Abstract
In two pre-registered studies, we investigated the relationship of left-wing authoritarianism with the ego-focused trait of narcissism. Based on existing research, we expected individuals with higher levels of left-wing authoritarianism to also report higher levels of narcissism. Further, as individuals with leftist political attitudes can be assumed to be striving for social equality, we expected left-wing authoritarianism to also be positively related to prosocial traits, but narcissism to remain a significant predictor of left-wing authoritarianism above and beyond those prosocial dispositions. We investigated our hypotheses in two studies using cross-sectional correlational designs. Two nearly representative US samples (Study 1: Nā=ā391; Study 2: Nā=ā377) completed online measures of left-wing authoritarianism, the Dark Triad personality traits, and two variables with a prosocial focus (i.e., altruism and social justice commitment). In addition, we assessed relevant covariates (i.e., age, gender, socially desirable responding, and virtue signaling). The results of multiple regression analyses showed that a strong ideological view, according to which a violent revolution against existing societal structures is legitimate (i.e., anti-hierarchical aggression), was associated with antagonistic narcissism (Study 1) and psychopathy (Study 2). However, neither dispositional altruism nor social justice commitment was related to left-wing anti-hierarchical aggression. Considering these results, we assume that some leftist political activists do not actually strive for social justice and equality but rather use political activism to endorse or exercise violence against others to satisfy their own ego-focused needs. We discuss these results in relation to the dark-ego-vehicle principle.
On 6 January 2021, the United States Capitol in Washington DC was attacked by a group of individuals who tried to impede the validation of the presidential election. Due to the violent attack, several people were killed and injured. Many argued that the outgoing president Donald Trump had incited the attack on the Capitol ā he had lost the 2020 presidential election to president-elect Joe Biden but refused to concede claiming election fraud. But who were the people following Donald Trumpās (alleged) call? According to the New York Times (Barry et al., 2021, January 9), āthey came from around the country with different affiliations ā QAnon, Proud Boys, elected officials, everyday Americans [ā¦]ā But even though they stemmed from different traits of life, did some of these individuals share a common personality trait called authoritarianism?
In psychological and political research, authoritarianism has been characterized as (1) a submissiveness to authority figures and (2) a dominance towards subordinates (Adorno et al., 1950). In the past, authoritarianism has been dominantly investigated in individuals with right-wing political ideologies which led to the term right-wing authoritarianism (RWA; Altemeyer, 1996). Individuals with high levels of RWA have been described as people striving for (1) the strict endorsement of conservative social norms and values (i.e., conventionalism), (2) the compliance with established authorities (i.e., authoritarian submission), and (3) antagonistic behavior toward outgroup members (i.e., authoritarian aggression). In line with this description, current research found individuals with higher levels of RWA to be more close-minded (e.g., Hodson et al., 2009), fundamentalistic in their religious orientation, and prejudiced towards minority groups (Altemeyer & Hunsberger, 1992). However, in later theoretical conceptualizations (Altemeyer, 1998), it was postulated that RWA seems to be more reflective of the submissive aspect of authoritarianism while another construct ā social dominance orientation (SDO) ā was assumed to mirror the dominance aspect of authoritarianism. Individuals with high SDO have been characterized as people opposing social equality in support of group-based hierarchies by striving to dominate weaker out-groups. Supporting this later postulate, empirical studies have shown that SDO is associated with racism, homophobia, and attitudes unsupportive of womenās rights whilst negatively correlated with empathy, tolerance, and altruism (Pratto et al., 1994). Further, it was found that SDO is related to narcissism (e.g., Cichocka et al., 2017; Zeigler-Hill et al., 2021) and dark personality traits (e.g., Hodson et al., 2009).
While there is wide agreement that RWA and SDO are valid psychological constructs (for a critical review of the measures on RWA, see Harms et al., 2018), the notion of left-wing authoritarianism (LWA) has been met with skepticism by many researchers (e.g., Altemeyer, 1996; Jost et al., 2003; Nilsson & Jost, 2020) even though some empirical studies found evidence for the existence of authoritarianism also on the left side of the political spectrum (e.g., Conway et al., 2018; Crawford & Brandt, 2020). Recently, the discussion around LWA has gotten fresh attention: On 25 May 2020, George Floyd ā an unarmed Black man āwas killed by Minneapolis police. This event induced massive Black Lives Matter (BLM) protests all over the US and the world. And while most (94%) of the racial justice protests in the US were conducted peacefully, some of these protests (6%) included āviolence, clashes with the police, vandalism, looting, or other destructive activityā (ACLED, May 2021, p. 1). Later reports found that much of the violence had been directed at the pro-BLM demonstrators, for example by the police (Chenoweth & Pressman, 2020, Oct 16). But had some left-wing authoritarians also been involved in the aggressive activities? This question was addressed by a very recent empirical study (Costello et al., 2022, study phase 6). Out of a nationally representative US sample of Nā=ā834 participants, the researchers found 67 individuals who endorsed the violence during the BLM protests. Among those individuals, LWA (but not RWA) was found to predict the expressed support and the extremity of the support. Also, 19 individuals reported to have actually engaged in violence during the BLM protests. However, their aggressive engagement was neither statistically significantly associated with LWA nor RWA. Further, 73 individuals reported to having desired to use violence for a political cause during the last five years (i.e., aside from the pro-BLM protests). Among those participants, the study found that both LWA and RWA were positively correlated with the desired frequency of violence but only the correlation with RWA to reach statistical significance. In concert, these results indicate that authoritarianism cannot only be found on the right side of the political spectrum but might also be prevalent on the political left (see also Conway et al., 2018). This notion is further supported by findings of the Polarization Research Lab (2022, December) which show that even though most followers of both political sides reject violence, some left-wingers are more likely than right-wingers to endorse harming or even murdering their political opponents.
Based on those previous empirical findings, the goal of the present paper is to further investigate ego-focus correlates of LWA. Throughout this paper, based on the conceptualization by Costello et al. (2022), we assume LWA to be a tripartite construct comprising of three correlated dimensions: (1) anticonventionalism, (2) top-down censorship, and (3) antihierarchical aggression. The anticonventionalism dimension of LWA is characterized by the absolute endorsement of progressive moral values. For example, individuals with high levels of anticonventionalism might declare anyone to be homophobic who is opposing gay marriage. The LWA dimension of anticonventionalism seems to contrast the RWA dimension of conventionalism which is mirrored by the strict endorsement of conservative social norms and values. However, Costello et al. (2022) found similiarities between the nomological nets of LWA and RWA/SDO. For example, after controlling for political ideology, LWA anticonventionalism was also associated with lower openness and higher dogmatism.
LWA anticonventionalism is assumed to not only lead to an intolerance towards conservative values but also to the desire to impose those progressive moral values on others (Costello et al., 2022). This desire goes along with top-down censorship as well as antihierarchical aggression. The top-down censorship dimension of LWA is described as the preference for the use of authority (governmental and institutional) to deal with opposition and the strive to suppress any speech that is considered as offensive and intolerant. For example, individuals with high levels of top-down censorship may strive to suppress free speech to regulate the expression of right-wing beliefs in educational institutions. Similarly, individuals high in RWA support the limitation of free speech, however, as a means to endorse right-wing values.
The antihierarchical aggression dimension of LWA has been defined as āthe motivation to forcefully overthrow the established hierarchy and punish those in powerā (Costello et al., 2022, p.162). For example, individuals might express their antihierarchical aggression by the endorsement of political violence to fight for social justice. Individuals with high levels of LWA are thus assumed to be hostile towards the present social and moral authorities while feeling morally superior and endorsing the use of violence to reach oneās own political goals. Thus, aggression is prevalent in individuals high in LWA. However, aggression is not a phenomenon reserved for the political left ā aggression directed at members of the opposite political party can also be found in individuals high in SDO as conceptualized by Altemeyer (1998).
Contemporary empirical studies on the ego-focused correlates of LWA are comparatively rare. The existing research, however, points to a relationship between LWA and ego-focused traits such as narcissism (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2021) as well as psychopathy (Costello et al., 2022, phase 4). In the present research, we thus wanted to shed further light on the relations of LWA with narcissism. Going beyond the research of Zeigler-Hill et al. (2021), we investigated the relationship between narcissism and the three subfacets of LWA (i.e., anticonventionalism, top-down censorship, and antihierarchical aggression). However, as left-wing political attitudes also include prosocial concerns (i.e., the striving for social equality), in the present research, we simultaneously controlled for the relationship of LWA and prosocial focused dispositions (i.e., altruism and social justice commitment). This approach allowed us to investigate if narcissism is related to LWA above and beyond the predictive power of those prosocial traits.
To investigate our pre-registered research questions, we conducted two cross-sectional correlational studies using nationally nearly representative US samples. In particular, Study 1 explored the associations of LWA with narcissism beyond and above altruism, while Study 2 investigated the relationships between LWA and narcissism (Paulhus & Williams, 2002) above and beyond social justice commitment. All measures were assessed with self-report instruments. To acknowledge the limitations of self-reports regarding individualsā tendency to answer in a socially desirable way (Stƶber et al., 2002), we additionally included established measures to account for such tendencies in both studies. As another covariate, we included participantsā age as narcissistic traits are susceptible to change over oneās life course (Cramer, 2011). Finally, we included gender as a covariate as gender-specific differences in narcissism have been reported (Grijalva et al., 2015).
Study 1
Study 1 had two goals. Firstly, we wanted to investigate the relationship between LWA (and its respective subfacets) and narcissism. In the following, we use the terms ānarcissismā and ānarcissistā when referring to subclinical narcissism as a personality trait (i.e., to the continuous distribution of narcissism in the broader community; Furnham et al., 2013). Further, paralleling the research of Zeigler-Hill et al. (2021), we relied on the three-dimensional conception of narcissism provided by Miller et al. (2016) who characterize individuals high in narcissistic traits as (1) demonstrating manipulative and exploitative behaviors, indulging in self-perceived entitlement, arrogance, reactive anger, distrust, lack of empathy, and thrill-seeking (so called antagonistic narcissism); (2) acclaim seeking, authoritative, indulging in grandiose fantasies, and demonstrating exhibitionism (so called extraverted narcissism); and (3) experiencing shame, low indifference, and a need for admiration (so called neurotic narcissism).
Previous studies regarding the relationship between narcissism and global LWA (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2021) found LWA to be positively correlated with antagonistic narcissism (rā=ā.61, pā<ā.001), extraverted narcissism (rā=ā.38, pā<ā.001), and neurotic narcissism (rā=ā.23, pā<ā.001). The results of those studies also showed that LWA (global) was most strongly predicted by antagonistic narcissism (bā=ā0.34, pā<ā.001) when neurotic narcissism (bā=ā0.17, pā<ā.001) and extraverted narcissism (bā=ā0.00, pā>ā.05) were simultaneously included in the analysis.
Interestingly, the authors found similar patterns for SDO, which was also most strongly predicted by antagonistic narcissism (bā=ā0.25, pā<ā.05) when neurotic narcissism (bā=ā0.12, pā<ā.05) and extraverted narcissism (bā=ā0.10, pā>ā.05) were included in the analysis. From these results, Zeigler-Hill and his colleagues concluded that individuals with high levels of antagonistic narcissism may be ruthlessly motivated to endorse either right- or left-wing ideological attitudes depending on which of these attitudes seems to be more advantageous to them in a specific situation.
Based on these finding on the association between LWA and narcissism (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2021), in Study 1, we expected LWA to be positively related to narcissism (pre-registered Hypothesis 1). In particular, for the main analyses, we assumed the global LWA score as well as each of the three subfacets of LWA (i.e., anticonventionalism, top-down censorship, and antihierarchical aggression) to be positively related to the three subfacets of narcissism (i.e., antagonistic, extraverted, and neurotic narcissism). Thus, going beyond existing research, we examined not only narcissism but also LWA differentially by considering its subfacets. In a secondary analysis, we investigated the relationship between global LWA and its subfacets on the one hand and narcissism on the other hand applying an alternative conceptualization of narcissism which differentiates between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism (Sherman et al., 2015). Again, we expected positive relationships between each of the LWA scores and these two subfacets of narcissism.
Secondly, as a novel aspect, we explored the relationship between LWA and narcissism above and beyond dispositional altruism. Altruistic individuals are people with prosocial tendencies who support others at the price of personal cost (Dargan & Schermer, 2022). They have been described as sympathetic, soft-hearted, and generous (Lee & Ashton, 2006). Trait altruism is observed in individuals who demonstrate altruistic behaviors across different situations, for example giving money to strangers, helping others to move their household, or volunteering for a social cause (Rushton et al., 1981). Studies show that altruism also predicts participation in politics such as voting, volunteering, and protesting (Fowler & Kam, 2007). Further, altruism has been demonstrated to be positively related to left-wing political attitudes (Zettler & Hilbig, 2010). Hence, we also expected LWA (and its subfacets) to be positively related to altruism (pre-registered Hypothesis 2).
Method
Open Science and ethical requirements
Study 1 was pre-registered before the data collection with AsPredicted.org (https://aspredicted.org/qc4hy.pdf ā identifier #97,770) under the title āUnderstanding Left-Wing Authoritarianism ā Study 1ā. The study was approved (identifier #2022-05-00006) by the human research ethics committee of the University of Bern. Informed consent was obtained according to the guidelines of the university. The complete study materials including the raw data are available at https://researchbox.org/751.
Power analysis and participants
We calculated the sample size necessary to obtain sufficient power of 80% to detect an existing effect (Cohen, 1988) using G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2009). Assuming an effect of each of the predicted sixteen bivariate Pearson product-moment correlations of |Ļ| = 0.21 (cf., Schƶnbrodt & Perugini, 2013) and a respective Bonferroni corrected error probability of αā=ā0.003125 (two-tailed), the results of the power analysis revealed a minimum sample size of Nā=ā320. The sample was recruited using the Prolific recruitment platform (https://www.prolific.co). Expecting some data loss (e.g., due to failed attention checks), we aimed at recruiting Nā=ā400 participants nationally representative of the US via this online-crowdsourcing system (detailed information on representative samples recruited via Prolific is provided by Costello et al., 2022). In particular, we applied Prolificās option for US nationally representative sampling. Ultimately, 413 individuals started to participate in the survey. Twenty-two of these participants were excluded from the original sample because they did not complete the survey (nā=ā11), failed the attention check (nā=ā8), or were suspected of being duplicate respondents (nā=ā3). Hence, the final sample consisted of Nā=ā391 individuals (Mage = 46.230 years, SDā=ā16.390, rangeā=ā18ā93 years). For the full report of the sample demographics for Study 1, see supplementary Table S1.
Procedure
Study 1 was announced as an āAttitudes and Behaviors Studyā and lasted about 20Ā min. The study was conducted online ā all instructions and measures were provided via the survey software Qualtrics. After giving written informed consent, participants indicated their age and gender as well as other demographic information (i.e., ethnicity, native language, highest educational level, income, marital status, and sexual orientation). Next, an attention check (Bertrams & Schlegel, 2020) was administered to ensure that participants had carefully read the instructions. In particular, participants were presented with a question (i.e., āWho was the first president of the United States of America?ā) and three possible answers (i.e., Abraham Lincoln, George Washington, or Thomas Jefferson). Participants were explicitly instructed not to answer this question to demonstrate that they had given attention to this instruction. Next, participants were randomly presented with the items measuring narcissism and altruism (i.e., some participants first answered the items measuring narcissism while other participants first answered the altruism items). Afterwards, participantsā proneness to socially desirable responding (i.e., self-deception and impression management) was assessed with the respective items of both scales presented in a mixed and random order. Further, LWA was measured. In a final step, participants reported on their political orientation as well as their political ideology, and the political party they identify with. After that, participants provided their Prolific ID, were thanked for their participation, and debriefed. After completing the study, participants received a fixed payment of Ā£2.70 (about $3.50).
Measures
Narcissism
Narcissism was measured with the Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory (FFNI; Sherman et al., 2015). The FFNI is a self-report measure with 60 items and allows for the assessment of narcissism on three subdimensions (Miller et al., 2016): antagonism (32 items; e.g., āIām pretty good at manipulating peopleā), agentic extraversion (16 items; e.g., āLeadership comes easy for meā), and neuroticism (12 items; e.g., āWhen I realize I have failed at something, I feel humiliatedā). Ratings were made using 5-point scales from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly). After recoding the respective inverse items, we calculated a total FFNI mean score with high scores indicating a high level of narcissism. The scale showed good internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.893). Next, we calculated a mean score for each of the three respective FFNI dimensions with high scores indicating a high level of antagonism, agentic extraversion, or neuroticism. The subscales showed good to excellent internal consistency (antagonism: McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.910; agentic extraversion: McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.875; neuroticism: McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.911). Finally, for analyses with an alternative conception of narcissistic traits (Sherman et al., 2015), we calculated a mean score assessing grandiose narcissismFootnote 1 (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.910) and vulnerable narcissism (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.859).
Altruism
Altruism was assessed with the Self-Report Altruism Scale (SRA; Rushton et al., 1981). The SRA measures altruism with twenty items representative of altruistic behaviors (e.g., āI have given money to a charityā). Participants were instructed to rate how often they had engaged in such behaviors in the past using 5-point scales from 1 (never) to 5 (very often). For the data analyses, we calculated a mean score including all twenty SRA items, with high scores indicating a high level of altruism. The scale showed good internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.874).
Socially desirable responding
Proneness to socially desirable responding was assessed with the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding (BIDR; Paulhus, 1984; 1988). With its 40 items, the BIDR measures two constructs: self-deceptive enhancement (SDE; 20 items; e.g., āMy first impressions of people usually turn out to be rightā) and impression management (IM; 20 items; e.g., āI never cover up my mistakesā). Ratings were made using 7-point scales from 1 (not true) to 7 (very true). First, we recoded the respective inverse items. Second, in line with the findings of Stƶber et al. (2002), we used continuous scoring (i.e., all answers on the continuous answer scale are counted). This way, we calculated a mean SDE score with high scores indicating a high level of self-deceptive enhancement. The SDE scale showed good internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.798). Further, we calculated a mean IM score high scores indicating a high level of impression management. The IM scale also showed good internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.859).
Left-wing authoritarianism
LWA was measured with the Left-Wing Authoritarian Index (LWAI; Costello et al., 2022). The LWAI is a self-report measure with 39 items allowing for the assessment of LWA and its three subdimensions: anticonventionalism (13 items; e.g., āAnyone who opposes gay marriage must be homophobicā), top-down censorship (13 items; e.g., āUniversity authorities are right to ban hateful speech from campusā), and antihierarchical aggression (13 items; e.g., āThe rich should be stripped of their belongings and statusā). Ratings were made using 7-point scales from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). After recoding the respective inverse items, we calculated a total LWA mean score with high scores indicating a high level of LWA. The scale showed excellent internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.953). Further, we calculated a mean score for each of the three respective LWA subdimensions with high scores indicating a high level of anticonventionalism, top-down censorship, or antihierarchical aggression. The subscales showed excellent internal consistency (anticonventionalism: McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.938; top-down censorship: McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.898; antihierarchical aggression: McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.916).
Political orientation
In accordance with the approach of Costello et al. (2022), participantsā political orientation was assessed with one item asking participants to place themselves on a 7-point scale with regard to their political views ranging from 1 (extremely left-wing/far-left) to 7 (extremely right-wing/far-right).
Results
Common method bias test
In the present research, we relied on self-report questionnaires. The adoption of self-report questionnaires can lead to common method variance when the examined variables are assessed at the same point in time (Chang et al., 2010). To examine, if a common method bias occurred, we used Harmanās single-factor test as described by Zhang et al. (2022). For this purpose, we included all measured items in an unrotated principal component analysis to test if only one component emerges from this analysis or if the first component explains the vast majority of the variation (>ā40%) as both cases would be indicative of the existence of severe common method bias. In the analysis, 35 components emerged explaining 69.67% of the variance in Study 1. Further, the first component explained only 13% of the variation, which is below the critical value. Thus, we assume that common method variance did not severely bias the results of Study 1.
Preregistered main analyses
Descriptive statistics for all variables can be found in supplementary Table S1. Bivariate correlations for all analyzed variables are depicted in TableĀ 1. In line with our predictions, inter-correlations revealed a positive association between LWA and narcissism: Overall, we found a positive correlation between the global LWA score and the FFNI total score (rā=ā.178, pā<ā.001). With regard to the three narcissism subscales, we found the total LWA score to be statistically significantly related only to the FFNI neuroticism score (rā=ā.268, pā<ā.001). With respect to the LWA subscale scores, we found antihierarchical aggression to be positively related to the FFNI scores for antagonism (rā=ā.249, pā<ā.001) and neuroticism (rā=ā.222, pā<ā.001). The other LWA subscales were positively related only to the FFNI neuroticism score (LWA anticonventionalism: rā=ā.255, pā<ā.001; LWA top-down censorship: rā=ā.205, pā<ā.001). Surprisingly, we did not find the expected positive associations between LWA and altruism. On the contrary, altruism was negatively correlated to the total LWA score (r = ā.153, pā=ā.002) as well as to antihierarchical aggression (r = ā.180, pā<ā.001).
To further test our assumptions, in a next step we calculated four hierarchical multiple linear regressions predicting LWA (i.e., the total LWA score and each of the three LWA subscales respectively) by altruism and the three FFNI subfacets of narcissism. As general assumptions for the regression analyses (Field, 2018), we first checked the variance inflation factor (VIF) to test for extreme collinearity. VIF values for all variables were lower than the threshold of 10.00 (all VIFsā<ā2.56). Therefore, extreme collinearity did not occur in the data. Second, the results of the respective Durbin-Watson tests showed that there was no autocorrelation.
The results revealed that the total LWA score (Model 1a, supplementary Table S2) was only predicted by the FFNI neuroticism score (bā=ā0.279, SE bā=ā0.060, βā=ā0.233, pā<ā.001). However, the FFNI neuroticism score was no longer a significant predictor of LWA when we controlled for age, gender, self-deceptive enhancement, and impression management in a second block (see Model 1b, supplementary Table S2). Similar results were found when we predicted the LWA score for anticonventionalism (see Models 2a & 2b, supplementary Table S3) and top-down censorship (see Models 3a & 3b, supplementary Table S4). The only exception from this pattern was found when we predicted LWA antihierarchical aggression: The respective LWA score was only statistically significantly predicted by the FFNI score for antagonism (bā=ā0.380, SE bā=ā0.121, βā=ā0.181, pā=ā.002) when we controlled for age, gender, self-deceptive enhancement, and impression management (see Model 4b, TableĀ 2).
Preregistered secondary analyses
In a final step, we repeated the data analyses to examine the relationships between LWA and the alternative narcissism subfacets of grandiose and vulnerable narcissism (Sherman et al., 2015). Again, we calculated four hierarchical multiple linear regressions predicting LWA (i.e., the total LWA score and each of the three LWA subscales respectively), but this time by altruism and these two subfacets of narcissism (i.e., FFNI grandiose narcissism, FFNI vulnerable narcissism). All VIF values were againā<ā2.56 and results of the respective Durbin-Watson tests showed that there was no autocorrelation.
In all these regression analyses, the respective LWA score was statistically significantly predicted by the FFNI score for vulnerable narcissism (see Models 6a to 9a, supplementary Tables S5 to S8). However, this FFNI score was no longer predictive for any of the respective LWA scores, when we controlled for age, gender, self-deceptive enhancement and impression management (see Models 6b to 9b, supplementary Tables S5 to S8).
Discussion
In Study 1, we investigated the relationship between LWA and the ego-focused trait of narcissism above and beyond the influence of altruism. In a first step, the results of the data analyses showed that LWA (and all its subfacets) were predicted by neurotic narcissism. Interestingly, and contrary to our predictions, we did not find any relationship between LWA (and its subfacets) and altruism. These results seem to imply that individuals high in LWA are also individuals with high levels of neurotic narcissism (i.e., individuals who strongly care about what others might think about them, who experience high levels of shame, and have a strong need for admiration).
However, when we controlled for other relevant variables such as age, gender, and the tendency for socially-desirable responding, the relationship between LWA (and its subfacets) and neurotic narcissism was no longer detectable. Then, a robust relationship between the LWA subfacet of antihierarchical aggression and antagonistic narcissism was unveiled. Antihierarchical aggression represents the drive to use force to overthrow those in power and who endorse conservative values. The results of Study 1 suggest that this motivation can be more likely found in individuals who exploit others for their own interests, lack empathy, have a sense of entitlement, are arrogant and manipulative, demonstrate reactive anger and distrust others while at the same time seeking thrill.
The results of Study 1 are also in line with previous research (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2021) that found LWA (global) to be most strongly predicted by antagonistic narcissism (bā=ā0.34, pā<ā.001) when controlled for neurotic narcissism (bā=ā0.17, pā<ā.001) and extraverted narcissism (bā=ā0.00, pā>ā.05). In their study, Zeigler-Hill and his colleagues further found the relationship between antagonistic narcissism and global LWA to be mediated by a competitive social worldview. From this, the authors concluded that individuals with high levels of antagonistic narcissism may perceive the world as a highly competitive place which needs to be dominated, for example via the strong endorsement of left-wing ideological attitudes. Interestingly, Zeigler-Hill and his colleagues found a similar pattern for the relationship between antagonistic narcissism and SDO. From these results, the authors concluded that individuals with high levels of antagonistic narcissism may be ruthlessly motivated to endorse either right- or left-wing ideological attitudes depending on which of these attitudes seems to be more advantageous to them in a specific situation. For example, an individual with a high level of antagonistic narcissism may engage in social justice (i.e., left-wing) protesting as long as they are not in a privileged position themselves. As a novel aspect, the results of Study 1 of the present research suggest that such an individual may particularly be motivated to endorse violent attacks against the established social institutions demonstrating LWA antihierarchical aggression.
Study 2
In Study 2, we further investigated the relationship between narcissism and LWA. However, we now focused on LWA antihierarchical aggression as it was the only LWA subfacet robustly related to narcissism in Study 1. Based on the results of Study 1, we expected the LWA dimension of antihierarchical aggression to be positively related to narcissism (pre-registered Hypothesis 3). As a novel aspect, we used another measure for narcissism (i.e., the Short Dark Triad; Jones & Paulhus, 2014) that incorporates antagonism in a narcissism subscale besides two other dark traits (i.e., Machiavellianism and psychopathy).
Secondly ā parallel to the design of Study 1 ā we explored the relationship between narcissism and LWA antihierarchical aggression above and beyond individualsā prosocial motives. However, for Study 2, we did not include altruism but social justice commitment as an expression of participantsā prosocial attitudes. Social justice focuses on the positive outcome of āchanging or transforming inequality among underprivileged subgroups within society to be more equitableā (Fietzer & Ponterotto, 2015) and tackles issues like the fight against poverty, racism, and discrimination (e.g., due to sexual orientation, ethnicity, religion, or gender) (Miller et al., 2009). Social justice commitment refers to an individualās intention to pursue social justice related (i.e., prosocial) activities in the future. Research shows that social justice commitment is related to political activism (e.g., participating in a pro-BLM demonstration; Hope et al., 2016). Further, social justice was shown to be positively related to political liberalism and negatively associated with SDO (Janoff-Bulman et al., 2008). Hence, we assume the LWA dimension of antihierarchical aggression to be positively associated with social justice commitment (preregistered Hypothesis 4).
Given the overlap between narcissism and Machiavellianism as well as psychopathy found in previous research (Jones & Paulhus, 2014), we also considered these two dark personality traits as covariates. Further, LWA anticonventionalism and LWA top-down censorship were treated as covariates. To acknowledge the limitations of self-reports regarding individualsā tendency to answer in a socially desirable way (Stƶber et al., 2002), we included virtue signaling as another measure of socially desirable responding in Study 2. According to Ok et al. (2021), virtue signaling refers to an individualās (i.e., the senderās) demonstration of symbolic behaviors so that any observing person (i.e., the receiver) makes favorable inferences about the signalerās moral character. Lastly, we again included participantsā age and gender as covariates in the analyzes because narcissistic traits are depending on changes over oneās life course (Cramer, 2011) and gender-differences (Grijalva et al., 2015).
Method
Open science and ethical requirements
The second survey was also pre-registered before the data collection with AsPredicted.org (https://aspredicted.org/2sy8f.pdf ā identifier #99,079) under the title āUnderstanding Left-Wing AuthoritarianismĀ ā Study 2ā. The second survey was also approved (identifier #2022-05-00006) by the human research ethics committee of the University of BernĀ . Informed consent was obtained the same way as it was obtained for the first survey. The complete materials and the raw data for the second study are available at https://researchbox.org/752.
Power analysis and participants
For study 2, we again used G*Power 3.1 (Faul et al., 2009) to calculate the necessary sample size. Assuming a bivariate effect of |Ļ| = 0.21 (cf., Schƶnbrodt & Perugini, 2013) for the two relationships of central interest (LWA: antihierarchical aggression and narcissism; LWA: antihierarchical aggression and social justice commitment) and a respective Bonferroni corrected error probability of αā=ā0.025 (two-tailed), the results of the power analysis revealed a minimum sample size of Nā=ā212. The sample was recruited using the Prolific recruitment platform (https://www.prolific.co). Again, we aimed at recruiting Nā=ā400 participants nationally representative of the US via this online-crowdsourcing. Applying Prolificās respective option, we a priori excluded those individuals who had already participated in Study 1. Ultimately, four hundred and twenty-eight individuals started to participate in the survey. Fifty-one of these participants were excluded from the original sample because they did not complete the survey (nā=ā19), failed the attention check (nā=ā19), or were suspected of being duplicate respondents (nā=ā13). Thus, the final sample consisted of Nā=ā377 individuals (Mage = 46.040 years, SDā=ā15.973, rangeā=ā18ā79 years). For the full report of the sample demographics for Study 2, see supplementary Table S1.
Procedure
The second survey was announced as an āAttitudes and Behaviors Studyā and lasted about 15Ā min. The survey was conducted online via the survey software Qualtrics. After giving written informed consent, participants reported their demographic information. Next, the same attention check (Bertrams & Schlegel, 2020) as in Study 1 was administered. Then, participants were randomly presented with the items measuring the dark triad and social justice commitment (i.e., some participants first answered the items measuring the dark triad while other participants first answered the items assessing social justice commitment). Afterwards, participantsā proneness to conduct virtue signaling was assessed. Finally, LWA was measured. In a final step, participants reported on their political orientation as well as their ideology and which political party they identify with. After that, participants provided their Prolific ID, were thanked for their participation in the survey and debriefed. After completing the survey, participants received a fixed payment of Ā£2.00 (about $2.46).
Measures
Dark triad
The dark triad was measured with the Short Dark Triad (SD3; Jones & Paulhus, 2014). With its 27 items, the SD3 assesses the three socially aversive traits of Machiavellianism (e.g., āMost people can be manipulatedā), narcissism (e.g., āI insist on getting the respect I deserveā), and psychopathy (e.g., āIāll say anything to get what I wantā). Ratings were made using 5-point scales from 1 (disagree strongly) to 5 (agree strongly). After recoding the respective inverse items, we calculated a dark triad score using the mean score of all 27 items. The SD3 items showed good internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.878). Next, we calculated a mean score for each of the three respective SD3 dimensions with high scores indicating a high level of the respective socially aversive trait. Together, the nine items assessing narcissism (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.794) showed good internal consistency. The same was true for the nine items assessing Machiavellianism (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.831) and the nine items measuring psychopathy (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.775).
Social justice commitment
Social justice commitment was assessed with the Social Issues Questionnaire (SIQ; Miller et al., 2009). The SIQ is a 52-item scale measuring interest in social justice with six separate scales. For the present study, we only used those four items of the SIQ that measure social justice commitment (e.g., āIn the future, I intent to engage in social justice activities.ā). Participants rated their agreement with the four items using 10-point scales from 0 (strongly disagree) to 9 (strongly agree). For the data analyses, we calculated a mean score including all four SIQ items measuring social justice commitment with high scores indicating a high level of self-reported social justice commitment. The scale showed excellent internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.965).
Virtue-signaling
Virtue-signaling was assessed with the Moral Identity Scale (MIS; Aquino & Reed, 2002) in four steps. With its ten items, the MIS measures two subdimensions of moral identity: internalization (i.e., the extent to which nine morality-related traits are central to oneās own self-concept) and symbolization (i.e., the tendency to publicly express oneās own moral identity). In a first step, participants were presented with a list of nine different positive and morality-related traits (i.e., caring, compassionate, fair, friendly, generous, hardworking, helpful, honest, and kind) and asked to visualize how individuals having these traits would think, feel, and act. In a second step, participantsā internalization of the nine morality-related traits was measured with the five items of the internalization dimension (e.g., āI strongly desire to have these traitsā). Agreement with these five items was rated using 5-point scales from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). After recoding two inverse items, we calculated a mean internalization score with high scores indicating a high level of internalization of moral identity. The internalization subscale showed good internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.801). In a third step, we assessed participantsā tendency to use symbolic actions to publicly express their moral identity with the five items of the symbolization dimension (e.g., āI am actively involved in activities that communicate to others that I have these characteristicsā). Again, ratings of these five items were made using 5-point scales from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5 (strongly agree). Next, we calculated a mean symbolization score with high scores indicating a high level of symbolization of moral identity. The symbolization subscale also showed good internal consistency (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.858). In a fourth and final step, in line with the recommendations of Ok et al. (2021), we used a linear regression analysis to control for participantsā baseline virtue levels by predicting their mean symbolization scores by their respective mean internalization scores and saving the unstandardized residuals as participantsā virtue signaling scores.Footnote 2 Thus, the virtue signaling score represents the degree of how strongly someone tends to publicly demonstrate morality independent of their internalized moral values.
Left-wing authoritarianism
LWA was again measured with the LWAI (Costello et al., 2022). In Study 2, the LWAI showed excellent internal consistency regarding the items included in the total mean LWA score (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.954), assessing antihierarchical aggression (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.916), anticonventionalism (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.938), and top-down censorship (McDonaldās Ļā=ā0.903).
Political orientation
As in Study 1, political orientation was again assessed with one item asking participants to place themselves on a 7-point scale with regard to their political views ranging from 1 (extremely left-wing/far-left) to 7 (extremely right-wing/far-right).
Results
Common method bias test
For Study 2, we again tested for severe common method variance using Harmanās single-factor test as described by Zhang et al. (2022). In the principal components analysis, 15 components emerged explaining 65.18% of the variance in Study 2. The first component explained only 21.01% of the variation, which is again below the critical value of 40%. Thus, we assume that common method variance did also not severely bias the results of Study 2.
Preregistered analyses
Descriptive statistics for all variables can be found in supplementary Table S1. Bivariate correlations for all analyzed variables are depicted in TableĀ 3. The results revealed a positive relationship between narcissism and LWA antihierarchical aggression. However, contrary to our prediction, this correlation did not reach statistical significance (rā=ā.097, pā=ā.059). Surprisingly, we found a significant association between Machiavellianism and antihierarchical aggression (rā=ā.217, pā<ā.001) and an even stronger relationship between psychopathy and antihierarchical aggression (rā=ā.335, pā<ā.001). With regard to social justice commitment, in line with our expectation, we found a significant positive relationship with LWA antihierarchical aggression (rā=ā.357, pā<ā.001).
To further test our expectations, we calculated a hierarchical multiple linear regression predicting LWA anti-hierarchical aggression. All VIF values were <ā1.94 and the results of the two respective Durbin-Watson tests showed that there was no autocorrelation. In a first block, we included only narcissism and social justice commitment as predictors. The results of these analyses can be found in TableĀ 4 (Model 5a). We had expected narcissism to predict LWA antihierarchical aggression above and beyond social justice commitment. However, in this first regression model, LWA anti-hierarchical aggression was only predicted by social justice commitment (bā=ā0.150, SE bā=ā0.021, βā=ā0.350, pā<ā.001). However, when we controlled for the other dark triad traits (i.e., Machiavellianism and psychopathy), the other LWA subfacets (i.e., anticonventionalism and top-down censorship) as well as for age, gender, and virtue signaling (see Model 5b, TableĀ 4), a different pattern emerged: In this analysis, only psychopathy (bā=ā0.470, SE bā=ā0.094, βā=ā0.655, pā<ā.001) was predictive for LWA antihierarchical aggression but neither were narcissism (b = āā0.039, SE bā=ā0.078, β = ā0.022, pā=ā.613) nor social justice commitment (bā=ā0.025, SE bā=ā0.019, βā=ā0.058, pā=ā.170).
Discussion
In Study 2, we further investigated the relationship between narcissism and the LWA subfacet of antihierarchical aggression. Based on the results of Study 1, we had antihierarchical aggression expected to be positively related to narcissism above and beyond individualsā prosocial attitudes (i.e., their social justice commitment). In a first step, the results of the data analyses revealed that antihierarchical aggression was only predicted by social justice commitment but not by narcissism. These results seem to imply that individuals high in antihierarchical aggression (i.e., individuals who endorse the use of violence to reach their own political goals) are also more likely to strive for social justice (i.e., for more equality for the underprivileged subgroups within society). However, when we controlled for the other traits of the dark triad (i.e., Machiavellianism and psychopathy), the other LWA subfacets (i.e., anticonventionalism and top-down censorship) as well as for age, gender, and virtue signaling, a different pattern emerged: In this analysis, the association between antihierarchical aggression and social justice commitment vanished and a relationship between antihierarchical aggression and psychopathy was unveiled.
On the one hand, this result fits the theoretical argument that psychopathy is a strong predictor for extremistsā violence (Gill & Corner, 2017). It is further in line with previous empirical research that found a correlation between LWA and psychopathy (Costello et al., 2022). On the other hand, this result seems to somewhat contradict our pre-registered hypothesis that narcissism would be predictive for antihierarchical aggression. However, it is worth noting that psychopathy is ā besides narcissism and Machiavellianism ā also one of the three personality traits of the dark triad (Jones & Paulhus, 2014). Individuals with dark triad traits share several attributes ā they are self-promoting, emotionally callous, and have a tendency to manipulate others to take advantage of them (Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Hence, all three dark triad traits are overlapping personality traits (Furnham et al., 2013). Nevertheless, each of these traits also has unique aspects: For psychopaths, the callousness is accompanied by a general disregard for social norms and the element of impulsivity (Jones & Paulhus, 2014). In contrast, Machiavellians are rather obsessed with gaining power, while narcissists display self-grandiosity and self-enhancement (Ok et al., 2021; Paulhus & Williams, 2002). Accordingly, the results of Study 2 showed not only significant correlations between the dark triad traits, but also revealed that each of these traits have unique aspects as only psychopathy was significantly predicting antihierarchical aggression.
It is further worth remembering that in Study 1, we used a different measure for narcissism ā the Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory (FFNI; Sherman et al., 2015). The FFNI measures narcissism on three subdimensions (Miller et al., 2016): antagonism, agentic extraversion, and neuroticism. The results of Study 1 revealed that the LWA subfacet of antihierarchical aggression was significantly predicted only by antagonistic narcissism. Antagonistic narcissists are individuals exploiting others for their own interests, lacking empathy, and having a sense of entitlement. Also, they are arrogant and manipulative, demonstrating reactive anger and distrust in others while seeking conflict and thrill. In contrast, the FFNI dimension of agentic extraversion is more representative of narcissistic behaviors like acclaim seeking, authoritativeness, grandiose fantasies, and exhibitionism. Thus, antagonistic narcissism (as measured with the FFNI) seems to represent a blend of narcissistic and psychopathic attributes, while agentic extraversion (measured with the FFNI) seems to be rather representative of the narcissism trait measured with the Short Dark Triad (SD3; Jones & Paulhus, 2014). Taking all this into account, the results of Study 2 replicate the results of Study 1, showing that individuals who strongly endorse antihierarchical aggression to overthrow those in power are narcissistic individuals with psychopathic attributes and thus driven by ego-focused motives.
General discussion
In two pre-registered studies, we investigated the relationship of LWA with the ego-focused trait of narcissism. Based on existing research (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2021), we expected individuals with higher levels of LWA to report higher levels of narcissism. The results of both studies are in line with this prediction: In particular, the results of Study 1 showed that the LWA subfacet of anti-hierarchical aggression was significantly predicted by antagonistic narcissism above and beyond individualsā prosocial dispositions (i.e., altruism). While antihierarchical aggression represents the drive to use force to overthrow those in power and who endorse conservative values, antagonistic narcissism is characterized by exploitation of others, lack of empathy, a sense of entitlement, arrogance and manipulative behavior. Accordingly, the results of Study 1 show that a strong ideological view, according to which a violent revolution against existing societal structures is legitimate is rather endorsed by individuals with ego-focused motives. This interpretation is further supported by the results of Study 2 which showed that LWA antihierarchical aggression was predicted by psychopathy again above and beyond individualsā prosocial dispositions (i.e., social justice commitment). Unexpectedly, neither dispositional altruism (Study 1) nor social justice commitment (Study 2) was found to be related to antihierarchical aggression. Considering these results, we assume that some political activists on the left side of the political spectrum do not actually strive for social justice and the support of underprivileged groups or persons, but rather endorse or express violence for the satisfaction of their own ego-focused, sometimes even antisocial, needs.
As a new contribution to the literature on dark personality traits, we interpret the results of both studies as expressions of a phenomenon we term the dark-ego-vehicle principle. According to this principle, individuals with dark personalities ā such as high narcissistic and psychopathic traits ā are attracted to certain ideologies and forms of political activism. We assume that such individuals use ideologies and political activism as a vehicle to satisfy their own ego-focused needs instead of actually aiming at social justice and equality. For example, a highly narcissistic/psychopathic person may participate in a pro-BLM protest pretending to fight against racism while actually using such protesting activities to meet their own aggressive motives and thrills (e.g., via violent escalations during pro-BLM protests). Further, such individuals might be attracted to pro-BLM activism, because this form of activism can provide them with opportunities for positive self-presentation (e.g., virtue signaling).
Three ancillary points are worth mentioning. Firstly, the dark-ego-vehicle principle does not mean that activism per se was narcissistic/psychopathic. It rather says that some forms of political activism can be attractive for narcissist/psychopaths; however, people also get involved in political activism due to their altruistic motives (Fowler & Kam, 2007). Secondly, the dark-ego-vehicle principle means that involvement in (violent) political activism is not solely attributable to political orientation but rather to personality traits manifesting in individuals on the (radical) left and right of the political spectrum. In particular, we argue that the dark personality correlates of authoritarianism per se might be the driving forces behind the aggression and violence expressed during protests like the attack on the United States Capitol in Washington DC and the pro-BLM protests mentioned in the introduction of this paper. This argument is in line with previous research (Zeigler-Hill et al., 2021) which showed that antagonistic narcissism is not only a strong predictor of LWA but simultaneously predicts SDO ā a trait that is clearly related to RWA (Altemeyer, 1998). These results also show that some individuals with high levels of antagonistic narcissism may be motivated to endorse either right- or left-wing ideological attitudes depending on which of these attitudes seems to be more opportune to them given a specific situation. Thus, it is necessary to argue very carefully in each case for what reason a specific dark personality should be attracted to particular ideologies/political activism.
Finally, the present research is not based on an elaborated explanatory theory (cf., Sandberg & Alvesson, 2021) as there is a lack of such a theory. Thus, we refer to a principle and not to a theory. However, we consider the present research as a first step within the complex process of Theory Construction Methodology sensu Borsboom et al. (2021) as our research aimed at the identification of an empirical phenomenon to develop a prototheory.
Limitations and future research
The presents research has some limitations which need to be addressed. First, while the samples of both present studies are much more diverse (see supplementary Table S1) than typical psychological research samples (Sears, 1986), it was not perfectly representative. We applied Prolificās option to recruit a US national representative sample. However, some participants had to be excluded from the analyses (e.g., for failing the attention checks). By this, representativeness might have been slightly reduced.
Second, there may potential issues resulting from the fact that we collected our data using an online panel (e.g., Smith et al., 2016; Wood et al., 2017). In particular, our samples may also include āprofessional survey takersā who were primarily interested in completing our surveys to receive the monetary incentive. Thus, our samples may include some specific demographic groups (e.g., unemployed or underemployed individuals with higher than average education levels). Further, some of the participating individuals may have not given the required attention to the survey questions because they were speeding through the survey. To deal with the second issue of careless responding, we included respective attention checks in both surveys and excluded individuals who failed those checks from the data analyses. However, in future studies, further removals of participantsā data based on participantsā response speed and consistency should be considered as suggested by Wood et al. (2017).
Third, the present findings are based on self-report measures. The usefulness of self-report measures has been criticized in the past (Baumeister et al., 2007). However, very recent empirical findings (Saunders et al., 2022) indicate self-report data can be very useful. Still, we acknowledge that self-reports are distal measures of actual behaviors and might be biased by participantsā tendencies to answer in a socially desirable way (Stƶber et al., 2002). To tackle the later restriction, in the present research we included well-established measures of socially desirable responding (Study 1) and virtue signaling (Study 2).
Fourth, the self-reports used in the present research may also be problematic as correlations could be artificially inflated due to common method variance which may arise particularly when the examined variables are assessed cross-sectionally (Chang et al., 2010) and/or due to semantic similarities of the items (Wood et al., 2022). However, the results of Harman single-factor tests as described in Zhang et al. (2022) suggested that common method bias did neither seriously affect the results of Study 1 or Study 2.
Fifth, future research should seek more empirical evidence for the dark-ego-vehicle principle. One particular area of interest, we plan to tackle in the near future is the relationship between the dark triad traits and political activism (e.g., in the context of contemporary prevalent feminist activism). Also, we argue that the dark-ego-vehicle principle holds independently of any political orientation. To test this assumption, future research should investigate the validity of the principle including individuals on both sides of political spectrum and involving social topics to which both sides have strong opinions (for example abortion rights or gun control laws). If the dark-ego-vehicle principle is valid, such research should find evidence for narcissistic individuals making of such topics to satisfy their own ego-focused needs independently of their (alleged) political views (i.e., some liberal narcissistic individuals should be found to be fighting for abortion rights or gun control laws while some conservative narcissistic individuals should be found to be fighting against abortion rights or gun control laws).
Lastly, future empirical research on the nomological network of dark triad personalities with regard to their āvehiclesā (i.e., ideologies and activism) is needed. Further, we think that a number of moderating variables are likely to play a crucial role as well, for example the overall prestige of the ideology/activism in a society, the distribution of supporters and opponents of this ideology/activism in the culture under study, and the likelihood of individually sufficient rewards for advocating pro and contra for this ideology/activism.
Conclusion
The results of our research significantly contribute to the research on LWA as empirical evidence regarding the correlates of LWA are still rare and controversial. With the present two studies, we provide empirical evidence for the relationship between LWA and dark personality traits as well as prosocial variables (i.e., altruism and social justice commitment). Also, with the dark-ego-vehicle principle, our research provides a possible explanation for the psychological mechanisms driving some individuals to participate in political activism independently of their political orientation.
Data Availability
Prior to the data collection, the hypothesis and analysis plan for the current studies were pre-registered at https://aspredicted.org (Study 1: https://aspredicted.org/qc4hy.pdf; Study 2: https://aspredicted.org/2sy8f.pdf).
Materials availability
The materials of the current studies and the full datasets generated for the current studies are available at https://researchbox.org/751 (Study 1) and https://researchbox.org/752 (Study 2).
Notes
In calculating the mean score for grandiose narcissism, we did include the items measuring Indifference (items 9, 24, 39, 54) in the original form (i.e., without recoding).
R for the regression model was statistically significantly different from zero, F(1, 375)ā=ā28.335, pā<ā.001. Altogether, 7.0% (6.8% adjusted) of the variability of participantsā mean symbolization scores was explained by participantsā mean internalization scores. Results showed that participantsā mean symbolization scores were statistically significantly predicted by their mean internalization scores, bā=ā0.439 (SEā=ā0.082), pā<ā.001.
References
Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. Harper.
Altemeyer, B. (1996). The authoritarian specter. Harvard University Press.
Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other āauthoritarian personality.ā. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (30 vol., pp. 47ā92). Academic Press.
Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2, 113ā133. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327582ijpr0202_5
Aquino, K., & Reed, I. I., A (2002). The self-importance of moral identity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1423ā1440. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1423
Armed Conflict Location & Event Data Project (ACLED) (May 2021). A year of racial justice protests: Key trends in demonstrations supporting the BLM movement. https://acleddata.com/2021/05/25/a-year-of-racial-justice-protests-key-trends-in-demonstrations-supporting-the-blm-movement/
Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Psychology as the science of self-reports and finger movements: whatever happened to actual behavior? Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 396ā403. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00051.x
Barry, D., McIntire, M., & Rosenberg, M. (2021, Jan 9). āOur president wants us hereā: The mob that stormed the Capitol. New York Times, A1.
Bertrams, A., & Schlegel, K. (2020). Speeded reasoning moderates the inverse relationship between autistic traits and emotion recognition. Autism, 24(8), 2304ā2309. https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361320937090
Borsboom, D., van der Maas, H. L. J., Dalege, J., Kievit, R. A., & Haig, B. D. (2021). Theory construction methodology: a practical framework for building theories in psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(4), 756ā766. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620969647
Chang, S. J., van Witteloostuijn, A., & Eden, L. (2010). From the editors: common method variance in international business research. Journal of International Business Studies, 41(2), 178ā184. https://doi.org/10.1057/jibs.2009.88
Chenoweth, E., & Pressman, J. (2020, Oct 16). This summerās Black Lives Matter protestors were overwhelmingly peaceful, our research finds. The Washington Post. https://www.radcliffe.harvard.edu/news-and-ideas/black-lives-matter-protesters-were-overwhelmingly-peaceful-our-research-finds
Cichocka, A., Dhont, K., & Makwana, A. P. (2017). On self-love and outgroup hate: opposite effects of narcissism on prejudice via social dominance orientation and right-wing authoritarianism. European Journal of Personality, 31(4), 366ā384. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.2114
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Conway, L. G., Houck, I. I. I., Gornick, S. C., L. J., & Repke, M. A. (2018). Finding the Loch Ness monster: left-wing authoritarianism in the United States. Political Psychology, 39(5), 1049ā1067. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12470
Costello, T. H., Bowes, S. M., Stevens, S. T., Waldman, I. W., Tasimi, A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2022). Clarifying the structure and nature of left-wing authoritarianism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 122(1), 135ā170. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000341
Cramer, P. (2011). Narcissism through the ages: what happens when narcissists grow older? Journal of Research in Personality, 45(5), 479ā492. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.06.003
Crawford, J. T., & Brandt, M. J. (2020). Ideological (a) symmetries in prejudice and intergroup bias. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 34, 40ā45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2019.11.007
Dargan, S., & Schermer, J. A. (2022). Predicting altruism with personality ābeyondā the Big five. Personality and Individual Differences, 185, 111258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2021.111258
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., & Lang, A. G. (2009). Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 1149ā1160. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149
Field, A. (2018). Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics (5th ed.). Sage Publications.
Fietzer, A. W., & Ponterotto, J. (2015). A psychometric review of instruments for social justice and advocacy attitudes. Journal for Social Action in Counseling and Psychology, 7(1), 19ā40. https://doi.org/10.33043/JSACP.7.1.19-40
Fowler, J. H., & Kam, C. D. (2007). Beyond the self: social identity, altruism, and political participation. The Journal of Politics, 69(3), 813ā827. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2508.2007.00577.x
Furnham, A., Richards, S. C., & Paulhus, D. L. (2013). The Dark Triad of personality: a 10 year review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 7(3), 199ā216. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12018
Gill, P., & Corner, E. (2017). There and back again: the study of mental disorder and terrorist involvement. American Psychologist, 72(3), 231ā241. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000090
Grijalva, E., Newman, D. A., Tay, L., Donnellan, M. B., Harms, P. D., Robins, R. W., & Yan, T. (2015). Gender differences in narcissism: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 141(2), 261ā310. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038231
Harms, P. D., Wood, D., Landay, K., Lester, P. B., & Lester, V., G (2018). Autocratic leaders and authoritarian followers revisited: a review and agenda for the future. The Leadership Quarterly, 29(1), 105ā122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.leaqua.2017.12.007
Hodson, G., Hogg, S. M., & MacInnis, C. C. (2009). The role of ādark personalitiesā (narcissism, Machiavellianism, psychopathy), big five personality factors, and ideology in explaining prejudice. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 686ā690. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.005
Hope, E. C., Keels, M., & Durkee, M. I. (2016). Participation in black lives matter and deferred action for childhood arrivals: modern activism among black and latino college students. Journal of Diversity in Higher Education, 9(3), 203ā215. https://doi.org/10.1037/dhe0000032
Janoff-Bulman, R., Sheikh, S., & Baldacci, K. G. (2008). Mapping moral motives: Approach, avoidance, and political orientation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1091ā1099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2007.11.003
Jones, D. N., & Paulhus, D. L. (2014). Introducing the short Dark Triad (SD3): a brief measure of dark personality traits. Assessment, 21(1), 28ā41. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191113514105
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339ā375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2006). Further assessment of the HEXACO personality inventory: two new facet scales and an observer report form. Psychological Assessment, 18(2), 182ā191. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.182
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D. R., McCain, J. L., Few, L. R., Crego, C., Widiger, T. A., & Campbell, W. K. (2016). Thinking structurally about narcissism: an examination of the five-factor narcissism inventory and its components. Journal of Personality Disorders, 30(1), 1ā18. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2015_29_177
Miller, M. J., Sendrowitz, K., Connacher, C., Blanco, S., de la PeƱa, C. M., Bernardi, S., & Morere, L. (2009). College studentsā social justice interest and commitment: a social-cognitive perspective. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 56(4), 495ā507. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017220
Nilsson, A., & Jost, J. T. (2020). The authoritarian-conservatism nexus. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 34, 148ā154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.03.003
Ok, E., Qian, Y., Strejcek, B., & Aquino, K. (2021). Signaling virtuous victimhood as indicators of dark triad personalities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(6), 1634ā1661. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000329
Paulhus, D. L. (1984). Two-component models of socially desirable responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(3), 598ā609. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.598
Paulhus, D. L. (1988). Assessing self-deception and impression management in self-reports: the Balanced Inventory of Desirable Responding. (Manual available from the author).
Paulhus, D. L., & Williams, K. M. (2002). The Dark Triad of personality: Narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy. Journal of Research in Personality, 36(6), 556ā563. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6
Polarization Research Lab (2022, December). Low support for political violence. https://polarizationresearchlab.org/2022/11/03/low-support-for-political-violence/
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance orientation: a personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 741ā763. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.4.741
Rushton, J. P., Chrisjohn, R. D., & Fekken, G. C. (1981). The altruistic personality and the self-report altruism scale. Personality and Individual Differences, 2, 293ā302. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(81)90084-2
Sandberg, J., & Alvesson, M. (2021). Meanings of theory: Clarifying theory through typification. Journal of Management Studies, 58(2), 487ā516. https://doi.org/10.1111/joms.12587
Saunders, B., Milyavskaya, M., & Inzlicht, M. (2022). Longitudinal evidence that event related potential measures of self-regulation do not predict everyday goal pursuit. Nature Communications, 13, 3201. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-30786-7
Schƶnbrodt, F. D., & Perugini, M. (2013). At what sample size do correlations stabilize? Journal of Research in Personality, 47(5), 609ā612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.05.009
Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the laboratory: influences of a narrow data base on social psychologyās view of human nature. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(3), 515ā530. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.3.515
Sherman, E. D., Miller, J. D., Few, L. R., Campbell, W. K., Widiger, T. A., Crego, C., & Lynam, D. R. (2015). Development of a short form of the five-factor narcissism inventory: the FFNI-SF. Psychological Assessment, 27(3), 1110ā1116. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0000100
Smith, S. M., Roster, C. A., Golden, L. L., & Albaum, G. S. (2016). A multi-group analysis of online survey respondent data quality: comparing a regular USA consumer panel to MTurk samples. Journal of Business Research, 69, 3139ā3148. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.busres.2015.12.002
Stƶber, J., Dette, D. E., & Musch, J. (2002). Comparing continuous and dichotomous scoring of the Balanced Inventory of Desirable responding. Journal of Personality Assessment, 78(2), 370ā389. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327752JPA7802_10
Wood, D., Harms, P. D., Lowman, G. H., & DeSimone, J. A. (2017). Response speed and response consistency as mutually validating indicators of data quality in online sample. Social Psychology and Personality Science, 8(4), 454ā464. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550617703168
Wood, D., Lowman, G. H., Armstrong, B. F. III, & Harms, P. D. (2022). Using retest-adjusted correlations as indicators of the semantic similarity of items. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000441
Zhang, Y., Hou, Z., Wu, S., Li, X., Hao, M., & Wu, X. (2022). The relationship between internet addiction and aggressive behavior among adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic: anxiety as a mediator. Acta Psychologica, 227, 103612. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2022.103612
Zeigler-Hill, V., Sauls, D., & Malay, P. (2021). Through the eyes of narcissus: competitive social worldviews mediate the associations that narcissism has with ideological attitudes. Self and Identity, 20(6), 811ā840. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2020.1779118
Zettler, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2010). Attitudes of the selfless: explaining political orientation with altruism. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, 338ā342. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.002
Funding
Open access funding provided by University of Bern
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
All authors contributed to the paper. All authors developed the study concept and design. The data was analyzed and interpreted by all authors. AK prepared the draft manuscript. AB provided critical revisions. All authors approve the final version to be published and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that they have no financial conflict of interest. AB declares that during the conducting and the publication process of the current studies, he was a member of the party FDP.The Liberals (FDP.Die Liberalen) Switzerland and the Network for Academic Freedom (Netzwerk Wissenschaftsfreiheit). No funds, grants, or other support was received.
Ethics Approval
The current studies were approved by the local ethics commission of the University of Bern (no. 2022-05-00006). All participants provided informed consent prior to participating.
Additional information
Publisherās Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Rights and permissions
Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the articleās Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the articleās Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Krispenz, A., Bertrams, A. Understanding left-wing authoritarianism: Relations to the dark personality traits, altruism, and social justice commitment. Curr Psychol (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04463-x
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-023-04463-x
Keywords
- Altruism
- Dark triad
- Left-wing authoritarianism
- Social justice commitment