Introduction

Narcissism and narcissistic traits are Janus-faced qualities: on the one hand, they are associated with personal benefits to the narcissistic individual but, on the other, they may present a burden to other people, family and society (Twenge et al. 2008). Narcissism is originally a psychoanalytically-oriented construct (Ronningstan 2013); however, it has a robust impact on almost every field of psychology (Campbell and Miller 2012). With the development of empirically-based scientific methods and psychological measurements, the emphasis gradually transferred from clinical case studies to the trait-based, personality psychological approaches. However, several projective techniques have their own narcissism indices (Bandi et al. 2017) the most popular independent forms of the assessment of narcissism are self-report scales. Despite the common critiques related to the problem of social desirability in the assessment of maladaptive subclinical personality traits, numerous self-report narcissism tests have emerged (Emmons 1987).

According to the widely accepted approach, two major forms of narcissism can be differentiated: grandiose narcissism and vulnerable narcissism (Pincus and Lukowitsky 2010), and assessment of narcissism generally focuses on one or the other (Hendin and Cheek 1997; Cheek et al. 2013). Individuals with grandiose narcissistic traits are described as arrogant, exploitative and entitled, while their self-esteem regulation is mainly characterised by self-aggrandisement and devaluation of others (Zeigler-Hill et al. 2008). Vulnerable narcissistic people, in contrast, are self-inhibited and rely heavily on the approval of others (Cooper and Maxwell 1995; Zeigler-Hill et al. 2008), but they also have grandiose expectations of themselves and others (Kealy and Rasmussen 2012; Wink 1996).

Narcissism viewed as a set of traits however is limiting our understanding of the role of normal narcissistic personality functioning. It offers limited help in explaining why these exact traits form narcissism and what can be in the core of these different narcissistic tendencies. To deal with this gap recent models of narcissism are beginning to conceptualise narcissism as a dynamic self-regulatory process (Giacomin 2016) in which all narcissistic behaviours are serving the aim of maintaining a positive self-view (Morf and Rhodewalt 2001; for a review, see Giacomin 2016).

Several contemporary measures exist to assess both vulnerable and grandiose narcissistic traits together (eg. the Five-Factor Narcissism Inventory (FFNI; Glover et al. 2012), the Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI; Pincus et al. 2009), but the most widespread measure of narcissistic grandiosity is still the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin and Terry 1988; Cain et al. 2008). Although this scale is widely used, its internal measurement model is still not clear, and different factor structures have been proposed (Fig. 1). Raskin and Terry (1988) originally identified seven principal components: Authority, Exhibitionism, Superiority, Entitlement, Exploitativeness, Self-Sufficiency and Vanity. The large correlations among the primary factors implied a secondary general factor, though it was not tested (Raskin and Terry 1988). Corry et al. (2008) offered a two-factor solution with only Leadership/Authority and Exhibitionism/Entitlement factors; however, these factor structures could not be confirmed in independent confirmatory research (Ackerman et al. 2011). Ackerman et al. (2011) proposed a three-factor solution distinguishing the adaptive Leadership/Authority (L/A) factor from the maladaptive Grandiose Exhibitionism (GE) and Entitlement/Exploitativeness (E/E) factors. One of the greatest advantages of the results from Ackerman and colleagues’ work (2011) is the appearance of the possible social adaptational value of narcissistic tendencies, which is consonant with current interest around the construct of ‘healthy’ narcissism (Solan 2015). Nevertheless, the confirmation of this three-factor solution in independent samples is necessary to accept it as a measurement model.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Model 8

Previous research mainly tested models with primary factors, though to understand the possible uni- or multidimensionality of narcissistic personality inventory hierarchical models within a confirmatory factor analysis framework should be considered. If we view narcissism as a dynamic self-regulatory process, all narcissistic traits or behaviours should have a common underlying aim of maintaining a positive self-view, questioning the dimensionality of the constructs used. It is of increasing interest to apply both second-order and bifactor models in personality research (Reise 2012), in order to clarify the dimensionality questions of a given construct. In second-order models, the higher order factors are said to explain the correlation between the primary factors, while the bifactor model approach aims to differentiate the amount of variance explained by a single general factor and specific relevant factors. The other advantage of the bifactor approach is that specific and general factor’s relationship with external variables can be captured separately (Reise et al. 2010). Moreover, recent studies suggested that bifactor models can be applied to general psychopathology as well (the “p” factor) supporting a transdiagnostic approach (Caspi et al. 2014), and this general factor is stable over time, and describes temporal changes in psychopathology well (Gluschkoff, Jokela and Rosenström 2019).

As there was no clear agreement on the different factor structures, and higher order factor models were not or rarely tested previously, the first aim of our two studies was to compare the competing measurement models with a series of confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) to identify the best fitting solution to our data collected with both the Hungarian and English versions of the NPI. The second aim was to test the concurrent validity and associations of the best fitting model in two sets of confirmatory factor analysis models with covariates including self-esteem, other measures of vulnerable and grandiose narcissistic traits and well-being measures.

Grandiose narcissism and explicit self-esteem are constantly found to be positively correlated (Hyatt et al. 2018; Campbell et al. 2002; Kernis and Sun 1994), because both constructs capture positive perceptions of the self and are positively related to agentic traits, however the difference lies between their association to communal traits (Hyatt et al. 2018). Out of the subscales of NPI, Entitlement/Exploitativeness has the strongest negative associations with communal traits (Pryor et al. 2008), while Leadership/Authority and Grandiose Exhibitionism are positively associated with agentic traits (Gentile et al. 2013). Therefore, self-esteem seems to be an appropriate construct to test the construct validity of the NPI. Moreover, if we consider narcissism as a self-regulatory process for maintaining positive self-views we can expect a positive relationship between explicit self-esteem and grandiose narcissism in general.

Furthermore, there is a growing interest in differentiating the adaptive and maladaptive sides of narcissism (for a review see Cai and Luo 2018; Rose 2002); therefore, eudaimonic well-being, as in living a complete, authentic, self-directed life with satisfying relationships (Ryff and Keyes 1995), seems to be another useful construct with which to test this construct. Previous research showed that narcissism is positively related to greater eudaimonic and hedonic well-being especially when controlled for other dark triad traits (Aghababaie and Blachnio 2015) and this relationship is mediated through self-esteem (Zuckerman and O’Loughlin 2009); we thus expect positive relationships with both self-esteem and eudaimonic well-being.

Lastly, the differentiation between vulnerable and grandiose narcissistic traits also presents a useful opportunity to test discriminant validity, as previously no significant relationship was found between grandiose and vulnerable narcissism using the NPI (Miller et al. 2018; Cheek et al. 2013; Hendin and Cheek 1997). Only Entitlement/Exploitativeness subscale have shown a positive association with measures of vulnerable narcissism in previous studies (e.g. Crowe et al. 2018a and b; Krusemark et al. 2018).

Therefore, the aim of this paper is to enable the differentiation of the variance between the general narcissism factor and the specific factors offered by previous research - measured in the single-stimulus format of the NPI.

In Study 1 we tested the different measurement models and the association of those with covariates on a large sample (N = 791) using the Hungarian version of the NPI, while in Study 2 we tested the associations of the best-fitting model with other contemporary measures of vulnerable and grandiose narcissism with the English version of the NPI (N = 319). To assess measurement invariance between the Hungarian and English versions of the NPI, and to provide further evaluation of the bifactor model across different response formats we also conducted a third study (Study 3; N = 237).

Study 1

Methods

Participants and Procedure

We pooled three convenience samples in the current study. Participants in the first sample (Sample 1) were 226 university students (76.5% women, mean age = 21.17; SD = 2.57) from a large university in Budapest; for the second (Sample 2; n = 414) and third samples (Sample 3; n = 152) 566 university students (76.9% women, mean age = 27.29; SD = 10.95) were recruited from a university in Pécs. As more samples were pooled in Study 1, ethical approvals were granted in both universities for conducting data collection, to comply with research ethical guidelines (reference numbers 2017/65 in Pécs and 2018/229 in Budapest).

Participants were recruited through safe internal university portals, where students could register for different research studies in exchange for course credits. After registration participants were given detailed information about the aim and tasks related to the study and were asked for their consent. For those agreed to participate surveys were distributed through email messages which participants could fill out online.

Measures

Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry 1988)

The original NPI forced participants to choose from 40 pairs of statements, one being the non-narcissistic and the other being the narcissistic option. This response format, however, only offered the elimination of socially desirable responses if very similar statements were compared, which is not the case in the NPI (Wetzel et al. 2016). On the other hand, in another study response format was shown to effect the factor structure of the NPI, where a three factor solution was more adequate for the forced-choice format, while a five factor solution offered better fit for the single-stimulus response formats (Ackerman et al. 2016). In this study, we used single-stimulus dichotomous items as the response format of NPI.

The translation was carried out in accordance with the standard test-adaptation procedure. The items were translated into Hungarian by three members of the research group. This process was followed by cross-checking, to explore the source of any possible differences and to find the most suitable alternatives. We also applied back-translation by a linguist with advanced linguistic and psychological knowledge; this helped us to improve linguistic validity and avoid translational biases. The differences between the original and back-translated texts were reviewed and revised during the final refining of the items.

Explicit Self-Esteem

Explicit self-esteem was assessed using the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg 1965), a widely used 10-item instrument capturing global self-esteem. Participants respond to questions (e.g., ‘I am able to do things as well as most other people’) on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 – ‘strongly disagree’ to 3 – ‘strongly agree’. The measure consists of five reversed items. In the present study we have used the Hungarian translation of the measure (Urbán et al. 2014). This scale was administered only to Sample 1. The internal consistency of the scale was adequate (α = .90).

Eudaimonic Well-Being

To measure psychological well-being, we used the Ryff and Keyes well-being scale (1995). It measures Ryff’s original six factors of eudaimonic well-being (1989), with three questions for each factor: autonomy, self-acceptance, positive relations, environmental mastery, purpose in life and personal growth. The Hungarian version of the test has adequate psychometric properties (Oláh 2012), and also offers good internal consistency in our sample (α = .86). This scale was administered only to Sample 1.

Vulnerable Narcissism

Vulnerable narcissism was assessed using the Maladaptive Covert Narcissism Scale (Hendin and Cheek, 2013) which is the updated version of the Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale (HSNS, Hendin and Cheek 1997), developed previously. The measure consists of 23 questions, and internal consistency of the questionnaire was adequate in this study (α = .86). Preliminary results show that the Hungarian translation of the scale has adequate psychometric properties (Bandi 2014). This scale was administered only to Sample 2.

Statistical Analysis Plan

We applied confirmatory factor analysis with Mplus 8.0 (Muthén and Muthén 1998–2015) to estimate the degree of fit of previously proposed measurement models to the data. We tested four different measurement models that included one-factor, two-factor (Corry et al. 2008), three-factor (Ackerman et al. 2011) and seven-factor (Raskin and Terry 1988) models (for the models see the Electronic Supplementary Materials and Fig. 1). We also checked meaningful error covariances in each model. Due to the strong correlations between the first-order factors, we also tested the second-order factor models in which the secondary factor is specified to account for the covariation among multiple factors. We estimated the secondary factor model with three and seven first-order factors. In these cases second-order represents the narcissism trait. Furthermore, we also tested the bifactor models. A bifactor model represents a general narcissism dimension on which each item loaded; and specific factors on which only the specific items were loaded; and the correlations between specific factors were fixed to zero. The specification of a bifactor model requires that the specific factors correlate neither with each other nor with the general factor (Reise et al. 2010).

The advantage of bifactor modelling is the opportunity to quantify the degree of unidimensionality. Therefore, we applied the percentage of common variance attributable to the general factor through the use of the explained common variance (ECV; Bentler 2009; Ten Berge and Sočan 2004). We also used omega and omega hierarchical indices to measure how precisely a specific factor score assesses the combination of general and specific constructs and a certain target construct (Brunner et al. 2012).

Because of the binary responses to items, we used the weighted least squares mean and variance adjusted estimation method (WLSMV; Brown, 2006; Finney and DiStefano 2006). In confirmatory factor analysis, a satisfactory degree of fit requires the comparative fit index (CFI) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) to be higher than or close to 0.95, and the model should be rejected when these indices are less than 0.90 (Brown, 2006). The next fit index was the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA). RMSEA below 0.05 indicates excellent fit, a value around 0.08 indicates adequate fit, and a value above 0.10 indicates poor fit. Closeness of model fit using RMSEA (CFit of RMSEA) is a statistical test (Brown, 2006), which evaluates the statistical deviation of RMSEA from the 0.05–value. Non-significant probability values (p > 0.05) indicate acceptable model fit (Brown, 2006).

Furthermore, to test the concurrent validity of the best fitting models, we used CFA with covariates in two separate university student samples from two large universities in Hungary. The covariates in the first sample were gender, explicit self-esteem and psychological well-being and, in the second sample, gender and vulnerable narcissism.

Results

Testing Alternative Measurement Models of the NPI

We tested nine measurement models on the combined sample of the three subsamples, Sample 1, Sample 2 and Sample 3 (N = 791). The fit indices are reported in Table 1. According to current traditions, only Model 2, 3, 5, 7 and 8 provided acceptable fit indices. The bifactor model with three specific factors (Model 8) fitted best to the data (χ2 = 617.0, CFI = 0.971, TLI = 0.965, RMSEA = 0.044, CI [0.039–0.048]) compared to the competing solutions; however, the bifactor model with two specific factors (Model 7) produced nearly as good fit indices. Therefore, factor loadings are presented for both Model 8 (Table 2) and Model 7 (Appendix, Table 4). In both bifactor models with two and three specific factors (Model 7 and Model 8) all items loaded significantly on the general narcissism factor, and the factor loadings ranged from 0.20 to 0.66, and 0.28 to 0.67, respectively. Four items (Items 8, 11, 32 and 3) in Model 7 and two items in Model 8 did not load significantly on the specific factors Model 7, both rather representing a global narcissism factor.

Table 1 Competing measurement models of grandiose narcissism: confirmatory factor analysis
Table 2 Factor loadings, explained common variance, omega and omega hierarchical scores of the bifactor model of grandiose narcissism: one general and three specific factors (Model 8) in Study 1, Study 2 and Study 3

In Model 8 only three items produced lower factor loadings than the recommendations of Tabachnick and Fidell (2014), where .32 can be considered as a cut-off value for poor factor loadings. These three items were item 19, 26 and 14 with factor loadings ranging from .27 to .31 on the General factor similarly in every study conducted. These items however, had higher loadings for the specific factors Grandiose Exhibitionism and Entitlement/Exploitativeness ranging from 0.37 to 0.58 across the three studies.

We estimated common variance index in the models and found that the global narcissism factor explains 58.9% of the common variance in the bifactor model with two specific factors (Model 7) and 53.5% in the three specific factors solution (Model 8); therefore, a meaningful global factor seems to be present. The explained variances of the specific factors are also reported in Table 2 and Appendix, Table 4.

We evaluated the internal consistency of the factors by calculating Cronbach’s alpha, omega and omega hierarchical coefficients. Omega coefficients assess how certain scale scores’ variance is the combination of general and specific factors, therefore estimating the reliability of a latent factor (Brunner et al. 2012). The omega hierarchical coefficient, on the other hand, estimates the reliability of a latent factor, with all other latent construct variances removed (Brunner et al. 2012; Reise et al. 2010). Reise et al. (2013) proposed a strict rule that the omega hierarchical coefficients should be higher than 0.5 for the specific factors. However, our results (ranging from 0,23–0,41) suggest that the specific factors explain at least 23% of the variance in specific factor scores, underlying again the importance of a general narcissism factor.

Omega coefficients are presented in Table 2 and Appendix Table 4 showing that the specific scale scores adequately represent the mixture of general narcissism and the respective specific factors.

Confirmatory Factor Analysis with Covariates

To assess the concurrent validity of the best fitting three-factor model we used confirmatory factor analysis with covariates. The standardised coefficients are presented in Table 3.

Table 3 Correlates of the bifactor model of grandiose narcissism: confirmatory factor analyses with covariates (Study 1, Study 2 and Study 3)

Explicit self-esteem had a significant medium-sized positive relationship with the general narcissism factor and Grandiose Exhibitionism had a negative relationship with the Entitlement/Exploitativeness specific factor. Gender, on the other hand, only had a significant positive relationship with Entitlement/Exploitativeness. In the second sample, vulnerable narcissism had a positive relationship with the general factor and Entitlement/Exploitativeness, while it had a negative relationship with the leadership factor. Gender had a significant negative relationship with the general narcissism factor and the Leadership/Authority specific factor.

Study 2

Method

Participants

Participants in Study 2 were 319 university students (73% women, mean age = 20.18; SD = 2.31) from a first-year international student pool at a large Dutch university. Participants were recruited through safe internal university portals, where students could register for different research studies in exchange for course credits. After registration participants were given detailed information about the aim and tasks related to the study and were asked for their consent. For those agreed to participate surveys were distributed through email messages which participants could fill out online. The study was approved by the university research ethical committee (registration number: 18102-S).

Measures

Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry 1988)

In Study 2 the rating-scale version of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI-40) was used, which presents the original 40 narcissistic options of the NPI. Participants need to decide to which degree from a scale from 1 to 5 are those statements are describing them. This response format is recently gaining popularity (Wetzel et al. 2016; Miller 2018), and it offers the opportunity to evaluate the bifactor model with a different response format as well. In Study 2 we used the original English version of the NPI.

Narcissistic Grandiosity Scale (NGS; Rosenthal et al. 2007)

The NGS is an adjective scale that contains 16 items. Participants are presented with the 16 adjectives (eg. Glorious, Prestigious), and they need to rate how much these adjectives describe themselves. The scale has good psychometric properties (Crowe et al., 2016), and it is in strong association with other measures of narcissistic grandiosity. The internal consistency of the scale was adequate (α = .94).

Pathological Narcissism Inventory (PNI; Pincus et al. 2009)

The PNI assesses seven factors of both vulnerable and grandiose narcissism. Wright et al. (2010) proposed a factor structure of two higher order dimensions: factors Exploitativeness, Self-Sacrificing Self-Enhancement and Grandiose Fantasies form the grandiosity factor while Contingent Self-Esteem, Hiding the Self, Devaluing and Entitlement rage factors form the vulnerability factor. Items are rated from “Not at all like me” as 0 to “Very much like me” as 5. The internal consistency of the scale was adequate (α = .94).

Narcissistic Vulnerability Scale (NVS; Crowe Et al., 2018)

The NVS is also an adjective based measure similarly to the NGS, with 11 items such as Underappreciated, Insecure or Fragile. Participants needed to decide on a scale from 1 to 7 how much these adjectives describe them. The internal consistency of the scale was adequate (α = .87).

Maladaptive Covert Narcissism Scale (MCNS; Hendin & Cheek, 2013)

The MCNS is the updated version of the Hypersensitive Narcissism Scale (HSNS; Hendin & Cheek 1997) developed previously to assess vulnerable narcissism. The measure consists of 23 items. The internal consistency of the scale was adequate (α = .85).

Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg 1965)

Explicit self-esteem was assessed using the Rosenberg Self-esteem Scale (RSES; Rosenberg 1965), a widely used 10-item scale capturing global self-esteem. Participants respond to questions (e.g., “I am able to do things as well as most other people”) on a 4-point Likert scale ranging from 0 - “strongly disagree” to 3 - “strongly agree”. The measure consists of five reversed items. The internal consistency of the scale was adequate (α = .88).

Results

In Study 2 we tested the fit of the two bifactor models on a different sample with confirmatory factor analysis using the weighted least squares mean and variance adjusted estimation method (WLSMV; Brown, 2006; Finney and DiStefano 2006) similarly to Study 1, using Mplus version 8.0. We used the full information maximum likelihood estimator to deal with missing data (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2015). The fit indices are reported in Table 1. The models showed acceptable fit to the data according to current traditions, although the fit was slightly more modest than in Study 1. Standardized factor loadings of the general factor ranged from 0.28 to 0.69 on both the two and three factor models (Table 2).

We estimated common variance index in the models and found that the global narcissism factor explains 63.6% of the common variance in the two-factor model and 60.6% in the three-factor model (see Table 2 and Appendix Table 4 for more details). These results suggest, that the relevance of the global factor was also supported in Study 2.

Associations of the General and the Specific Factors

To assess the associations of the best fitting three-factor model we used confirmatory factor analysis with covariates, where we regressed the factors on the covariates. The standardised coefficients are presented in Table 3. Each covariate was regressed separately.

The general factor of grandiose narcissism was in positive medium sized association with other commonly used measures of grandiose narcissism for example the grandiosity factors of the PNI, or the NGS. Furthermore, most measures of vulnerable narcissism were unrelated with the general factor. Leadership/Authority specific factor was associated significantly only with the NGS. Grandiose Exhibitionism moderately associated with other grandiose narcissism measures, and weaker but not negligible associations with vulnerability measures. Entitlement/Exploitativeness on the other hand showed medium-strong associations with both measures of narcissistic vulnerability and grandiosity. We also observed modest gender differences on the general narcissism factor favouring men, and on both the Grandiose Exhibitionism and Entitlement/Exploitativeness factor favouring women.

Explicit self-esteem had significant positive relationship only with Grandiose Exhibitionism and Leadership/Authority factors while having a non-significant negative relationship with the Entitlement/Exploitativeness specific factor.

Study 3

Method

Participants and Procedure

Participants in Study 3 were 237 university students (75% women, mean age = 22.10; SD = 3.91) from a student pool at a large Hungarian university. Participants were recruited through safe internal university portals, where students could register for different research studies in exchange for course credits. After registration participants were given detailed information about the aim and tasks related to the study and were asked for their consent. For those agreed to participate surveys were distributed through email messages which participants could fill out online. The study was approved by the university research ethical committee (registration number: 2018/229).

Measures

Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI; Raskin & Terry, 1988)

In Study 3 also the Likert version of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI-40) was used (Wetzel et al., 2016; Miller, 2018), administered in Hungarian. Item response distributions were skewed, therefore we handled the scale as ordinal in our analysis.

Big Five Inventory (BFI-2; Soto & John, 2017)

The big five personality traits were measured with the Big Five Inventory (BFI-2), which contains 60 items forming the factors Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Negative emotionality and Open-mindedness. Participants need to decide to which degree from a scale of 1 to 5 are the statements describing them. The Hungarian version of the test was translated based on the protocol of Beaton et al. (2000). The internal consistency was mostly acceptable of the trait measures, except for the open-mindedness trait, which offered limited reliability: extraversion (α = .82), agreeableness (α = .79), conscientiousness (α = .72), negative emotionality (α = .86), open-mindedness (α = .49).

Results

In Study 3 we tested the fit of the two bifactor models with a confirmatory factor analysis using the weighted least squares mean and variance adjusted estimation method (WLSMV; Brown, 2006; Finney and DiStefano, 2006) similarly to Study 1 and Study 2, using Mplus version 8.0. We used the full information maximum likelihood estimator to deal with missing data (Muthén and Muthén, 1998–2015). The fit indices are reported in Table 1. The models showed acceptable fit to the data according to current traditions, the fit indices were similar than in Study 2. Standardized factor loadings of the general factor ranged from 0.22 to 0.71 on both the two and three factor models (Table 2 and Appendix, Table 4).

Measurement Invariance

In Study 3 we used the Likert version of the NPI similarly to Study 2, however we administered the test in Hungarian. Item response distributions were skewed, therefore we handled the scale as ordinal in our analysis following the recommendation of Finney and DiStefano (2006). They argued, that although it is a common practice in psychological research to handle rating scales as linear, in case of non-normally distributed data maximum likelihood-based χ2 values can be inflated, and model fit indices can also be underestimated (Finney & DiStefano, 2006).To overcome the possible differences with the translation we tested the invariance of the measurement with the adapted version of the NPI. Metric invariance could not be calculated due to the bifactor model and the categorical items, although scalar invariance also contains metric invariance. The configural and scalar model were significantly different from each other (χ2 = 389.41, df = 117; p < 0.001), however the differences in the fit indices (configural model: χ2 = 1112.55, CFI = 0.948, TLI = 0.937, RMSEA = 0.067, CI [0.062–0.073]; scalar model: χ2 = 1434.50, CFI = 0.931, TLI = 0.932, RMSEA = 0.070, CI [0.065–0.075]) diminished less than the recommended cut-off values proposed by Chen (2007). The allowed difference in CFI and TLI measures across the configural and scalar model could not be higher than 0.02 (Chen, 2007), which was 0.017 in the case of CFI change and 0.005 in TLI change in our model.

Associations of the General and the Specific Factors

In Study 3 the associations of the bifactor model of the NPI was also tested with a Big Five personality inventory (Table 3). Extraversion was positively associated with the general, the Leadership/Authority and the Grandiose exhibitionism factors. Agreeableness was negatively related with the general factor and it was unrelated to the specific factors. Conscientiousness was in a significant positive association with only the Leadership/Authority factor, while open-mindedness was only positively associated with the general factor. Negative emotionality on the other hand was negatively related to the general factor, while positively to the Entitlement/Exploitativeness factor.

Discussion

Our study supported the bifactor measurement models of grandiose narcissism. Besides the strong general narcissism factor, the specific factors also have meaningful, but much less, explanatory power. Previous research found mixed results regarding the factor structure of the Narcissistic Personality Inventory (NPI), applying either principal components analysis (Emmons, 1987; Raskin & Terry, 1988) or exploratory factor analysis (Ackerman et al., 2011, 2015; Corry et al. 2008; Kubarych et al., 2004) or confirmatory factor analysis (Ackerman et al., 2011; Kubarych et al., 2004).

Applying bifactor modelling in clinical measurement is gaining more popularity as it can differentiate between the shared variance of an underlying general factor and a subset of items which share variance in addition to this general factor (Rodriguez, Reise, & Haviland, 2016).

In bifactor models every item is determined by two separate latent factors, therefor two factor loadings exist: the loading on the general factor, and the loading on one of the specific factors. In our three studies the Explained Common Variance was ranging from 53.5%–63.3% and the Omega hierarchical was ranging from .76 to .80 for the General narcissism factor. Therefore, we can conclude, that this General factor plays the most important role in interpreting the results, while the specific factors have a substantially lower explanatory power, although a few items seem to reflect these specific factors to a greater extent. Moreover, the good fit of the model and the strong associations of the general factor with other measures of grandiose narcissism support the validity of the bifactor model.

Both the competing two- and three-factor solutions yielded an adequate fit to the data, however the three-factor solution provided a slightly closer fit, and it uses more items from the original construct.

As Ackerman and his colleagues argued (2016) based on a series of exploratory factor analyses, differences can occur between the factor structure of the single stimulus and forces choice response formats. In their study a five-factor solution was well-suited to the single stimulus format of the NPI. Three items formed a separate Vanity factor, although these items also had moderate cross-loadings with their Exhibitionism factor (.44–.50). In our solution the bifactor model offered the chance of disentangling the variance coming from the general narcissism factor and the specific factor of Grandiose Exhibitionism, which was originally proposed for the forced-choice response format of the NPI (Ackerman et al., 2011). The significant factor loadings of the items in both the general, both the specific factors and the good fit of the measurement model suggest, that the original factor solution of Ackerman et al. (2011) could be used in the NPI with a single-stimulus format as well when a bifactorial model is applied.

Therefore, our study also implies that it can be advisable to use the NPI in a bifactor measurement setting as latent variables in further research.

In these studies, we used two separate single-stimulus response formats of the NPI, namely the binary and the rating scale option in two different languages. Regardless of the differences in response formats used, the bifactor model with one general factor and three specific factors offered good fit to the data. Neither the Omega hierarchical measures nor the factor loadings were substantially different between the binary and the rating scale options (for the details see Table 2). Based on these results, we can conclude, that the underlying constructs measured in the NPI seem to be similar when applying the bifactor model, especially when we are using ordinal scaling following the recommendation of Finney and DiStefano (2006).

We also tested measurement invariance between the Hungarian and English versions of the NPI in a rating scale setting. Our results supported the assumption that the measurement model was invariant across the translations which further improves the generalizability of the bifactor model, showing similar factor structure in two different cultural settings.

Despite the acceptable fit provided by bifactor models in general results should be interpreted with caution (Bonifay, Lane & Reise, 2017; Rodriguez et al., 2016). The two main concerns raised are that first, bifactor models tend to fit better to any possible data therefore can result in overfitting the studied model (Bonifay & Cai; 2017). Second, it might be hard to interpret what remains of a specific factor, after accounting for an orthogonal general factor (for details see Rodriguez et al., 2016). These are indeed serious concerns, however our aim with the present study was not only to report a well-fitting model, but to investigate to what degree does the NPI seem a multidimensional construct by nature. Our results suggest, that with relevant criterion variables and other concurrent measures of narcissism our bifactor model showed mostly well-interpretable findings.

The results of the bifactor measurement model of the NPI were mainly in line with previous research. Both the other two measures of narcissistic grandiosity shown a strong positive association with the general factor, and weaker but still significant association with Grandiose Exhibitionism and Entitlement/Exploitativeness. Vulnerable narcissism on the other hand was mainly unrelated with both the general factor both the specific factors except for the Entitlement/Exploitativeness factor. This positive medium sized association is in line with other studies (Crowe et al., 2018a and b; Krusemark et al., 2018), sometimes Entitlement/Exploitativeness has been treated as an index of vulnerable narcissism (Rhodewalt & Peterson, 2009) despite the several differences between the two constructs including their different associations with self-esteem, or that people scoring higher on Entitlement/Exploitativeness are less concerned about social evaluation and avoiding aversive states (Hart et al., 2017). Furthermore, vulnerable narcissism involves being self-inhibited and constantly feeling uncertain, with a great reliance on others’ approval (Kealy & Rasmussen, 2012; Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001), all of which are in contrast with the content of the other specific factors.

The validity of a bifactor model was also tested with covariates. General and specific factors have shown different relationships with explicit self-esteem: whereas the general factor and the Grandiose Exhibitionism specific factor were positively related to explicit self-esteem, the Entitlement/Exploitativeness factor was negatively related to it both in Study 1 and Study 2, in different strength. This might have several explanations. Grandiose narcissism is consistently found to be in positive relationship with explicit self-esteem (Hyatt et al., 2018; Di Pierro et al., 2016; Maxwell et al., 2011); however, our results show that this, on the one hand is based on the general factor and, on the other, on Grandiose Exhibitionism. Individuals with defensive high self-esteem can be afraid to admit negative self-feelings, therefore presenting an overly positive or grandiose image might affect answering self-esteem related items (Kernis, 2003). In this sense, reported high self-esteem can also be part of grandiosity itself. Entitlement/Exploitativeness, on the other hand, is in negative relationship with self-esteem. This also is consistent with previous results assessing the Entitlement/Exploitativeness subscale (Ackerman and Donnellan, 2013) alone, explaining this type of entitlement as being driven by vulnerability.

Big Five personality traits were also assessed in Study 3, and the results were mostly consistent with previous studies (Ackerman et al., 2011; Dinić & Vujić, 2018), however the general factor plays a significant role in all big five traits except conscientiousness. Agreeableness was rather negatively associated with the general factor, and it was unrelated to the Entitlement/Exploitativeness specific factor, which is different from previous studies (eg. Ackerman et al., 2011; Dinić & Vujić, 2018). Negative emotionality on the other hand was negatively related to the general factor and positively to Entitlement/Exploitativeness which might also confirm, that this specific factor can be considered the maladaptive aspect of grandiose narcissism. It also means, that the general narcissism factor is masking the association between Entitlement/Exploitativeness and negative emotionality. Similarly, specific factor’s association with Agreeableness and Open-mindedness is negligible if the general factor is already accounted for. These results also support the advantages of the bifactor model, as it can differentiate the effect of the general and specific factors in the associations with other covariates.

Although in both studies gender was a significant covariate, there were some differences between the studies. Women tended to score higher on the Entitlement/Exploitativeness specific factor in both studies, while men scored higher on the general factor. Grandiose Exhibitionism was also higher in female participants in 2 out of the 3 samples. Our findings are in contrast with a recent meta-analysis (Grijalva et al., 2015), which found that men scored higher on every specific narcissistic factor than women. This discrepancy may highlight the possibility that culture moderates gender differences in narcissism; however, our results in both studies were captured from samples of university students, and therefore do not represent the general population of neither Hungary, nor the Netherlands. Students applying to psychological studies in both countries may face significant competition, which might be in connection with higher levels of narcissism.

A further possible explanation is that recent generations are more narcissistic than previous ones, in particular, women have become more similar to men with the changing social roles (Grijalva et al., 2015). This, however, only provides a partial explanation as, in most studies – despite the changes – men are found to be more exploitative and to feel more entitled. The exact items that show the biggest gender difference favouring women were: ‘I expect a great deal from other people’ and ‘I insist upon getting the respect that is due to me’. This finding is consistent with the observation of a strong gender inequality (WEF 2017), implying higher awareness among women about receiving the same respect as is afforded men. Furthermore, these differences are larger in the sample of psychology students, whose curricula and social psychological studies often contain gender inequality. Therefore, it is likely that this difference is not closely connected to feeling entitled. The other item related to the specific factor, ‘I find it easy to manipulate people’, was found to be endorsed more often by male, as opposed to female, participants. This finding is consistent with previous results (Grijalva et al., 2015).

Conclusions

According to our findings, grandiose narcissism as a general factor accounts for at least half of the variance captured in the NPI while the role of specific factors remains limited. Therefore, the measurement of this general narcissism factor seems important when we would like to understand the associations with other psychological constructs. Based on our results the general narcissism factor and the relevant specific factors offered by Ackerman and his colleagues (2011) can be assessed using a shorter version of the NPI containing 25 items. Associations of these factors with other relevant variables seem to justify the validity of the bifactor model, although further research is needed to identify the associations with other relevant constructs, in other languages or other cultural settings. Moreover, further studies are needed to identify the role of the specific factors, namely what remains of these factors after the variance accounted for by the general grandiose narcissism factor is substracted from them, and how these specific factors are related to other relevant psychological constructs.

Our study also aims to offer results regarding the response format used in the NPI. According to our findings, the factor structure offered for the forced-choice response format of the NPI (Ackerman et al., 2011) offers acceptable fit to the data if the single-stimulus response is used with the bifactor measurement model.

Limitations

The present study also has its limitations. First, we used university samples in both studies, which does not represent the general population of the two countries, however most of the current literature of narcissism is based on student samples. Second, a wider range of covariates might be included into the analyses in the future supplemented with observation-based or expert rating variables. Third, in all of our studies, we used the single-stimulus response format versions of the NPI. Further studies are needed to validate, if this bifactorial model could also be applied in the original, forced choice response format.