Introduction

Anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent mental disorders in childhood (Merikangas et al. 2009), hamper daily functioning and tend to persist into adulthood (Bandelow and Michaelis 2015). It is, therefore, necessary to examine processes linked to the onset and maintenance of anxiety disorders in order to improve or develop effective interventions.

Current theoretical frameworks state that information processing deficits are at the base of anxiety disorders (Beck and Clark 1997; Eysenck et al. 2007; Ouimet et al. 2009). Anxious people display several information processing biases that are not, or to a lesser extent, observed in non-anxious persons (Ouimet et al. 2009). A bias that has lately gained attention is confirmation bias. This bias is expressed in the tendency to search for information that confirms the (negative) view a person holds, while ignoring alternative information that challenges the current view. Such preference for verification over falsification strategy then automatically results in the reinforcement and maintenance of the fear (De Jong et al. 1997; Mathews and MacLeod 2005).

Several studies have indicated that (indirectly) establishing a negative view in children results in inclined search for information that confirms this view (Muris et al. 2011; Muris et al. 2009; Remmerswaal et al. 2010). Even the mere perception of threat, without explicit establishment, is sufficient to encourage such strategy (Dibbets et al. 2014; Dibbets and Meesters 2017). The search for negative information can in turn enhance fear levels, indicating a reciprocal relationship between fear and confirmation bias (Dibbets and Meesters 2017; Remmerswaal et al. 2014).

In the studies by Muris and colleagues, children received negative, positive or ambiguous information about an unknown animal. The attitudes towards the animal changed in line with the (indirectly) provided information. However, this change was not equally strong. Providing negative information resulted in a stronger change in attitude than providing positive information (Muris et al. 2009; Remmerswaal et al. 2014). This imbalance in change in attitudes is not limited to fear attitudes (Dibbets et al. 2012). In general, bad impressions and stereotypes are faster to form and more resilient to disconfirmation than good ones (Baumeister et al. 2001).

As information can affect attitudes and attitudes are reflected in search for information, a logical deduction would be that changes in attitudes should also be reflected in changes in search for information. Though the objective of previous studies was not to link changes in attitudes to confirmation bias, the results seem indicative of such relationship. Providing negative information resulted in a strong change in attitude. This negative attitude was subsequently reflected in a negative confirmation bias pattern with an increased search for negative information. However, providing positive information did not yield equal decreases in attitude; the decrease was not — or only weakly— reflected in increased search for positive information (Muris et al. 2009; Remmerswaal et al. 2014). Though this seems to imply that a change in attitude results in an accompanying change in confirmation bias, this cannot directly be deduced from these data. Confirmation bias was only measured once, so no change in bias could be observed.

The current study is set up to fill this gap and seeks to link alterations in attitude to changes in confirmation bias in children. To this end, emotionally valued stimulus material will be used: a positively valued unfamiliar animal, the quokka, and a negatively valued unknown animal, the aye aye (cf. Dibbets et al. 2014; Dibbets and Meesters 2017). Baseline attitudes and search for information about these animals will be assessed. Subsequently, counter-attitudinal information will be provided and the attitude towards the animals and confirmation bias will be reassessed. Based on previous studies, it is hypothesized that it is easier to increase the negative value of the quokka than to decrease the aversiveness of the aye aye. Secondly, we expect that a putative change in attitude will result in a change in confirmation bias patterns. This change will, probably, be mostly pronounced after providing negative counter-attitudinal information (quokka) and not, or to a lesser extent, after positive information (aye aye).

The results of this study are important, because if providing alternative information affects attitudes and, subsequently, accompanying confirmation bias patterns, this may provide a starting point for further development of anxiety treatments and preventions.

Methods

Participants

The sample consisted of sixty-three Dutch children (27 boys, 36 girls), aged 9–12 years (M = 10.44, SD = 10 months) recruited via a regular primary school. Parents received an information letter and an informed consent. Of the parents, the majority was Dutch (87.1%), 4.7% had the German nationality, 4.7% had the Indonesian nationality and the parents of the remaining children had different nationalities (e.g., Moroccan, Turkish and Italian). Children with a history of anxiety problems were excluded from participation. The study was carried out in line with the declaration of Helsinki (Williams 2008) and approved by the local ethical committee (approval code: 178_11_04_2017).

Animals

Two animals were used for the current study, the quokka and the aye aye (cf. Dibbets et al. 2014; Dibbets and Meesters 2017). In a previous study the aye aye was rated as threatening and negative, whereas the quokka received positive ratings. The pictures were printed on a separate paper (163 cm2) or presented via a computer screen and labelled with the species name (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Picture of the quokka (left) and aye aye (right)

Fear Beliefs Questionnaire

Two distinct Fear Beliefs Questionnaires, FBQs, were filled out; one for the quokka and one for the aye aye (cf. Dibbets et al. 2014; Dibbets and Meesters 2017; Muris et al. 2009). A picture of the animal questioned accompanied the questionnaire. The questionnaire was adjusted, that is, next to the 10 statements that measured the amount of fear, 10 statements about how much the child liked the animal were included. An example of a fear-related item is as follows: “Would you find it scary to touch a quokka?” and of a likeability item “Would you like to play with the quokka?” Children could rate these items on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “No, not at all” to “Yes, definitely”. Per animal a total fear score and likeability score can be calculated by summing the ratings on all related items (range 10–50). Cronbach’s alpha for the subscales ranged from .77 (quokka fear-related) to .95 (aye aye likeability).

Attitudes towards the Animals

Attitudes towards the animals were assessed by printed and computerized Visual Analogue Scales, VASprint and VASpc, respectively. The printed version consisted of two negative (e.g.,“Do you think that the quokka is a dangerous animal?”) and two positive items (e.g., “Do you think that the aye aye is a kind animal?”). The scores of the two negative and of the two positive items were averaged to a respectively, negative and positive valence score. In the computerized version, the child was asked for the scariness of the animal. All scores ranged between 0 (not at all) to 100 (extremely). The computerized version was added as it allowed a fast, immediate valence rating of the animals before, in between and after the confirmation bias tasks. In total, the VASpc was offered four times for each animal (VASpc#1, VASpc#2, VASpc#3 and VASpc#4).

Confirmation Bias

The confirmation bias task strongly resembled that of previous research (Dibbets et al. 2014; Dibbets and Meesters 2017; Remmerswaal et al. 2015). The task was programmed using e-prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh, PA; Schneider et al. 2012) and ran on a laptop connected to a 24 in. monitor. In this task, the tendency to search for positive, neutral and negative information was measured. For each animal 12 trials were administered; the VASpc was presented before and after these trials (VASpc#1 and VASpc#2, respectively). A trial started with the presentation of the following question on the screen: “What do you further want to know about the quokka/aye aye?”. The child could select one of three information options at the bottom of the screen: a negative option (e.g., “I want to know if the aye aye eats other animals”), a neutral option (e.g., “I want to know if the aye aye eats green leafs”) and a positive option (“I want to know if the aye aye eats delicious fruits”). The order of the information options systematically varied across trials. The picture of the particular animal was presented at the middle of the computer screen, between the question and information options. After selection, a confirmative answer was displayed. For example, selection of the neutral option “I want to know if the aye aye eats green leafs” would result in the answer “Yes, the aye aye eats green leaves”. For each animal two sets of information options were available so that the task could be presented twice. The number of positive, neutral or negative selection choices were summed up per animal. Both the set of information options and the order in which the animals were presented were counterbalanced across participants.

Selection of Information Task

The selection of information task (SIT) consists of 9 statements about each animal. Of these statements 3 are positive, 3 negative and 3 neutral; the order of the items is random. The child is requested to select three statements per animal.

Counter-Attitudinal Information

For both the aye aye and the quokka an imaginary Wikipedia page was made, containing counter-attitudinal information. Each page contained information about the animal’s habitat, eating pattern, family life and response to humans. The text was accompanied by a (novel) picture of the animal. For the aye aye positive statements were presented; for the quokka negative information was provided.

Procedure

Children were tested in their classroom under supervision of two research assistants. The assistants guided the children through the session by providing instructions, exemplifying several questionnaires and they ensured that children did not consult each other during testing. Children filled out the FBQs and VASprint for each animal. Children seated next to each other were provided with different versions of the questionnaires (e.g., some children started with the FBQ concerning the quokka, others with the FBQ regarding the aye aye). Subsequently, the confirmation task was individually conducted in a separate room. The order of animals during the confirmation bias task was similar to that of the questionnaires (confirmation bias I). Before and after the confirmation bias task of each animal, the children rated that particular animal with the VASpc (VASpc#1 and VASpc#2, respectively). After completion of the confirmation bias task for both animals, children received counter-attitudinal information about the first animal. Next, the second confirmation bias task (confirmation bias II) and VASpc (VASpc#3 and VASpc#4, respectively) of this particular animal were presented. This procedure was repeated for the remaining animal. Finally, they completed the SIT. Debriefing took place after the last participant was tested.

Statistical Analyses

A total of 7 out of 3024 items of the confirmation bias answers were missing. These incomplete sumscores (frequency of positive, negative and neutral answers) were proportionally corrected. The questionnaire and VAS data were analyzed parametrically (General Linear Model with repeated measures, GLM); the SIT selection (number of selected negative, neutral or positive statements) was analyzed by means of non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon Signed Ranks test). Main effects are not reported in case of an interaction. Bonferroni Holm corrections were made in case of multiple comparisons. In case of violation of sphericity Greenhouse-Geisser corrections were made. The rejection criterion was set at p < .05.

Results

Attitudes towards Animals

Table 1 summarizes the questionnaire data. A GLM with animal (quokka and aye aye) and valence (likeability and fear-related) was run on the FBQ data. This analysis revealed an interaction between animal and valence, F(1,62) = 105.40, p < .001, ηρ2 = .63. Separate GLMs for the likeability and fear-related data indicated that the quokka received higher likeability ratings, F(1, 62) = 108.17, p < .001, ηρ2 = .64, and lower fear-related ratings, F(1, 62) = 78.69, p < .001, ηρ2 = .56, than did the aye aye. A similar GLM was run on the VASprint data. In this analysis valence (positive and negative) and animal (quokka and aye aye) served as within subjects factors. This analysis yielded an animal x valence interaction, F(1, 62) = 59.69, p < .001, ηρ2 = .49. Separate GLMs for the negative and positive scores indicated that the quokka was perceived as more positive, F(1, 62) = 68.90, p < .001, ηρ2 = .53, and less negative, F(1, 62) = 19.09, p < .001, ηρ2 = .24, than the aye aye. The first VASpc also indicated that the aye aye was rated scarier than the quokka, F(1, 62) = 62.30, p < .001, ηρ2 = .50. In sum, these results indicate that the aye aye was perceived as more negative and threatening than the quokka.

Table 1 Mean scores (SDs) on the questionnaires and tests concerning the animals

Confirmation Bias I

The confirmation bias data (I and II) are represented in Fig. 2. A GLM was run with animal (quokka and aye aye) and information type (frequency of positive, neutral and negative choices) as within-subjects factors. This analysis yielded an animal x information type interaction, F(2, 124) = 19.40, p < .001, ηρ2 = .24. Separate GLMs were run per animal to assess the search for information patterns. Both analyses revealed main effects for type of information, Fs(2, 124) > 6.81, ps < .003, ηρ2 > .098. Pairwise comparisons indicated that for the quokka no specific confirmation bias pattern was observed, the children requested more negative and positive than neutral information, ps < .003. No difference between negative and positive information was observed, p = .85. For the aye aye more negative than neutral and positive information was requested, ps < .001, and more positive than neutral information was asked for, p = .007. Direct comparisons between the animals indicated that, compared to the aye aye, for the quokka more positive, F(1, 62) = 16.64, p < .001, ηρ2 = .21, more neutral, F(1, 62) = 6.95, p = .011, ηρ2 = .10, and less negative information, F(1, 62) = 26.43, p < .001, ηρ2 = .30, was requested.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean response patterns and SEMs for confirmation bias task I (left side) and task II (right side)

Change in Attitude I

The scariness of both animals before (VASpc#1) and after the confirmation bias task (VASpc#2) was assessed. A GLM repeated measures with time (VASpc#1 and VASpc#2) and animal (quokka and aye aye) as within-subject factors was run. The GLM indicated that, overall, the aye aye received higher scariness ratings than did the quokka, F(1, 62) = 77.67, p < .001, ηρ2 = .56, no effect of time of interaction were observed, Fs < 1. Providing the counter-attitudinal information did change the VASpc scariness ratings, as the GLM did revealed an interaction between time (VASpc#2 before counter-attitudinal information and VASpc#3 after information) and animal (quokka and aye aye), F(1, 620 = 13.11, p = .001, ηρ2 = .17. Separate GLMs per animal indicated that the scariness ratings of the aye aye decreased, F(1, 62) = 20.91, p < .001, ηρ2 = .25, no change in ratings for the quokka was observed, F < 1. Note that after the information the aye aye was still rated as scarier than the quokka, F(1, 62) = 13.69, p < .001, ηρ2 = .18.

Confirmation Bias II

The GLM with animal (quokka and aye aye) and information type (number of positive, neutral and negative information choices) as within-subjects factors Revealed no (interaction) effects, Fs < 1. Separate analyses per animal revealed that for both animals no main effect of information was observed, Fs < 1. These results indicated that the children did not have a specific preference for one type of information, regardless of the animal.

Change in Attitude II

The scariness of the animals was assessed before and after the second confirmation bias task, VASpc#3 and VASpc#4, respectively. A GLM with time and animal as within-subjects factors revealed a main effect of animal, F(1, 62) = 12.42, p = .001, ηρ2 = .17, and a marginally significant interaction between time and animal, F(1, 62) = 3.47, p = .067, ηρ2 = .053. This nearly-significant interaction was caused by the tendency of an increase in scariness for the quokka, F(1, 62) = 2.99, p = .089, ηρ2 = .046, and the absence of a significant change in the aye aye, F < 1.

Change in Confirmation Bias

A GLM with time (before and after counter-attitudinal information), animal (quokka and aye aye) and selected information type (number of positive, neutral and negative choices) as within-subject factors was run. This analysis yielded a time x animal x information interaction, F(2, 124) = 11.84, p < .001, ηρ2 = .16. For all types of information an interaction between time and animal was observed, Fs(1, 62) > 4.80, ps < .05, ηρ2 > .071. Separate GLMs per animal and information type indicated that for the quokka only an increase in search for neutral information was observed, F(1, 62) = 8.65, p = .005, ηρ2 = .12, whereas no significant changes in negative or positive information were observed, Fs(1, 62) < 1.07, ps > .30, ηρ2 < .017. For the aye aye an increase in search for positive, F(1, 62) = 8.01, p = .006, ηρ2 = .11, and neutral information, F(1, 62) = 40.70, p < .001, ηρ2 = .40, was observed, and a decrease in negative information, F(1, 62) = 40.95, p < .001, ηρ2 = .40. These results indicate that presenting counter-attitudinal information can change information search patterns.

Selection of Information (SIT)

Wilcoxon signed ranks tests indicated that for both the aye aye and the quokka more positive than neutral options were selected, |Z|s > 2.21, ps < .044. No differences were observed between the animals, |Z|s < .27, ps > .79.

Discussion

The present study examined the influence of counter-attitudinal information on confirmation bias in children. Sixty-three non-clinical children were invited to search information about the dangerous-looking aye aye and the kindly-perceived quokka (confirmation bias I). Counter-attitudinal information about each animal was provided followed by a repetition of the confirmation bias task (confirmation bias II). Additionally, fear beliefs (FBS) and attitudes (VASprint and VASpc) and selection of information (SIT) were measured.

The aye aye was rated as more negative than the quokka. This negative attitude was reflected in the search pattern during confirmation bias I, more negative information was requested for the aye aye and less positive and neutral information. These results are in line with previous research on confirmation bias in children (Dibbets et al. 2014; Dibbets and Meesters 2017; Muris et al. 2011; Muris et al. 2009; Remmerswaal et al. 2010). Presenting counter-attitudinal information resulted in a decreased scariness for the aye aye, this change was also visible in the subsequent confirmation bias task. Compared to the first task, a decrease in negative information and an increase in positive and neutral information were observed. Providing negative information about the quokka tended to increase the scariness of the animal; additionally, an increase in search for neutral information was observed. Furthermore, at the end, when free to select statements, for both animals more positive than neutral items were selected.

The results are not in line with our expectations and previous research. Contrary to our hypotheses, providing positive information about the negatively rated aye aye resulted in an altered search for information. Providing negative information only marginally affected the scariness of the quokka and was not reflected in a subsequent increase in search for negative information. A possible explanation is that the increased likeability functioned as a buffer for the negative information. As the difference in likeability surpassed the difference in fear between the animals, F(1, 62) = 9.58, p = .003, ηρ2 =. 13, it might be more difficult to adjust the former than the latter. An alternative explanation is that the provided information differed in valence. Thought we tried to control for the topics discussed, it might be the case that the negative information was not as strongly valued as the positive information. Unfortunately, we did not rate the value of the fake information provided. For a next study, we would advise to incorporate this measurement or to pilot the Wiki pages used to ensure that the information is equally strongly valued.

The current experiment indicates that it is relatively easy to change the attitudes towards the animals. However, this change is not straightforwardly reflected in subsequent search behaviour. This might be due to a discrepancy between the explicit attitude measurements (VASpc and VASprint) and the more implicit selection tasks. In the attitude measurements, the children are explicitly asked to value the animals; whereas the search for information tasks do not mention the value of the animals, but simply invite the children to gather information. Explicit attitudes more quickly change in response to relatively small amounts of counter-attitudinal information. This is thought to reflect rule-based reasoning, in which children have to solve two competing propositions (e.g., the quokka is sweet vs. the quokka is evil), resulting in an attitude change to resolve this inconsistency (Gawronski and Bodenhausen 2006). On the other hand, implicit attitudes are modified by a slow-learning, associative system of evaluation. They are less malleable and need more counter-attitudinal information in order to change (Rydell et al. 2007). Therefore, it is not certain that a change in explicit attitude will be immediately reflected in subsequent (search) behaviour. For future research, it might be interesting to use multiple sessions to promote more thorough establishment of both explicit and implicit attitude changes and test the trajectories of these changes during information bias tasks.

Admittedly, the present study suffers from several limitations. Firstly, we did not observe a positive confirmation bias for the quokka (confirmation bias I). Though the quokka was perceived as more kind, this did not result in increased search for positive information. Even more, the amount of positive and negative information requested did not differ from each other. However, when we take a closer look at the data we do see that increased likeability (FBQ) and a more positive VASprint score, coincide with increased search for positive information, rs > .28, ps < .023, and, if any, with a decreased search for negative information, r = −.26, p = .040. These results do suggest the presence of a positive confirmation bias.

A second limitation is that we only included children without anxiety problems. Therefore, we cannot generalize our results to a clinical population. The pre-existing attitudes regarding the novel animals might be rather weak compared to established fears, making these attitudes more susceptible to change than clinical fears. A next logical step for future research would be to replicate current findings with more firmly established attitudes (e.g., spider fear) and, thereafter, to examine the influence of counter-attitudinal information in a clinical sample.

In conclusion, we demonstrated that change in attitude results in a change in confirmation bias patterns. This effect was most obvious after providing positive information. Though unexpected, this is promising as this might indicate that it is possible to adjust negative views and their accompanying information search patterns. This observation can help to explore further techniques to alleviate anxiety problems.