Skip to main content
Log in

Time for politics: The relationship between political attitude and implicit space-time mappings

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Temporal Focus Hypothesis (TFH) proposes that people’s implicit space-time mappings are shaped by their attentional focus on temporal events. We used this hypothesis to examine predictions made by psychologists who suggest that the conservatives tend to endorse tradition and are more past-focused while liberals prefer progressive change and are more future-focused. In line with these predictions, we found that the conservatives, who showed more agreement with past-focused statements, were more likely to conceptualize the past as in front of them, whereas liberals, who showed more agreement with future-focused statements, were more likely to conceptualize the future as in front of them. Our results shed new light on the TFH and reveal a relationship between political attitude and implicit space-time mappings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Previous research has shown that there is an interaction between time-of-day, chronotype and spatial conceptions of time (Li 2018). A different line of evidence has also shown that chronotype is related to political ideology. For instance, morning types tend to be more conservative and evening types are more liberal (Ksiazkiewicz 2019). In order to provide a complete picture of people’s spatial conceptions of time, we collected our data throughout a day and used a relatively large sample to exclude individual differences in biographic properties such as chronotype.

  2. All participants in the experiment also responded to 4 items which described thinking about the present in the TFS. However, there were no significant differences among the three groups (p > .50). Since they were unrelated to the main hypothesis, we didn’t report them in the studies.

References

  • Ahrens, K. V., & Huang, C. R. (2002). Time passing is motion. Language and Linguistics, 3(3), 491–519.

    Google Scholar 

  • Almond, G. A., & Verba, S. (2015). The civic culture: Political attitudes and democracy in five nations. Princeton University Press.

  • Alverson, H. (1994). Semantics and experience: Universal metaphors of time in English, mandarin, Hindi, and Sesotho. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Athanasopoulos, P., Samuel, S., & Bylund, E. (2017). The psychological reality of spatio-temporal metaphors. In A. Athanasiadou (Ed.), Studies in figurative thought and language (pp. 296–321). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, M., & Lammers, J. (2016). Past-focused environmental comparisons promote proenvironmental outcomes for conservatives. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(52), 14953–14957.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28.

  • Bylund, E., Gygax, P., Samuel, S., & Athanasopoulos, P. (2019). Author accepted manuscript: Back to the future? The role of temporal focus for mapping time onto space. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.

  • Caprara, G. V., & Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Personalizing politics: A congruency model of political preference. The American Psychologist, 59, 581–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D. (2016). Temporal language and temporal thinking may not go hand in hand. In B. Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk (Ed.), Conceptualizations of time (pp. 169–186). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casasanto, D., & Jasmin, K. (2012). The hands of time: Temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics, 23, 643–674.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, C., & Lin, Z. (2012). A cross-cultural examination of the positivity effect in memory: United States vs. China. International Journal of Aging & Human Development, 75(1), 31–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • De la Fuente, J., Santiago, J., Román, A., Dumitrache, C., & Casasanto, D. (2014). When you think about it, your past is in front of you: How culture shapes spatial conceptions of time. Psychological Science, 25, 1682–1690.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duffy, S. E., & Feist, M. I. (2014). Individual differences in the interpretation of ambiguous statements about time. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(1): 29–54.

  • Eikmeier, V., Hoppe, D., & Ulrich, R. (2015). Response mode does not modulate the space–time congruency effect: Evidence for a space–time mapping at a conceptual level. Acta Psychologica, 156, 162–167.

  • Gallup. 2015. Majority say moral values getting worse. Available at www.gallup.com/poll/183467/majority-say-moral-values-getting-worse.aspx. Accessed 20 Nov 2017.

  • Gromet, D. M., Hartson, K. A., & Sherman, D. K. (2015). The politics of luck: Political ideology and the perceived relationship between luck and success. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 59, 40–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Israel, J. (1966). Student nationalism in China: 1927–1937. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, D. (2006). Citizen movements and china's public intellectuals in the hu-wen era. Pacific Affairs, 79(2), 183–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ksiazkiewicz, A. (2019). Conservative larks, liberal owls: The relationship between chronotype and political ideology. Journal of Politics.

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lane, R. E. (1965). Political life: Why and how people get involved in politics. Free Press.

  • Li, H. (2017). Time on hands: Deliberate and spontaneous temporal gestures by speakers of mandarin. Gesture, 16, 400–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, H. (2018). A future-minded lark in the morning: The influence of time-of-day and chronotype on metaphorical associations between space and time. Metaphor and Symbol, 33(1), 48–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, H., & Cao, Y. (2017a). The hope of the future: The experience of pregnancy influences women’s implicit space-time mappings. The Journal of Social Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.2017.1297289.

  • Li, H., & Cao, Y. (2017b). Personal attitudes toward time: The relationship between temporal focus, space-time mappings and real life experiences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 58(3), 193–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, H., & Cao, Y. (2018). Time will tell: Temporal landmarks influence metaphorical associations between space and time. Cognitive Linguistics, 29(4), 677–701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, H., Bui, V. Q., & Cao, Y. (2018). One country, two cultures: Implicit space–time mappings in southern and northern Vietnamese. European Journal of Social Psychology, 48(4), 560–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin, F., Sun, Y., & Yang, H. (2015). How are Chinese students ideologically divided? A survey of Chinese college students’ political self-identification. Pacific Affairs, 88(1), 51–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGlone, M. S., & Harding, J. L. (1998). Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(5), 1211–1223.

  • Núñez, R., & Cooperrider, K. (2013). The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(5), 220–229.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Núñez, R. E., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30(3), 401–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pew Research Center (2016) Campaign exposes fissures over issues, values and how life has changed in the U.S. Available at www.people-press.org/2016/03/31/campaign-exposes-fissures-over-issues-values-and-how-life-has-changed-in-the-u-s/. Accessed 20 Nov 2017.

  • Robinson, M. D., Cassidy, D. M., Boyd, R. L., & Fetterman, A. K. (2015). The politics of time: Conservatives differentially reference the past and liberals differentially reference the future. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 45(7), 391–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shipp, A. J., Edwards, J. R., & Lambert, L. S. (2009). Conceptualization and measurement of temporal focus: The subjective experience of the past, present, and future. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110(1), 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Valentino, N. A., Hutchings, V. L., & White, I. K. (2002). Cues that matter: How political ads prime racial attitudes during campaigns. American Political Science Review, 96(1), 75–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zheng, Y. (1999). Discovering Chinese nationalism in China: Modernization, identity, and international relations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

This work was supported by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities, Southwest University (SWU1909753); Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities”, Zhongnan University of Economics and Law (2722019JCG038).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Heng Li.

Ethics declarations

This study was approved by the ethical board of the Department of Humanities, Northumbria University and all procedures performed in it involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Li, H., Cao, Y. Time for politics: The relationship between political attitude and implicit space-time mappings. Curr Psychol 41, 1184–1190 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00640-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00640-4

Keywords

Navigation