Skip to main content
Log in

Spontaneous and instructed emotion regulation in dysphoria: Effects on emotion experience and use of other emotion regulation strategies

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Compared to healthy individuals, individuals with depressive symptoms are likely to respond to negative events by overusing maladaptive emotion regulation (ER) strategies, which are associated with greater negative emotional intensity and intolerance. Research suggests that during negative event recall, self-distancing, a putatively adaptive ER strategy, is associated with decreases in maladaptive ER strategies (e.g., rumination), negative affect, and emotional intensity. However, less is known about the effects of self-distancing as a sad evocative event unfolds. Given that the experience of frequent and prolonged periods of sad emotion is characteristic of depressive syndromes, identifying a useful strategy to regulate emotion during sad events could help de-intensify and shorten those periods, which may ultimately reduce depression vulnerability. The present study examined spontaneous ER, instructed self-distancing, and their effects on emotion intensity and intolerance in dysphoric and nondysphoric individuals. Participants viewed sadness-inducing film clips, regulating emotions without specific instructions in one condition, and engaging in instructed self-distancing in the other. After each film, participants reported the intensity and intolerance of their emotions, and ER strategy use. The dysphoric group reported significantly greater intensity and intolerance of emotion than the nondysphoric group. Instructed self-distancing reduced emotional intensity more than spontaneous ER irrespective of group status, however it decreased rumination use only for the nondysphoric group. Decreased rumination use was associated with decreased emotion intensity and intolerance. Thus, promoting and monitoring the effectiveness of self-distancing may enhance ER. Pairing self-distancing with 2 other evidence-supported anti-rumination strategies may be needed to help reduce depression vulnerability more robustly.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

Available upon request.

Notes

  1. Despite its well-known utility, greater α coefficients reflect more narrowed, overlapping content among measure items (Loevinger, 1954; McCrae et al., 2011). Thus, it is quite possible for a three-item scale assessing a more broad construct to have a relatively low α coefficient but good construct validity (i.e., it efficiently captures important but only modestly-overlapping aspects of the construct). Further, principal components analysis can provide support for a construct operationalization’s unidimensionality, which is essential in evaluating reliability (Tavakol & Dennick, 2011). Thus based on the rumination scale’s low internal consistency at time 1, a principal components analysis, using varimax rotation, was conducted on time 1 SERQ data for full recruited sample. From this analysis a component (eigen value = 1.64, accounting for 13.63% of the variance) emerged with strong loadings (672, .690, and .737) for the three rumination items. No other items loaded above .222 on this component. Additionally, test-retest reliability for the rumination scale was as robust (r = .47) considering that the measure was administered before and after an experimental manipulation expected to influence T2 SERQ scale responding. Thus, PCA and test-retest findings provide some support for the operationalized composition of the construct within the measure despite its relatively low internal consistency value.

  2. It is possible that participants’ awareness that they would experience distress from viewing the first film clip induced anticipatory anxiety and uncomfortable uncertainty at baseline, leading to a ceiling effect of NA whereby film 1 did not increase NA significantly further. In partial support of this possibility, follow up analysis showed that level of NA prior to film 1 was marginally higher than prior to film 2 (F(1, 72) = 3.43, p = .07), which elicited a significant increase in NA. Moreover, the significant decrease in positive affect from film 1 is consistent with induction of depressed mood (Clark & Watson, 1991). Thus, as planned, film 1 promoted relevant affects for participants to regulate.

References

Download references

Code Availability

Not applicable.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jessica Balderas.

Ethics declarations

Ethics Approval

The methodology used in this study was approved by the University of Houston Clear-Lake Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects.

Consent to Participate

Informed consent was obtained from all individuals participating in this study.

Conflicts of Interest/Competing Interests

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Balderas, J., Bistricky, S.L., Schield, S. et al. Spontaneous and instructed emotion regulation in dysphoria: Effects on emotion experience and use of other emotion regulation strategies. Curr Psychol 42, 6257–6270 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01924-z

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01924-z

Keywords

Navigation