Skip to main content
Log in

The creepy, the bad and the ugly: exploring perceptions of moral character and social desirability in uncanny faces

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We used implicit and explicit measures to study whether “real” uncanny faces (by faces of Botox users and very ugly people) will be associated with perceptions of bad moral character and social avoidance. Implicit measures showed that uncanny faces were more strongly associated with negative aesthetic evaluations (“ugliness”) than with negative moral evaluation (“moral disgust”). At the explicit level, participants preferred greater social distance from uncanny faces than from neutral faces and inferred that they shared fewer moral values with uncanny faces than with neutral faces. Contrary to our hypotheses, only Ugly faces (but not Botox faces) were perceived as more likely to commit behaviors that indicate bad moral character. However, when this analysis was restricted to “sick” immoral actions, Botox faces were perceived as more likely to be engage in these kind of behaviors than neutral faces. Although exploratory in nature, this investigation suggest that ugliness (more than creepiness) may be the crucial evaluative dimension underlying rapid moral inferences from faces.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Notes

  1. In the words of Blanton and colleagues (2014, p.3): “Although no explicit rationale was ever published, cut-values centered around the D values of 0.15 (“slight preference”), 0.35 (“moderate preference”), and 0.64 (“strong preference”) were adopted. These same cut-points are used in the other IAT measures used to assess psychological states (Anthony Greenwald, personal communication, August 2002).”

  2. In particular, only Face #2 (Ugly category) showed middle values in the creepiness dimension (Appendix A).

References

  • Bartneck, C., Kulić, D., Croft, E., & Zoghbi, S. (2009). Measurement instruments for the anthropomorphism, animacy, likeability, perceived intelligence, and perceived safety of robots. International Journal of Social Robotics, 1(1), 71–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, D. V., Mortensen, C. R., Ackerman, J. M., Shapiro, J. R., Anderson, U. S., Sasaki, T., et al. (2011). Signal detection on the battlefield: Priming self-protection vs. revenge-mindedness differentially modulates the detection of enemies and allies. PloS One, 6(9), e23929.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., & Burrows, C. N. (2015). Implications of the implicit association test D-transformation for psychological assessment. Assessment, 22(4), 429–440. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073191114551382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bliss-Moreau, E., Barrett, L. F., & Wright, C. I. (2008). Individual differences in learning the affective value of others under minimal conditions. Emotion, 8(4), 479–493.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bluemke, M., & Friese, M. (2008). Reliability and validity of the single-target IAT (ST-IAT): Assessing automatic affect towards multiple attitude objects. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 977–997. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bocian, K., Baryła, W., Kulesza, W., Schnall, S., & Wojciszke, B. (2018). The mere liking effect: Attitudinal influences on attributions of moral character. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 79(2018), 9–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2018.06.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogardus, E. S. (1933). A social distance scale. Sociology and Social Research, 17, 265–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bohner, G., Siebler, F., González, R., Haye, A., & Schmidt, E. A. (2008). Situational flexibility of in-group-related attitudes: A single category IAT study of people with dual national identity. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 11, 301–317. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430208090644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buchner, A., Bell, R., Mehl, B., & Musch, J. (2009). No enhanced recognition memory, but better source memory for faces of cheaters. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(3), 212–224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.01.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burleigh, T. J., Schoenherr, J. R., & Lacroix, G. L. (2013). Does the uncanny valley exist? An empirical test of the relationship between eeriness and the human likeness of digitally created faces. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 759–771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chattopadhyay, D., & MacDorman, K. F. (2016). Familiar faces rendered strange: Why inconsistent realism drives characters into the uncanny valley. Journal of Vision, 16(11) 7, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 61–149. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(07)00002-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24(3), 285–290.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Ashmore, R. D., Makhijani, M. G., & Longo, L. C. (1991). What is beautiful is good, but…: A meta-analytic review of research on the physical attractiveness stereotype. Psychological Bulletin, 110(1), 109–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrari, C., Nadal, M., Schiavi, S., Vecchi, T., Cela-Conde, C. J., & Cattaneo, Z. (2017). The dorsomedial prefrontal cortex mediates the interaction between moral and aesthetic valuation: a TMS study on the beauty-is-good stereotype. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(5), 707–717.

  • Ferrari, C., Oh, D. W., Labbree, B., & Todorov, A. (2020). Learning the affective value of people: More than affect-based mechanisms. Actha Psychologica, 203, 103011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fetchenhauer, D., Groothuis, T., & Pradel, J. (2010). Not only states but traits—humans can identify permanent altruistic dispositions in 20s. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(2), 80–86. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.06.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chapman, H. A. (2017). Beyond purity: Moral disgust toward bad character. Psychological Science, 28(1), 80–91. https://doi.org/10.1177/095679761667319.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Moral character in person perception. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 38–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414550709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodwin, G. P., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2014). Moral character predominates in person perception and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 148–168. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034726.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, A. M., & Langlois, J. H. (2006). Stereotype directionality and attractiveness stereotyping: Is beauty good or is ugly bad? Social Cognition, 24, 212–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hartung, F., Jamrozik, A., Rosen, M. E., Aguirre, G., Sarwer, D. B., & Chatterjee, A. (2019). Behavioural and neural responses to facial disfigurement. Scientific Reports, 9, 8021. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-019-44408-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hassin, R., & Trope, Y. (2000). Facing faces: Studies on the cognitive aspects of physiognomy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 837–852.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C.-C., & MacDorman, K. F. (2010). Revisiting the uncanny valley theory: Developing and validating an alternative to the Godspeed indices. Computers in Human Behavior, 26(6), 1508–1518. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.05.015.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial expressions: Evidence from developmental cross-cultural research. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 288–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jamrozik, A., Oraa Ali, M., Sarwer, D. B., & Chatterjee, A. (2019). More than skin deep: Judgments of individuals with facial disfigurement. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 13(1), 117–129. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karpinski, A., & Steinman, R. B. (2006). The single category implicit association test as a measure of implicit social cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(1), 16–32. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.1.16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ma, D. S., Correll, J., & Wittenbrink, B. (2015). The Chicago face database: A free stimulus set of faces and norming data. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 1122–1135. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0532-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maestripieri, D., Henry, A., & Nickels, N. (2017). Explaining financial and prosocial biases in favor of attractive people: Interdisciplinary perspectives from economics, social psychology, and evolutionary psychology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, E19. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X16000340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 314–324. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAndrew, F. T., & Koehnke, S. S. (2016). On the nature of creepiness. New Ideas in Psychology, 43, 10–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mori, M. (1970/2005). The uncanny valley. (K. F. MacDorman, & T. Minato, trans.). Energy, 7, 33–35.

  • Nesse, R. M. (2005). Natural selection and the regulation of defenses: A signal detection analysis of the smoke detector principle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(1), 88–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Understanding and using the implicit association test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(2), 166–180. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271418.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olivera-La Rosa, A. (2018). Wrong outside, wrong inside: A social functionalist approach to the uncanny feeling. New Ideas in Psychology, 50, 38–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olivera-La Rosa, A., Arango-Tobón, O. E., & Ingram, G. P. (2019). Swiping right: Face perception in the age of tinder. Heliyon, 5(12), e02949. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2019.e02949.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Olivola, & Todorov. (2017). The biasing effects of appearances go beyond physical attractiveness and mating motives. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e38.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(32), 11087–11092.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pollick, F. E. (2010). In search of the uncanny valley. Lecture Note Institute Computer Sciences Telecommunications, 40, 69–78. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-12630-7_8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richetin, J., Costantini, G., Perugini, M., & Schönbrodt, F. (2015). Should we stop looking for a better scoring algorithm for handling Implicit Association Test data? Test of the role of errors, extreme latencies treatment, scoring formula, and practice trials on reliability and validity. PLoS ONE, 10(6), e0129601. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129601.

  • Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (2008). Disgust. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp. 757–776). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simpson, E. A., Varga, K., Frick, J. E., & Fragaszy, D. (2011). Infants experience perceptual narrowing for nonprimate faces. Infancy, 16(3), 318–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. (2014). Of ‘near pollution’and non-linear cultural effects: Reflections on Masahiro Mori and the Uncanny Valley. American Journal of Cultural Sociology, 2(3), 329–347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart, L. H., Ajina, S., Getov, S., Bahrami, B., Todorov, A., & Rees, G. (2012). Unconscious evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(4), 715–727. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027950.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Strohminger, N., & Nichols, S. (2014). The essential moral self. Cognition, 131(1), 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.12.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Szczurek, L., Monin, B., & Gross, J. J. (2012). The stranger effect: The rejection of affective deviants. Psychological Science, 23(10), 1105–1111. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612445314.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, K. M., Drevets, W. C., Whalen, P. J., Eccard, C. H., Dahl, R. E., Ryan, N. D., & Casey, B. J. (2001). Amygdala response to facial expressions in children and adults. Biological Psychiatry, 49(4), 309–316.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tinwell, A., Nabi, D. A., & Charlton, J. P. (2013). Perception of psychopathy and the Uncanny Valley in virtual characters. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(4), 1617–1625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Todorov, A., & Olson, I. R. (2008). Robust learning of affective trait associations with faces when the hippocampus is damaged, but not when the amygdala and temporal pole are damaged. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(3), 195–203. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsn013.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Tsukiura, T., & Cabeza, R. (2011). Shared brain activity for aesthetic and moral judgments: Implications for the beauty-is-good stereotype. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(1), 138–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Uhlmann, E. L., Pizarro, D. A., & Diermeier, D. (2015). A person-centered approach to moral judgment. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(1), 72–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691614556679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Verplaetse, J., Vanneste, S., & Braeckman, J. (2007). You can judge a book by its cover: The sequel. A kernel of truth in predictive cheating detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(4), 260–271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.04.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Villacampa, J., Ingram, G. P., Corradi, G., & Olivera-La Rosa, A. (2019). Applying an implicit approach to research on the uncanny feeling. Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis, 16(1), 11–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S., & Rochat, P. (2017). Human perception of animacy in light of the uncanny valley phenomenon. Perception, 46(12), 1386–1411.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S., Lilienfeld, S. O., & Rochat, P. (2015). The uncanny valley: Existence and explanations. Review of General Psychology, 19(4), 393–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watt, M. C., Maitland, R. A., & Gallagher, C. E. (2017). A case of the “heeby jeebies”: An examination of intuitive judgements of “creepiness”. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comportement, 49(1), 58–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yamada, Y., Kawabe, T., & Ihaya, K. (2013). Categorization difficulty is associated with negative evaluation in the “uncanny valley” phenomenon. Japanese Psychological Research, 55, 20–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaidel, D. W., & Nadal, M. (2011). Brain intersections of aesthetics and morals: Perspectives from biology, neuroscience, and evolution. Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, 54(3), 367–380.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Złotowski, J. A., Sumioka, H., Nishio, S., Glas, D. F., Bartneck, C., & Ishiguro, H. (2015). Persistence of the uncanny valley: The influence of repeated interactions and a robot’s attitude on its perception. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 883. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00883.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Evelyn Gil, Diana Estrada, Laura Quinchía and Joan Urriago for help with the experimental procedure.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Antonio Olivera-La Rosa.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in the studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amenmends or comprable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

All the authors declare no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Appendices

Target items (Botox faces, Ugly faces and Neutral faces)

Participants rated all faces by using two 7-point Likert scales: the aesthetic scale (from 1: “very ugly” to 7: “very handsome”) and the creepiness scale (from 1: “very creepy” to 7: “not creepy at all”).

BOTOX FACES

Botox Face 1: 2.75 (Ugliness); 2.82 (Creepiness).

Botox face 2: 2.8 (Ugliness); 2.87 (Creepiness).

Botox face 3: 2.36 (Ugliness); 2.53 (Creepiness).

UGLY FACES

Ugly face 1: 1.73 (Ugliness); 2.82 (Creepiness).

Ugly face 2: 2.23 (Ugliness); 3.64 (Creepiness).

Ugly face 3: 2.22 (Ugliness); 2.96 (Creepiness).

NEUTRAL FACES

Neutral faces were selected from The Chicago Face Database (Ma et al., 2015) under the criteria that they showed middle ratings in the aesthetic dimension and high ratings in the creepiness dimension (i.e., “not creepy at all”).

Neutral face 1: 4.86 (Ugliness); 6.08 (Creepiness).

Neutral face 2: 4.12 (Ugliness); 5.95 (Creepiness).

Neutral face 3: 4.61 (Ugliness); 5.93 (Creepiness).

Appendix B

Table 2 Attribute items

Moral character measure: predictions of the target face’s behaviors

Six items:

¿ Cuál de las siguientes nueve caras te parece más probable que … *Cocine y se coma a su propio perro, el cual murió por causas naturales. *Tenga relaciones sexuales con una gallina muerta.

*Se niegue a pagarle al tratamiento contra el cáncer a su madre. *Se niegue a cederle el asiento a una anciana con discapacidad. *Sea aficionado a los videos de pornografía infantil. *Decida llevarse una billetera que encontró perdida en la mesa de un restaurante.

Table 3 D-Measures for each ST-IAT

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Rosa, A.OL., Villacampa, J., Corradi, G. et al. The creepy, the bad and the ugly: exploring perceptions of moral character and social desirability in uncanny faces. Curr Psychol 42, 1146–1156 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01452-w

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01452-w

Keywords

Navigation