Skip to main content
Log in

Exploring preferences for present- and future-focused job opportunities across seniors and young adults

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Given the rapid increase of the senior population around the world, the current research documents how the concept of time influences the job preferences of older and younger adults in an organizational behavior context. Study 1 reported that older adults devoted significantly more attention towards the present compared to the past and future, while younger adults showed a small preference towards the future compared to the past and present. Mediation analyses revealed that older adults’ reduced sense of time remaining in life led to them devoting less time to the future. A novel finding from the current work was that sense of time remaining in life also significantly suppressed the relationship between age and present temporal focus. That is, an increased sense of time remaining in life was actually associated with enhanced focus on the present among seniors. Study 2 investigated the downstream consequences of temporal focus by examining older and younger adults’ preferences for present- and future-focused job opportunities. Findings revealed that older adults showed a distinct preference for present-focused job opportunities over future-focused ones, whereas younger individuals reported the opposite pattern. These findings highlight how the concept of time influences individuals of varying ages in unique ways, leading to important differences in both cognition and behavior. The practical implications of these findings across various domains such as the workplace, leadership, and team dynamics are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Notes

  1. Because data collection occurred in different locations with varying time constraints, not all of the participants were able to complete both questionnaires outlined in the measures section below. Specifically, though all 353 participants completed the temporal focus questionnaire, only 178 participants completed the additional questionnaire on sense of time remaining in life. Given our goal of examining mediation with an expected large-sized a-path (from age to sense of time remaining; see Lang & Carstensen, 2002), and anticipated medium-sized b-path (from sense of time remaining to each temporal focus DV), we were significantly above the n = 54 recommended for 80% power.

  2. To demonstrate that the effects on present and future time focus were independent of one another, we re-analyzed each mediation again, entering the other variable as a covariate. The mediation effect of age to sense of time remaining to future time focus remained significant, IE = −.60 [−.92, −.27], controlling for present time focus. The mediation effect of age to sense of time remaining to present time focus remained significant, IE = −.61 [−.88, −.39], controlling for future time focus.

    Furthermore, an anonymous reviewer suggested that only two “sense of time remaining” items had unambiguous construct validity (items #4 and #5). When a composite of just those two items were substituted for the full scale, all mediation paths remained significant in the same direction, and both the future temporal focus mediation, IE = −.69 [−1.04, −.35] and the present temporal focus mediation, IE = −.81 [−1.14, −.53], revealed significant indirect effects.

  3. Due to a clerical error during data collection, the younger adults sample was given a 1 to 7 Likert scale (1 = Not likely at all, 7 = very likely) instead of a 1 to 9 scale. All other facets of the scenario and question were the same.

References

  • Adams, G. A., & Rau, B. L. (2004). Job seeking among retirees seeking bridge employment. Personnel Psychology, 57, 719–744. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1744-6570.2004.00005.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bouisson, J., & Swendsen, J. (2003). Routinization and emotional well-being: An experience sampling investigation in an elderly French sample. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 58(5), P280–P282.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burke, W. W. (2002). Organization development: A process of learning and changing. Reading. UK: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bursch, D., & Kelly, K. (2014). Managing the multigenerational workplace. Retrieved from http://www.kenanflagler.unc.edu/~/media/Files/documents/executivedevelopment/managingthe-multigenerational-workplace-white-paper.pdf

  • Carstensen, L. L. (1992). Social and emotional patterns in adulthood: Support for socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology and Aging, 7(3), 331–338. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.7.3.331.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cepeda, L. M., & Davenport, D. S. (2006). Person-centered therapy and solution-focused brief therapy: An integration of present and future awareness. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 43(1), 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charles, S. T., Reynolds, C. A., & Gatz, M. (2001). Age-related differences and change in positive and negative affect over 23 years. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 136–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chawla, R. K. (1990). Dependency ratios: An international comparison. Perspectives on Labour and Income (Statistics Canada, Catalogue 75–001), 2(2), 50–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, F. F. (2008). What happens if we compare chopsticks with forks? The impact of making inappropriate comparisons in cross-cultural research. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(5), 1005–1018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Berker, A. O., Rutledge, R. B., Mathys, C., Marshall, L., Cross, G. F., Dolan, R. J., & Bestmann, S. (2016). Computations of uncertainty mediate acute stress responses in humans. Nature Communications, 7, 10996.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Duchesne, D. (2002). Seniors at work. Perspectives on Labour and Income, 14(2), 33–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ebner, N. C., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Developmental changes in personal goal orientation from young to late adulthood: From striving for gains to maintenance and prevention of losses. Psychology and Aging, 21(4), 664–678.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. R. (1991). Person-job fit: A conceptual integration, literature review, and methodological critique. In C. L. Cooper & I. T. Robertson (Eds.), International Revie of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 283–357). New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eichar, D. M., Norland, S., Michael Brady, E., & Fortinsky, R. H. (1991). The job satisfaction of older workers. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 12(7), 609–620.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34(3), 169–189. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3403_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fabrigar, L. R., & Wegener, D. T. (2012). Exploratory factor analysis. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fingerman, K. L., & Perlmutter, M. (1995). Future time perspective and life events across adulthood. The Journal of General Psychology, 122(1), 95–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forteza, J. A., & Prieto, J. M. (1994). Aging and work behavior. In H. C. Triandis, M. D. Dunnette, & L. M. Hough (Eds.), Handbook of industrial organizational psychology (2nd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 447–483). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fung, H. H., & Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Sending memorable messages to the old: age differences in preferences and memory for advertisements. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 163–178.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fung, H. H., Carstensen, L. L., & Lutz, A. M. (1999). Influence of time on social preferences: Implications for life-span development. Psychology and Aging, 14(4), 595–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Garfein, A. J., & Herzog, A. R. (1995). Robust aging among the young-old, old-old, and oldest-old. The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 50(2), S77–S87. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/50B.2.S77.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gross, J. J., Carstensen, L. L., Pasupathi, M., Tsai, J., Götestam Skorpen, C., & Hsu, A. Y. (1997). Emotion and aging: Experience, expression, and control. Psychology and Aging, 12(4), 590–599.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grühn, D., Sharifian, N., & Chu, Q. (2016). The limits of a limited future time perspective in explaining age differences in emotional functioning. Psychology and Aging, 31(6), 583–593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harper, S. (2014). Economic and social implications of aging societies. Science, 346(6209), 587–591. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1254405.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, A, F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper]. Retrieved from http://afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf.

  • Holman, E. A., & Silver, R. C. (1998). Getting“stuck” in the past: Temporal orientation and coping with trauma. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1146–1163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Imtiaz, F., Ji, L. J., & Vaughan-Johnston, T. (2018). Exploring the influence of a low-dose mindfulness induction on performance and persistence in a challenging cognitive task. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 2(4), 107–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (2004). Aging, adult development, and work motivation. Academy of Management Review, 29(3), 440–458. https://doi.org/10.5465/amr.2004.13670969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Killingsworth, M. A., & Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330(6006), 932–932.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kooij, D. T., van Woerkom, M., Wilkenloh, J., Dorenbosch, L., & Denissen, J. J. (2017). Job crafting towards strengths and interests: The effects of a job crafting intervention on person–job fit and the role of age. Journal of Applied Psychology, 102(6), 97–981.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kristof-Brown, A. L., Zimmerman, R. D., & Johnson, E. C. (2005). Consequences of individuals fit at work: A meta-analysis of person–job, person-organization, person-group, and person-supervisor fit. Personnel Psychology, 58, 281–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lambert-Pandraud, R., Laurent, G., & Lapersonne, E. (2005). Repeat purchasing of new automobiles by older consumers: Empirical evidence and interpretations. Journal of Marketing, 69(2), 97–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (1994). Close emotional relationships in late life: Further support for proactive aging in the social domain. Psychology and Aging, 9(2), 315–324.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Future time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology and Aging, 17(1), 125–139.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Löckenhoff, C. E., De Fruyt, F., Terracciano, A., McCrae, R. R., De Bolle, M., Costa, P. T., & Yik, M. (2009). Perceptions of aging across 26 cultures and their culture-level associates. Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 941–954.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Loi, J. L. P., & Shultz, K. S. (2007). Why older adults seek employment: Differing motivations among subgroups. Journal of Applied Gerontology, 26(3), 274–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, R. L., & Farrington, P. A. (2006). Age-related differences in the motivation of knowledge workers. Engineering Management Journal, 18(3), 20–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/10429247.2006.11431700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loughlin, C., & Barling, J. (2001). Young workers' work values, attitudes, and behaviors. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 74(4), 543–558.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKinnon, D. P., Krull, J. L., & Lockwood, C. M. (2000). Equivalence of the mediation, confounding, and suppression effect. Prevention Science, 1, 173–181.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Muszyńska, M. M., & Rau, R. (2012). The old-age healthy dependency ratio in Europe. Journal of population ageing, 5(3), 151–162.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Nolan, L. S. (2015). The roar of Millennials: Retaining top talent in the workplace. Journal of Leadership, Accountability & Ethics, 12(5), 69–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • O'Connor, R. C., & Cassidy, C. (2007). Predicting hopelessness: The interaction between optimism/pessimism and specific future expectancies. Cognition and Emotion, 21(3), 596–613.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmore, E. (1981). Social patterns in normal aging: Findings from the Duke longitudinal study. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. (2000). The future of optimism. American Psychologist, 55(1), 44–55. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pînzaru, F., Vatamanescu, E. M., Mitan, A., Savulescu, R., Vitelar, A., Noaghea, C., & Balan, M. (2016). Millennials at work: Investigating the specificity of generation Y versus other generations. Management Dynamics in the Knowledge Economy, 4(2), 173–192.

    Google Scholar 

  • Read, D., & Read, N. L. (2004). Time discounting over the lifespan. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 94(1), 22–32. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.01.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rechel, B., Grundy, E., Robine, J. M., Cylus, J., Mackenbach, J. P., Knai, C., & McKee, M. (2013). Ageing in the European union. The Lancet, 381(9874), 1312–1322. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(12)62087-X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson-Whelen, S., Kim, C., MacCallum, R. C., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1997). Distinguishing optimism from pessimism in older adults: Is it more important to be optimistic or not to be pessimistic? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(6), 1345–1353. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.73.6.1345.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rudolph, C. W., Kooij, D. T., Rauvola, R. S., & Zacher, H. (2018). Occupational future time perspective: A meta-analysis of antecedents and outcomes. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 39(2), 229–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sargent-Cox, K., Butterworth, P., & Anstey, K. J. (2011). The global financial crisis and psychological health in a sample of Australian older adults: a longitudinal study. Social Science & Medicine, 73(7), 1105–1112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seli, P., Beaty, R. E., Cheyne, J. A., Smilek, D., Oakman, J., & Schacter, D. L. (2018). How pervasive is mind wandering, really? Consciousness and Cognition, 66, 74–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M, E, P., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive psychology: An introduction American Psychologist, 55, 5–14. doi:https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.1.5.

  • Shipp, A. J., Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2009). Conceptualization and measurement of temporal focus: The subjective experience of the past, present, and future. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110(1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smallwood, J., Nind, L., & O’Connor, R. C. (2009). When is your head at? An exploration of the factors associated with the temporal focus of the wandering mind. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(1), 118–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.11.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Canada. (2016). An aging population. Statistics Canada catalogue no. 11-402-X. Ottawa. Version updated July 2016. Ottawa. /pub/11-402-x/2010000/chap/pop/pop02-eng.Htm (accessed Dec, 2016).

  • Strough, J., Bruine de Bruin, W., Parker, A. M., Lemaster, P., Pichayayothin, N., & Delaney, R. (2016). Hour glass half full or half empty? Future time perspective and preoccupation with negative events across the life span. Psychology and Aging, 31(6), 558–573. https://doi.org/10.1037/pag0000097.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of the human mind. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 123(2), 133–167.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. A., Shultz, K. S., & Doverspike, D. (2005). Recruiting and retaining talented older workers. In P. T. Beatty & R. M. S. Visser (Eds.), Thriving on an aging workforce: Strategies for organizational and system change (pp. 43–50). Malabar, FL: Krieger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tishman, F. M., Van Looy, S., & Bruyère, S. M. (2012). Employer strategies for responding to an aging workforce. The NTAR Leadership Center: Rutgers, The State University of New Jersey.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tout, K. (1989). Aging in developing countries. New York, N.Y: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vincent, G. K., & Velkoff, V. A. (2010). The next four decades: The older population in the United States: 2010 To 2050 (no. 1138). US Department of Commerce. US Census Bureau: Economics and Statistics Administration.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York, NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warr, P., Butcher, V., Robertson, I., & Callinan, M. (2004). Older people's well-being as a function of employment, retirement, environmental characteristics and role preference. British Journal of Psychology, 95(3), 297–324. https://doi.org/10.1348/0007126041528095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C. (2004). The sandwich generation. Perspectives on Labour and Income, 16(4), 7–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zacher, H., & Frese, M. (2009). Remaining time and opportunities at work: Relationships between age, work characteristics, and occupational future time perspective. Psychology and Aging, 24, 487–493. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zacher, H., Pearce, L. K., Rooney, D., & McKenna, B. (2014). Leaders’ personal wisdom and leader–member exchange quality: The role of individualized consideration. Journal of Business Ethics, 121(2), 171–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Faizan Imtiaz.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

ESM 1

(DOCX 54 kb)

Appendix 1 Study 1 Measures

Appendix 1 Study 1 Measures

Temporal Focus Scale (TFS; Shipp et al., 2009)

  1. 1)

    I think about things from my past.

  2. 2)

    I live my life in the present.

  3. 3)

    I think about what my future has in store.

  4. 4)

    I focus on what is currently happening in my life.

  5. 5)

    I focus on my future.

  6. 6)

    I replay memories of the past in my mind.

  7. 7)

    I imagine what tomorrow will bring for me.

  8. 8)

    My mind is on the here and now.

  9. 9)

    I reflect on what has happened in my life.

  10. 10)

    I think about where I am today.

  11. 11)

    I think back to my earlier days.

  12. 12)

    I think about times to come.

Sense of Time Remaining in Life (Future Time Perspective Scale; Lang & Carstensen, 2002).

  1. 1)

    Many opportunities await me in the future.

  2. 2)

    I expect that I will set many new goals in the future.

  3. 3)

    My future is filled with possibilities.

  4. 4)

    Most of my life lies ahead of me.

  5. 5)

    My future seems infinite to me.

  6. 6)

    I could do anything I want in the future.

  7. 7)

    There is plenty of time left in my life to make new plans.

  8. 8)

    I have the sense time is running out.

  9. 9)

    There are only limited possibilities in my future.

  10. 10)

    As I get older, I begin to experience time as limited.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Imtiaz, F., Ji, LJ. & Vaughan-Johnston, T. Exploring preferences for present- and future-focused job opportunities across seniors and young adults. Curr Psychol 42, 470–485 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01365-8

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-021-01365-8

Keywords

Navigation