From early 2020, humans around the world have been threatened to varying degrees by the emergence of a new Coronavirus (World Health Organization, 2020). The resulting fear and anxiety have subsequently shaped human thinking and behavior, both individually and collectively (Sobkow et al., 2020; da Silva Castanheira et al., 2021; Foad et al., 2021; Pazhoohi & Kingstone, 2021).

Initially, the pandemic was controlled by non-pharmacological interventions (NPIs) like masking or containments, while many researchers argued that NPIs had either little effect (Bendavid et al., 2021) or that policymakers were unable to weigh up costs and benefits of these NPIs properly (Lewis, 2022). One year later, from early 2021, the world has access to vaccines, but national coverage rates vary widely from country to country. One year after the launch of the Covid-19 vaccines, we can observe on the website Ourworldindata.org (January 31, 2022) that a country like Ethiopia plateaued at less than 2% of complete vaccination, Romania at 28%. The main industrialized nations (e.g., France, Germany, Israel, UK, USA) are in the middle of the table (around 55-75%) of this international ranking of vaccination rates, which is led by Chile and Portugal, which approach 90-100%.

Given that these vaccination campaigns address a health threat, individual differences such as threat sensitivity may help to account for variability in vaccination rates across countries. We examined the well-validated Neuroticism personality factor (McCrae & Costa, 1987; Lahey, 2009), which is the tendency to experience negative emotions, such as anxiety, worries and is associated with low tolerance for stress or aversive stimuli (McCrae & Costa, 1987). People who score high in neuroticism are more prone to understand everyday situations as threatening. Neuroticism, which is directly related to emotional reactivity and sensitivity to threat, appears to be of prime importance for the adoption of a strategy for dealing with a large-scale lethal threat. Neuroticism has been proposed as being important for public health (Lahey, 2009), and, some recent papers have shown that neuroticism positively predicts Covid-19 vaccine acceptance in the UK general population (Halstead et al., 2022) as well as the stability of intention (i.e., attitudes) toward recommended standard vaccinations in German patients with multiple sclerosis (Heidler et al., 2022). We hypothesized that this factor represents an excellent candidate to predict a significant part of variability in vaccination rates across countries.

Method

We selected the Neuroticism levels across 56 countries (Schmitt et al., 2007) and three of the main vaccination indicators (per 100 inhabitants) available in ourworldindata.org (OWID) as of 31 January 2022 (See Table 1): people vaccinated per hundred (at least one vaccine dose); people fully vaccinated per hundred (all prescribed doses); total boosters per hundred (additional doses). OWID is a centralized inventory of official national data published on each government platform. More information can be found here https://ourworldindata.org/coronavirus-source-data. We also entered other factors that have been shown to predict Covid-19 vaccination coverage, namely gross domestic product per capita, the median age of a country’s citizens and the rate of obesity (Body Mass Index, BMI rates >30), population size and population density (Basak et al., 2022; Oshakbayev et al., 2022). We hypothesized that neuroticism plays a significant positive role in predicting cross-country variability in vaccination rates, beyond the effects of the other health predictors. The study complies with the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki and its later addenda and an informed consent was sought and obtained from participants when necessary.

Table 1 Data concerning the 56 countries involved in the analyses. Selected on January 31, 2022 from Ouwourldindata.org (OWID) and for Neuroticism from Schmitt et al. (2007)

Results

Correlations with Neuroticism were highly significant for two of the three indicators (at least one dose: r(56) = .395, p = .003; fully vaccinated: r(56) = .376, p = .003), while the association with boosters remained marginal, r(56) = .238, p = .08. Regarding health predictors, the three vaccination rates were indeed significantly associated with variables such as gross domestic product per capita (.626 < r < .677, p < .001), the median age of the country’s citizens (.592 < r < .611, p < .001), and the rate of obesity (.278 < r < .342, p < .05). Population size or density did not correlate with vaccination rates. Importantly, partial correlations between neuroticism and vaccination rates remained significant after controlling for gross domestic product, median age, as well as for obesity rates (BMI >30) (.378 < r < .384, p < .01).

Stepwise regression analyses (Table 2) were then performed by first entering the gross domestic product, then the median age and the obesity rate (BMI >30) in a second step, and in the final step the average neuroticism level per country. The results of these regressions showed that the primary predictor of vaccination in a country was the country’s gross domestic product for all three vaccine indicators. Country gross domestic product alone represented between 41% and 53% of the cross-country variability in vaccination rates (R2 change). Neuroticism was the only other significant predictor for vaccination rate (at least one dose) and represented an additional 9% (R2 change) of the cross-country variance. The richer the country and the higher its average neuroticism score, the higher its rate of vaccination (at least one dose). In addition to gross domestic product, for the fully vaccinated rate, median age significantly predicts 5% of the additional variance while neuroticism adds significantly another 6% to the final model. The booster rate was only significantly predicted by gross domestic product, with neuroticism as well as any other predictors having no significant influence.

Table 2 Third step of the hierarchical stepwise regressions including remaining significant predictors of vaccination rates across 56 countries

Discussion

These findings highlight that neuroticism significantly predicts variations in country vaccination rates in different continents. These findings remained significant even after controlling for gross domestic product or physical health/risk factors such as age or obesity known to impact vaccine distribution (Basak et al., 2022; Oshakbayev et al., 2022). These results can be related at a more local level to data showing that neuroticism predicted negative affect level and variability during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany as well as with neuroticism’s associations with mean levels of attention to COVID-19 related information and worry (crisis preoccupation) (Kroencke et al., 2020). If people scoring high on neuroticism paid more attention to COVID-19 related information and worried more about one’s own health during the pandemic, it may explain neuroticism’s role in adhering to a vaccines’ national strategy. Our results also relate to UK findings showing that lower levels of neuroticism are associated with increased COVID-19 vaccination hesitancy (Halstead et al., 2022).

However, our data, in combination with the above-mentioned results, may seem to contradict other data from Russia showing that neuroticism is positively associated with vaccine hesitancy or resistance (Roshchina et al., 2022). It should be noted that most of the above-mentioned literature is related to vaccine intention (or attitude) when vaccination campaigns had not yet begun. This differs notably from the present data, which relates to actual vaccination rates rather than to anticipated behavior (intentions). The contradiction could, for example, be related to trust in vaccines or in the government, given that a recent study showed that a significant portion (around 30%) of those who had been vaccinated had nevertheless a low level of confidence in the Covid-19 vaccines (Gbenonsi et al., 2022).

From this perspective, it is interesting to note that in Roshchina et al. (2022), a high level of trust in institutions was associated with a stronger intention to be vaccined. Confidence in the solution (i.e., vaccines) together with fear of Covid-19 may appear as key factors to account for the apparent contradiction. The “appeal to fear” model (Witte, 1992) emphasizes that for a promoted behavior to be considered, one must feel at risk (susceptibility), and one must be confident that the solution (or response) is effective, easy to implement, and cost-effective. The appeal to fear model predicts that if risk susceptibility is high but confidence in the solution is low, there might be denial of the threat and rejection of the solution. In this sense, it is not surprising to observe in Roshchina et al. (2022) that vaccine resistance and hesitancy were greater in the regions with worse epidemiologic situations, where fear is potentially stronger because of threat proximity. This proposed explanation fits quite well with data showing that anxiety during covid Covid-19 is negatively related to confidence, controllability and physical distance to the nearest Covid-19 covid patients (Wu et al., 2021). In this way, it might be suggested that when there is a threat and confidence in the solution is high, neuroticism could increase acceptance, whereas if confidence is low, neuroticism could increase hesitancy.

Future research could aim to better understand how neuroticism not only influences vaccine intention but also the transformation of these intentions into actual behaviors. One way might be to use the photovoice method, which allows researchers to understand facilitators and barriers from the unique perspective of each participant, to understand the contextual conditions of participants, and to increase social justice by adequately informing mental health service providers and policy makers (Tanhan & Strack, 2020).