Skip to main content
Log in

Expectations Are More Predictive of Behavior than Behavioral Intentions: Evidence from Two Prospective Studies

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Annals of Behavioral Medicine

Abstract

Background

Understanding the gap between people’s behavioral intentions and their subsequent behavior is a key problem for behavioral scientists, but little attention has been paid to how behavioral intentions are operationalized.

Purpose

Test the distinction between asking people what they intend to do, as opposed to what they expect they will do.

Methods

Two studies were conducted in the domains of alcohol consumption (N = 152) and weight loss (N = 141). Participants completed questionnaires assessing their behavioral intentions, expectations, and self-efficacy at baseline; alcohol consumption/weight were assessed at both baseline and follow-up.

Results

In study 1, expectations were more predictive of alcohol consumption than behavioral intentions, controlling for baseline alcohol consumption and self-efficacy. In study 2, changes in expectations were more predictive of weight loss than changes in behavioral intentions, controlling for baseline weight and self-efficacy.

Conclusion

The findings support a potentially important distinction between behavioral intentions and expectations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that some researchers also make a distinction between “self-efficacy” and “perceived control over behavior” [6, 7]. For example, using factor analysis and a panel of experts, Tavousi et al. [8] were able to distinguish internal influences on perceived control (e.g. confidence in one’s own ability or “self-efficacy”) and external influences on perceived control (e.g. environmental barriers) in relation to substance use among young adolescents, see also [6, 7, 9]. However, we were unable to support such a distinction in study 1, and so we focused on self-efficacy, given that self-efficacy is consistently more predictive of behavior than perceived control over behavior [6, 7].

References

  1. Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum. 1991;50:179-211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Armitage CJ, Conner M. Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. Brit J Soc Psychol. 2001;40:471-499.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Warshaw PR, Davis FD. Disentangling behavioral intentions and behavioral expectations. J Exp Soc Psychol. 1985;21:213-228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Rothschild D, Wolfers J. Forecasting elections: Voter intentions versus expectations. 2011; Retrieved from: http://assets.wharton.upenn.edu/~rothscdm/RothschildExpectations.pdf

  5. Sheppard BH, Hartwick J, Warshaw PR. The theory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with recommendations for modifications and future research. J Consum Res. 1988;15:325-343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Armitage CJ, Conner M. Distinguishing perceptions of control from self-efficacy: Predicting consumption of a low fat diet using the theory of planned behavior. J Appl Soc Psychol. 1999;29:72-90. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1999.tb01375.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Armitage CJ, Conner M. The theory of planned behaviour: Assessment of predictive validity and “perceived control”. Brit J Soc Psychol. 1999;38:35-54. doi:10.1348/014466699164022.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Tavousi M, Hidarnia AR, Montazeri A, Hajizadeh E, Taremain F, Ghofranipour F. Are perceived behavioral control and self-efficacy distinct constructs? Eur J Scientif Res. 2009;30:146-152.

    Google Scholar 

  9. Norman P, Hoyle S. The theory of planned behavior and breast self-examination: Distinguishing between perceived control and self-efficacy. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2004;34:694-708. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02565.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. McConnon A, Raats M, Astrup A, et al. Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour to weight control in an overweight cohort. Results from a pan-European dietary intervention trial (DiOGenes). Appetite. 2012;58:313-318.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. NHS Information Centre. Statistics on alcohol: England, 2011. 2011; Retrieved from: http://www.ic.nhs.uk/statistics-and-data-collections/health-and-lifestyles/alcohol/statistics-on-alcohol-england-2011-%5Bns%5D

  12. Sobell LC, Sobell MB. Timeline follow-back: a technique for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption. In: Litten RZ, Allen JP, eds. Measuring alcohol consumption. Totowa, NJ: Humana Press; 1992:41-72.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  13. Zou GY. Toward using confidence intervals to compare correlations. Psychol Methods. 2007;12:399-413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Preacher KJ, Hayes AF. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Meth Instr. 2008;40:879-891.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Cohen J, Cohen P, West SG, Aiken LS. Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. London: Erlbaum; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Wing RR, Phelan S. Long-term weight loss maintenance. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;82:222S-225S.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Teixeira PJ, Going SB, Sardinha LB, Lohman TG. A review of psychosocial pre-treatment predictors of weight control. Obes Rev. 2005;6:43-65. doi:10.1111/j.1467-789X.2005.00166.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Carver CS, Scheier MF. Control theory—a useful conceptual framework for personality-social, clinical, and health psychology. Psychol Bull. 1982;92:111-135. doi:10.1037//0033-2909.92.1.111.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Al-Rashdan I, Al-Nesef Y. Prevalence of overweight, obesity, and metabolic syndrome among adult Kuwaitis: Results from community-based national survey. Angiology. 2010;61:42-48. doi:10.1177/0003319709333226.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Eastwood P. Statistics on obesity, physical activity and diet: England, 2011. London: The Health and Social Care Information Centre, Lifestyles Statistics; 2011

  21. Nackers LM, Ross KM, Perri MG. The association between rate of initial weight loss and long-term success in obesity treatment: Does slow and steady win the race? Int J Behav Med. 2010;17:161-167. doi:10.1007/s12529-010-9092-y.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. US National Heart, Lung and Blood Institute. Guidelines on overweight and obesity: Electronic textbook. 2005; Retrieved from: http://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/guidelines/obesity/e_txtbk/txgd/4311.htm

  23. Armitage CJ. Can the theory of planned behavior predict the maintenance of physical activity? Health Psychol. 2005;24:235-245.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Perugini M, Bagozzi RP. The distinction between desires and intentions Source. Eur J Soc Psychol. 2004;34:69-84. doi:10.1002/ejsp.186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Rothschild D. Forecasting elections comparing prediction markets, polls, and their biases. Public Opin Quart. 2009;73:895-916. doi:10.1093/poq/nfp082.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Armitage CJ. Time to retire the theory of planned behaviour? A commentary on Sniehotta, Presseau and Araújo-Soares. Health Psychol Rev. 2014. doi:10.1080/17437199.2014.892148.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors’ Statement of Conflict of Interest and Adherence to Ethical Standards

Christopher J. Armitage, Mark Conner, Paul Norman and Soud Alganem declare that they have no conflict of interest. All procedures, including the informed consent process, were conducted in accordance with the ethical standards of the responsible committee on human experimentation (institutional and national) and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2000.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christopher J. Armitage PhD.

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Armitage, C.J., Norman, P., Alganem, S. et al. Expectations Are More Predictive of Behavior than Behavioral Intentions: Evidence from Two Prospective Studies. ann. behav. med. 49, 239–246 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-014-9653-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-014-9653-4

Keywords

Navigation