogenetic perspective. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1008, 31–47. doi:10.1196/annals.1301.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Portes, S. (2009). The polyvagal theory: new insights into adaptive reactions of the autonomic nervous system. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 76(Suppl 2), S86–S90. doi:10.3949/ccjm.76.s2.17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Santayana, G. (1920). Character and opinion in the United States: with reminiscences of William James and Josiah Royce and academic life in America. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Santayana, G. (1980/1905). Reason in common sense. New York: Dover.

  • Savage, D. (2010). Grey rights now! The stranger, Savage Love, 18.11.2010 oid=5542868. Online.

  • Stilling, R. (2013). Microbial genes, brain & behaviour - epigenetic regulation of the gut-brain axis. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 13(1), 69–86. doi:10.1111/gbb.12109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tilly, C. (1996). Citizenship, identity and social history. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tilly, C. (1998). Social movements and (all sorts of) other political interactions – local, national, and international – including identities. Theory and Society, 27(4), 453–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tilly, C. (2000). How do relations store histories? Annual Review of Sociology, 26, 721–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitman, W. (1892/1855). Leaves of grass. Philadelphia: David McKay.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Gerald Ostdiek.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ostdiek, G. Me, Myself, and Semiotic Function: Finding the “I” in Biology. Biosemiotics 9, 435–450 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-016-9268-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12304-016-9268-1

Keywords

Navigation