Introduction

Social Networking Sites (SNSs) are “virtual communities where users can create individual public profiles, interact with real-life friends, and meet other people based on shared interests” (Kuss and Griffiths 2011 p. 3529). Social networking is currently one of the most popular forms of communication and entertainment among Internet users (GlobalWebIndex 2018). It is estimated, that in 2021, there will be approximately 3 billion SNSs users globally (Statista 2019). However, there is growing scientific evidence suggesting that excessive use of social networking sites may result in symptoms traditionally associated with substance-related addictions (Andreassen 2015; Grant et al. 2010; Griffiths et al. 2014; He et al. 2017).

SNS Addiction as an Example of Behavioral Addiction

SNS addiction is by some scholars regarded as a behavioral addiction, emerging from the framework, theoretical work and research on Internet addiction (Andreassen et al. 2016; Kuss and Griffiths 2011; Montag et al. 2014; Müller et al. 2016; van Rooij et al. 2017; Young 2009). It has been defined as “being overly concerned about SNSs, driven by a strong motivation to log on to or use SNSs, and to devote so much time and effort to SNSs that it impairs other social activities, studies/job, interpersonal relationships, and/or psychological health and well-being” (Andreassen and Pallesen 2014 p. 4054). According to the components model of addictions (Griffiths 2005), the symptoms of SNS addiction include salience, mood modification, tolerance, withdrawal, relapse, and conflict. Previous empirical findings suggest that SNS addiction may be a serious mental health problem with negative consequences for the psychosocial functioning of the sufferer and next of kin (see Andreassen 2015). Although there has been a rise in the number of studies on SNSs use and abuse, the status of SNS addiction is still to be formally acknowledged.

Conceptual and Methodological Perspectives on SNS Addiction

In the fourth quarter of 2018, Facebook had involved on average 1.52 billion daily active users (Facebook 2019). As one of the biggest social networking websites, Facebook has become almost synonymous with social networking (Griffiths 2012). Studies conducted on SNS addiction have so far mainly focused on Facebook addiction, as Facebook was one of the first SNS established and has since been regarded as a prototypical example of SNS (see Marino et al. 2018a; Ryan et al. 2014). However, with the variety of apps and services that SNSs users have at their disposal today, social networking has become eclectic (Kuss and Griffiths 2017). Internet users can decide to maintain accounts across a wide range of platforms (e.g. Instagram, Snapchat, Twitter, Pinterest). In 2016 the average Internet user had accounts on almost eight different social websites (GlobalWebIndex 2017). Among those who have account on Facebook only, SNS addiction and Facebook addiction should be regarded as equivalent. On the other hand, SNS addiction and Facebook addiction does not necessarily reflect the same phenomenon among multisite-social networkers. Accordingly, SNS addiction and Facebook addiction should be viewed as similar but separate entities among multisite-social networkers (Kuss and Griffiths 2017).

Based on SNS addiction theory and empirical findings, the dynamic evolution of social networking sites has spawn controversies from both conceptual and methodological perspectives (see Andreassen and Pallesen 2013; Griffiths 2012). According to uses and gratifications theory, the use of a particular media is goal-directed and could be related to different forms of gratification as well as distinct needs underlying this use (Katz et al. 1973). From this perspective it seems crucial to take results from studies about specific sites into account in order to understand the development of SNS addiction (Ryan et al. 2014). In line with this, studies have now focused on addiction to specific platforms, for example Instagram (Kircaburun and Griffiths 2018), Twitter (Ndasauka et al. 2016) and Snapchat (Punyanunt-Carter et al. 2017). Contrarily, some scholars still suggest that SNS addiction should be framed as a behavior detached of a particular SNS platform and that addictions to specific sites are only examples of SNS addiction (Griffiths et al. 2014; Kuss and Griffiths 2017). According to the latter approach, SNS addiction is viewed as a generic phenomenon (e.g. Tang and Koh 2017; Wang et al. 2018). In line with such understanding, impairment associated with Facebook addiction (related for example to higher stress and lower well-being) should also be present in SNS addiction generally. Another theoretical viewpoint concerns which aspects of a particular site (e.g. Facebook) or SNS in general, are in fact addictive and cause mental problems (Andreassen and Pallesen 2013, 2014).

Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale and Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale

Along with the discussion about the nature of the phenomenon of SNS addiction, research efforts have been taken with the aim of developing scales to measure addiction to specific sites (e.g. Facebook Intrusion Questionnaire; Elphinston and Noller 2011; Facebook Dependence Questionaire; Wolniczak et al. 2013) as well as SNS addiction in general (e.g. Addictive Tendences Towards SNSs; Wu et al. 2013; Social Networking Website Addiction Scale; Turel and Serenko 2012). The Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale (BFAS; Andreassen et al. 2012) was constructed based on general addiction theory, and measures Facebook addiction according to six basic addiction symptoms (i.e. salience, conflict, mood modification, withdrawal, tolerance, and relapse; Griffiths 2005). The BFAS has been adapted into several languages and has overall demonstrated good psychometric properties (e.g. Atroszko et al. 2018; Phanasathit et al. 2015; Wang et al. 2015). Nonetheless, a limitation of the scale is that is assesses addiction to one specific platform (i.e. Facebook) only. In order to overcome this limitation, the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS; Andreassen et al. 2016) was developed and represents a modified version of the BFAS replacing “Facebook” with “social networking sites”, the latter being defined as “Facebook, Twitter, Instagram and the like” in the instructions. While the names “Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale” and “Bergen Social Networking Addiction Scale” are used interchangeably in the literature (Andreassen et al. 2017; Andreassen et al. 2017), we use in the following “social networking sites” as social media and social networking sites reflect similar but yet distinct phenomena (see Kuss and Griffiths 2017). The psychometric robustness of the BSMAS has been examined in Italian (Monacis et al. 2017), Hungarian (Bányai et al. 2017), Persian (Lin et al. 2017), and Chinese (Leung et al. 2020; Chen et al. 2020) samples, where it has showed good properties. Although previous studies have used a variety of methods to examine this scale, there is currently no study that has investigated the psychometric properties of the BSMAS in comparison to the BFAS. What is more, there is currently no empirical study that has investigated the relationship between a specific and a generic SNS addiction scale.

SNS Addiction, Personality and Well-Being

Theoretical models of problematic Internet use and SNS addiction have emphasized the prominence of predisposing factors (Andreassen 2015; Atroszko et al. 2018; Brand et al. 2014; Caplan 2010; Davis 2001; Pelling and White 2009). Studies on SNS addiction have in this realm emphasized the role of certain personality traits predicting both use and abuse of SNS (Andreassen et al. 2013). With reference to the Big Five (Extraversion - being outgoing, talkative; Agreeableness - being sympathetic and warm; Conscientiousness - being organized and prompt, Neuroticism - being nervous and moody; Openness/Intellect- being creative and intellectually oriented) model of personality (Wiggins 1996) general SNS addiction and Facebook addiction have quite consistently been positively related to neuroticism (De Cock et al. 2014; Marino et al. 2018a) and negatively to conscientiousness (Błachnio et al. 2017; De Cock et al. 2014; Marino et al. 2018a). What is more, a meta-analysis showed a weak negative relationship between extraversion, agreeableness and openness to experience and Facebook addiction (Marino et al. 2018a). In terms of mental health both Facebook addiction and more general SNS addiction have been found to be associated with low well-being and psychological distress (Atroszko et al. 2018; Hou et al. 2017; Marino et al. 2018b; Pontes 2017). Hormes et al. (2014) reported that Facebook addiction is related to emotional regulation deficits and susceptibility to both substance and non-substance addiction. Addictive use of WeChat has been found to be negatively associated with users’ physical, mental and social health beyond personality traits and demographic variables (Xue et al. 2018). Previous studies have also suggest that both typical and excessive SNS users experience reduction in perceived stress and improvement in well-being following SNS abstinence of several days. Furthermore, the positive effects were more substantial in the excessive SNS user group than among typical SNS users (Tromholt 2016; Turel et al. 2018). These findings suggest that both general and specific SNS-addiction seems to have parallel relationships with well-being and mental health. However, in the context of the constantly changing nature of SNSs, it is crucial to investigate the relative contribution of those phenomena in terms of well-being and mental health among different SNSs users. Taking into account that SNS addiction as a generic form of addiction might cumulate addictive properties from different SNSs it should also impair the well-being above and beyond addiction of (e.g. Facebook) specific sites.

Aims of the Present Study

In the light of abovementioned theoretical and methodological discussion about SNS addiction, the aim of the present study was to investigate the validity and reliability of the BSMAS among social networking sites users. Among Facebook-only users and multisite-social networkers, SNS addiction and Facebook addiction do not necessarily reflect the same phenomenon. The present study aimed accordingly to compare the strength of the relationship between Facebook addiction and SNS addiction among Facebook-only users and multisite-social networkers, respectively. Previously, no study has investigated the psychometric properties of the BSMAS in comparison to the BFAS. Therefore, the present study is the first to compare the psychometric properties of the BSMAS in comparison to the BFAS in a sample of different SNSs users. In line with this, the present study is the first where a specific and a generic SNS addiction scale are compared. Previous empirical findings suggest that SNS addiction is related to specific personality risk factors and impairment of well-being and mental health. Accordingly, the aim of the present study was to examine the relationships between SNS addiction, personality traits, perceived stress and general subjective well-being. Last but not least, SNS addiction as a generic form might reflect addictive behavior in relation to several SNSs and is therefore expected to impair well-being and increase stress above and beyond addiction of (e.g. Facebook) specific sites. Consequently, the study aimed to identify the unique contribution of SNS addiction to stress and well-being beyond personality characteristics and Facebook addiction among multisite-social networkers.

Hypotheses

On the basis of previous research and theoretical frameworks, it was hypothesized that (i) the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale has good validity and reliability, and a single factor solution in a Polish sample (H1); (ii) the scores of the BSMAS and the BFMAS will correlate highly, although the relationship between the scores will be stronger in the group of Facebook-only users than in a group of multisite-social networkers (H2); (iii) emotional stability and conscientiousness are inversely related to SNS addiction (H3); (iv) SNS addiction is positively related to stress and inversely related to well-being (H4); (v) the relationships between Facebook addiction and criterion variables (gender, age, personality, stress and well-being) are the same as the relationships between SNS addiction and criterion variables in group of multisite-social networkers (H5); (vi) SNS addiction explains significantly proportions of variances in stress and well-being above and beyond age, gender, personality traits and Facebook addiction among multisite-social networkers (H6).

Methods

Sample

Initially, the sample comprised 1183 respondents. Before the analyses, data was screened and data from three participants who did not have accounts on any social networking sites were excluded. Due to missing data on relevant variables, 81 participants were further eliminated from the analyses. Thus, the final sample comprised 1099 participants. The sample was divided into two groups based on the number of accounts on social networks. Figure 1 presents the distribution of the number of accounts on social networking sites. There were 146 (13.3%) subjects who only had Facebook account (Facebook-only users) whereas 953 (86.7%) had accounts on Facebook and at least one additional SNS (multisite-social networkers). Multisite-social networker individuals reported using an average of 3.51 different SNS (SD = 1.30). In addition to Facebook, 738 (77.4%) of them had an Instagram account and 697 (73.1%) had a Snapchat account. The sample was diverse in terms of sociodemographic features (see Table 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Percentage distribution of the number of accounts on social networking sites

Table 1 Descriptive statistics of sociodemographic features in the sample

Instruments

Social Networking Sites Addiction

The Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS) is a modified version of the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale (Andreassen et al. 2012). The modification involves replacing the word “Facebook” with “social networking sites” the latter being defined as “Facebook, Twitter, Instagram and the like” in the instructions to participants (Andreassen et al. 2016). The Polish version of the scale was administered. The responses are provided on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from very rarely (1) to very often (5). The BSMAS has shown good validity and reliability in previous research (Andreassen et al. 2016; Andreassen et al. 2017; Bányai et al. 2017; Lin et al. 2017; Monacis et al. 2017). The scale has shown measurement invariance across two Chinese cultural areas (Leung et al. 2020), as well as time invariance (Chen et al. 2020). In the present study the Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient was .77. The Appendix provides a full list of the items in the Polish and English versions of the scale, respectively.

Facebook Addiction

The Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale (BFAS; Andreassen et al. 2012) includes six items that are based on the addiction components model (Griffiths 2005). Responses are provided on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from very rarely (1) to very often (5). The Polish version of the BFAS has shown good validity and reliability in previous studies (Atroszko et al. 2018; Charzyńska and Góźdź 2014). The scale has also shown good psychometric properties when adapted into other languages (Phanasathit et al. 2015; Pontes et al. 2016; Silva et al. 2018; Wang et al. 2015). In the present study the Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient of the BFAS was .81.

Personality

The Polish version (Atroszko 2015) of Ten Item Personality Inventory (TIPI; Gosling et al. 2003) was used to assess the five-factor model of personality: Extraversion, Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Emotional stability, and Openness to experience. Respondents provided answers on a 7-point Likert scale, ranging from disagree strongly (1) to agree strongly (7). The TIPI has shown good validity and reliability in previous studies (Atroszko 2015; Atroszko et al. 2016a, 2016b; Atroszko et al. 2018). In the present study the Spearman-Brown reliability coefficient was of .74 for Extraversion, .29 for Agreeableness, .72 for Conscientiousness, .62 for Emotional stability and .35 for Openness, respectively. The TIPI demonstrates good validity, still biased estimates of reliability using internal consistency measures which is expected due to the small number (just two) of items per dimension (Gosling et al. 2003). Therefore, less biased measures of reliability should be used, such as the test-retest reliability, which for the original scale yielded acceptable correlations between repeated measurements with 6-week interval, varying from .62 for Openness to .77 for Extroversion (Gosling et al. 2003).

Stress

Perceived stress was measured by the Polish short version of Perceived Stress Scale (PSS-4; Cohen et al. 1983). It consists of four items with a 5-point Likert response format scale, ranging from never (1) to very often (5). The scale has shown good validity and reliability in previous research (Atroszko 2015; Atroszko et al. 2015; Atroszko et al. 2018). The Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient was .75 in the present study.

Subjective Well-Being

The Polish version of the Ultra-Short Protocol for Measuring Subjective Well-Being (USP-SWB) was used to assess general subjective well-being. It consists of six items based on the WHOQOL-BREF (Skevington et al. 2004). The scale covers three (out of four) main domains distinguished by the WHO: physical (general health, sleep quality), psychological (satisfaction with life, meaning in life) and social (satisfaction with personal relationship, satisfaction with support received from friends). The responses are provided on a 9-point Likert scale ranging from (1) not at all to (9) an extreme amount in case of items regarding satisfaction with life and meaning in life. The response alternatives for the other items range from (1) very dissatisfied to (9) very satisfied. The scale has shown good validity and reliability in previous research (Atroszko et al. 2019). As this is a fairly new measure and since papers describing its psychometric properties have not been published yet, we decided to report the results of its structure using confirmatory factor analysis: The model with three first-order factors (physical, psychological, and social) and one second-order factor (general subjective well-being) showed good fit with the data: χ2(6) = 4.68, χ2/df = 0.78, CFI = 1.000, TLI = 1.001, RMSEA = .000, 90% CI [.000–.034]. In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha reliability coefficient of general subjective well-being was .75.

Procedure

Data were collected through both pen-and-pencil and an online questionnaire. Students from Polish universities: the University of Gdańsk, the Polish Naval Academy and the Gdańsk University of Technology were invited to participate anonymously during lectures or classes. The estimated response rate was above 95%. The online survey was conducted via a questionnaire placed on Facebook. Respondents were asked to click on a link to access the survey. The response rate for online surveys is impossible to determine (Fan and Yan 2010). Before starting to respond, the participants received detailed information about the study. Data collection occurred from November 2017 to March 2018. The final sample included 57.1% participants recruited via paper-and pencil survey and 42.9% via online survey. Participation was completely anonymous and no monetary or other material rewards were offered.

Statistical Analyses

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was performed to assess the construct validity of the BSMAS where a 6-item one-factor solution was tested. In order to investigate the validity of the BSMAS and the BFAS and to explore if SNS addiction and Facebook addiction do reflect the same phenomenon, a model with two latent variables (SNS addiction and Facebook addiction) was investigated where items corresponding to the same criteria were correlated. To indicate the differences between Facebook-only users and multisite-social networkers, the model was also investigated in the two samples. Robust Weighted Least Squares (WLSMV) estimator was used. The following measures were used to evaluate model fit: χ2 divided by degrees of freedom (χ2/df), the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) and the Root Mean Squared Error of Approximation (RMSEA). Cut-off scores for those indexes in terms of acceptable fit are: χ2/df ≤ 3, CFI ≥ 0.95, TLI ≥ 0.95, RMSEA ≤0.06 to 0.08 (Hu and Bentler 1999; Schreiber et al. 2006). Mplus 6.11 (Muthén and Muthén 1998–2010) was used to perform the CFA.

Means, standard deviations, percentages, and correlation coefficients were calculated for the entire sample. In the group of multisite-social networkers the correlations between SNS addiction and criteria variables were compared to correlations between Facebook addiction and the same variables. By comparing the correlations, it was possible to investigate whether the BSMAS and BFAS criterion validity was different. Two hierarchical regression analyses were conducted for the multisite-social networkers group where stress and subjective well-being comprised the dependent variables. An overview of the independent variables introduced in the subsequent steps can be found in Table 4. The proposed model allowed testing whether SNS addiction, regardless of other variables, is a source of decreased well-being and increased stress. For all linear regression analyses, preliminary analyses were conducted to ensure no violation of the assumptions of normality, linearity, multicollinearity, and homoscedasticity. To compare latent correlation coefficients between Facebook addiction and SNS addiction between the groups of Facebook-only users and multisite-social networkers a z test for independent groups was used. Furthermore, a z test for dependent groups was used to compare correlation coefficients between Facebook addiction and criterion variables and correlations between SNS addiction and the same criterial variables within the group of multisite-social networkers. All tests were two-tailed, and the alpha level was set to .05.

Ethics

The study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All gathered data was anonymous, and participants were informed about all the proper details about the study and their role in it, including that they could withdraw at any point. Attaining formal and written informed consent was not regarded as necessary as voluntary completion of the questionnaires was regarded as providing consent, the study was anonymous and since no medical information was gathered.

Results

The model with one factor of SNS addiction showed the following fit indices: χ2(9) = 251.64, χ2/df = 27.96, CFI = .924, TLI = .874, RMSEA = .157, 90% CI [.140–.174]. Standardized factor loadings of the items were: .66, .84, .66, .50, .73, .60, respectively. Due to lack of acceptable model fit, residuals of the first and second items were allowed to correlate on the basis of modification indices, similarly to previous studies concerning the BFAS (Atroszko et al. 2018; Charzyńska and Góźdź 2014) and the BSMAS (Monacis et al. 2017). Additionally, residuals of the fourth and sixth items were correlated on the basis of modification indices. The modified model had an acceptable fit: χ2(7) = 47.57, χ2/df = 6.80, CFI = .987, TLI = .973, RMSEA = .073, 90% CI [.054, .093]. Standardized factor loadings on items were: .48, .72, .71, .49, .80, .59, respectively. The correlation between residuals of the first and second item was .50, while the correlation between residuals of the fourth and sixth item was .23.

The model with two latent variables (Facebook addiction and Social Networking Sites addiction) and items corresponding to the same criteria correlated did not have an acceptable model fit in the group of Facebook-only users: χ2(47) = 211.06, χ2/df = 4.49, CFI = .916, TLI = .882, RMSEA = .155, 90% CI [.134, .176] and in the group of multisite-social networkers: χ2(47) = 577.11, χ2/df = 12.28, CFI = .951, TLI = .932, RMSEA = .109, 90% CI [.101, .117]. On the basis of modification indices residuals of the first and second item of the BSMAS, as well of the BFAS were allowed to correlate as well as the residuals of the fourth and sixth items of the BSMAS, as well in the BFAS. Further the residual of the fourth item of the BSMAS was correlated with the residual of the sixth item in the BFAS. Also, the residual of the sixth item of the BSMAS correlated with the residual of the fourth item on the BFAS. In the group of the Facebook-only users the model had a good fit: χ2(39) = 36.12, χ2/df = .93, CFI = 1.000, TLI = 1.003, RMSEA = .000, 90% CI [.000, .051]. In the group of the multisite-social networkers, the model also had a good fit: χ2(39) = 97.92, χ2/df = 2.51, CFI = .995, TLI = .991, RMSEA = .040, 90% CI [.030, .050]. Standardized factor loadings of the items can be found in Fig. 2. In the group of Facebook-only users the correlation between the BFAS and the BSMAS was .94. In the group of multisite-social networkers this relationship was .83. This difference was statistically significant (z = 6.12, p < .001).

Fig. 2
figure 2

The factor structure of the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS) and Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale (BFAS) for group of Facebook-only users (n = 146) and for multi-social networkers (n = 953). Standardized loadings are indicated on the arrows. All loadings are significant at p < .001. Results for multi-social networkers have been bolded

Table 2 presents mean scores, standard deviations, percentages, and correlation coefficients in the whole sample. In the group of the multisite-social networkers the correlations between SNS addiction and criterion variables were compared to correlations between Facebook addiction and criterion variables. A test z for dependent groups was used to compare these correlation coefficients (see Table 3). According to the polythetic approach (i.e., scoring 4 [often] or 5 [very often] on at least four out of six items), the percentage of people who meet the criteria of addiction was calculated showing that approximately 8.2% (95% CI = 6.7%– 9.9%) of young adults were identified as SNS addicts and 4.9% (95% CI = 3.8%– 6.3%) as Facebook addicts, respectively.

Table 2 Mean scores and standard deviations (SD), percentages, and correlation coefficients (Pearson product-moment/point-biserial) between study variables (n = 1099)
Table 3 Correlation coefficients (Pearson product-moment/point-biserial) between study variables for multisite-social networkers (n = 953)

Regression analysis for perceived stress (see Table 4) showed that the independent variables explained a total of 26.5% of the variance, F9,943 = 37.79, p < .001. Significant independent variables in Step 4 were extraversion (β = −.14, p < .001), conscientiousness (β = −.23, p < .001), emotional stability (β = −.31, p < .001) and SNS addiction (β = .14, p = .001).

Table 4 Results of hierarchical multiple regression analyses in which SNS addiction, Facebook addiction, age, gender and big five personality traits were regressed upon the scores on perceived stress and subjective well-being for multisite-social networkers (n = 953)

Regression analysis for general subjective well-being (see Table 4) showed that the independent variables explained a total of 26.5% of the variance, F9,943 = 37.73, p < .001. Significant independent variables in Step 4 were extraversion (β = .32, p < .001), conscientiousness (β = .23, p < .001), emotional stability (β = .19, p < .001) and SNS addiction (β = −.09, p = .029).

Discussion

Validity and Reliability of the BSMAS

The results showed that the model with one factor of SNS addiction had poor fit to the data. Previous studies suggest that such lack of acceptable fit might be related to the fact that measures based on the core components of addiction are additionally influenced by a general component of high time and energy investment into the particular behaviour, which manifests as correlations between items residuals (Atroszko et al. 2018; Atroszko et al. 2017). Thus, correlations between two previously reported pairs of items residuals (Item 1 and Item 2 as well as Item 4 and Item 6; see Atroszko et al. 2018; Bereznowski et al. 2016; Charzyńska and Góźdź 2014; Monacis et al. 2017) were allowed. The modified model showed acceptable fit with the data. All factor loadings were significant, their standardized values were above .40, and the scale had high internal consistency (H1 supported).

Relationship Between SNS Addiction and Facebook Addiction

The results showed that SNS addiction was strongly associated with Facebook addiction and that this relationship was stronger in the group of Facebook-only users than in the group of multisite-social networkers (H2 supported). These results support good convergent and discriminant validity of the BSMAS (H1 supported), and suggest that SNS addiction is completely equivalent to Facebook addiction among Facebook-only users (as Facebook is the only SNS these individuals use) alas not completely equivalent to Facebook addiction among multisite-social networkers (as Facebook is not the only SNS these individuals use). Therefore, the results support the notion that SNS addiction and Facebook addiction refer to the same underlying addictive process to social networking with somewhat different manifestations which could reflect level of addiction, stage of addiction or subtype of addiction. The construct of SNS addiction seems valid when researchers are interested in general aspects of SNS-functioning, which are relevant for all individuals addicted to SNSs. However, assessment of addiction to a specific SNS (e.g. Facebook) should be conducted when researchers are interested in unique aspects of functioning, which may differ between individuals addicted to specific SNSs (Andreassen and Pallesen 2013; Ryan et al. 2014; see also Throuvala et al. 2019). However, it must be kept in mind that the large majority of SNSs users are multisite-social networkers. Therefore, differentiating by SNSs use might not be the most cost-effective and informative strategy to collect data on addiction (Griffiths 2012; Kuss and Griffiths 2017). A more fruitful approach might be an investigation of SNS addiction along with individuals’ motives to use SNSs and activities in which individuals engage in on the SNSs (Andreassen and Pallesen 2013; Griffiths et al. 2014; Kuss and Griffiths 2017; Ryan et al. 2014; see also Throuvala et al. 2019).

SNS Addiction and Personality

Congruently with previous studies, the results showed that SNS addiction was inversely associated with conscientiousness and emotional stability (H3 supported; De Cock et al. 2014; Marino et al. 2018a). Additionally, SNS addiction was positively associated with extraversion but unrelated to agreeableness and openness to experience. It should be noted, that an in-depth investigation of these relationships requires application of multivariate analyses rather than the analysis of zero-order correlations. For example, currently the data suggests that while zero-order correlations between Facebook addiction and extraversion are either non-significant or negative (Marino et al. 2018a), the more complementary regression models controlling for wide range of personality factors show a positive relationship between them (see Andreassen et al. 2013; Atroszko et al. 2018), which could be potentially explained by the shared covariance between extraversion, neuroticism and Facebook addiction. However, future studies should investigate potential moderating effects of other variables (e.g., culture or patterns of use of SNSs [e.g., active vs. passive]) on the relationships between SNS addiction and personality traits (see also Marino et al. 2018a).

SNS Addiction, Facebook Addiction, and their Relationships with Criterion Variables in Group of Multisite-Social Networkers

SNS addiction and Facebook addiction differed in strengths of their relationships with gender and age (in both cases the relationship with SNS addiction was stronger). On the other hand, there were no differences in strengths of their relationships with personality traits, perceived stress, and general subjective well-being. However, it is worth noting that although the differences were not statistically significant, the relationships with SNS addiction were stronger than the relationships with Facebook addiction in cases of almost all criterion variables. These results show there were some minor differences in strengths of relationships of SNS addiction and Facebook addiction with the criteria variables; however, the differences were by and large marginal (H5 supported). Consequently, it suggests that, with adequate caution, studies of correlates of SNS addiction and Facebook addiction can be perceived as complementary (e.g. studies on correlates of Facebook addiction seem to converge with studies on correlates of SNS addiction and vice versa). Therefore, future studies on correlates of SNS addiction and Facebook addiction may be assumed to produce similar results, indicating the presence of generic antecedents and consequences. It should however be noted that this assumption might be limited to the present study’s variables and sample.

SNS Addiction, Facebook Addiction, Perceived Stress, and General Subjective Well-Being

The results showed that SNS addiction was positively associated with perceived stress and negatively associated with general subjective well-being even after controlling for Facebook addiction and other studied variables, which is congruent with previous studies (H4 and H6 substantiated; Hou et al. 2017; Marino et al. 2018b; Pontes 2017; Tromholt 2016; Turel et al. 2018; Xue et al. 2018). Facebook addiction showed a similar relationship with these variables before controlling for SNS addiction. These results suggest that there is a disproportion in the unique contribution of SNS addiction and Facebook addiction to the impaired functioning of individuals. Possibly, such disproportion is a result of differences in the specificity between SNS addiction and Facebook addiction (see Griffiths 2012). The former is a broader construct, which overlaps with the latter as well as include other addictions to particular SNSs (e.g. Instagram, Snapchat, Twitter; Kuss and Griffiths 2017). As a result, all of the impairment related to Facebook addiction are also present in SNS addiction, while SNS addiction seems to be associated with additional impairment. Hence, these results complement a picture of the relationship between SNS addiction and Facebook addiction.

Strengths and Limitations

To the best of our knowledge this is the first empirical study investigating the relationship between SNS addiction and Facebook addiction, being a subject of heated theoretical discussion during the recent years (see Andreassen and Pallesen 2013; Griffiths 2012; Griffiths et al. 2014; Kuss and Griffiths 2017). Moreover, the present study also sheds light on methodological and theoretical challenges related to the high number of multisite-social networkers among users of SNSs. It comprised a relatively large sample size (providing high statistical power) as well as use of valid and reliable psychometric tools. Consequently, it significantly adds to the existing literature on behavioral addictions and provide valuable insights into the nature of the relationship between SNS addiction and Facebook addiction.

In terms of limitations, the sample size of Facebook-only users was relatively small (although quite well representing proportions in population of SNSs users; GlobalWebIndex 2017) which reduced statistical power regarding the comparison between Facebook-only users and multisite-social networkers. Moreover, all data were self-reported and a cross-sectional design was used, rendering the data vulnerable to limitations associated with such data (e.g., common method, social desirability and recall biases). The sample was not representative of the general population of SNS users and mainly comprised students, which puts restrictions on the generalizability to other populations.

Conclusions and Future Studies Directions

The present study showed that SNS addiction can be validly and reliably measured among young adults in Poland. What is more, it provided empirical data about the relationship between SNS addiction and Facebook addiction, which appear to be reflecting common addictive process to social networking with somewhat specific manifestations. Nevertheless, SNS addiction seems to be a more suitable construct to study in most cases, firstly because the large majority of SNSs users are multisite-social networkers (GlobalWebIndex 2017). Thus, in many cases it is difficult to apply methodological and statistical procedures which would allow to study differences between addictions to particular SNSs (and in some cases it might not be even possible). Secondly, a more generic assessment approach seems preferable because other variables related to usage of SNSs (e.g. motives to use SNSs, activities in which users engage on SNSs, patterns of use of SNSs [e.g. active vs. passive]) seems to be more important to identify than to study correlates of particular SNS (Andreassen and Pallesen 2013; Griffiths et al. 2014; Kuss and Griffiths 2017; Ryan et al. 2014; see also Throuvala et al. 2019). Thirdly, a more generic assessment in terms of predictors and consequences seems to correspond well to the findings obtained when assessing addiction to more specific SNS. However, Facebook addiction and other addictions to particular SNSs might still be useful constructs, for example they might be used to determine which of addictions to particular SNS are the biggest contributors to the individuals’ harm (e.g. deteriorated well-being) or which of addictions to particular SNS are the biggest contributors to general SNS addiction. Consequently, such studies could lead to successful therapeutic interventions focusing on the most harmful SNSs.

Future studies should further investigate the relationship between SNS addiction and Facebook addiction (e.g. measurement invariance between BSMAS and BFAS, as well as multi-group analysis to compare the group of Facebook-only users and the group of multisite-social networkers). Such investigations should consider using purposive sampling in order to maximize the number of Facebook-only users participating, and thus the relevant statistical power. Future studies should also investigate the relationships between SNS addiction and other addictions to particular SNSs (e.g. Instagram, Snapchat, Twitter) as well as the relationships between different addictions to particular SNSs (e.g. Facebook vs. Instagram). Moreover, we suggest implementing question about SNSs used by participants as a standard procedure in future studies as it appears that differentiation between multisite-social networkers and users of a single SNS (e.g. Facebook-only users) might be essential to facilitate advancement in this field. Future studies should also investigate the potential role of motives to use SNSs, activities in which users engage on SNSs, and patterns of use of SNSs (e.g. active vs. passive) in terms of SNS addiction. Last but not least, future studies should also include further investigation of addictive properties of SNS addictions as well as harm and distress caused by specific and generic SNS addictions.