Skip to main content
Log in

Self-construal and communalism in Costa Rica: Subjective attitudes vs. implicit behavioral tendencies

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Early work by Hofstede (Behavior Science Research, 18 (4), 285–305, 1983) described Costa Rica as among the most culturally collective of 52 countries studied. Later work described the people of Costa Rica as low in group orientation, an outlier compared to other Latin American populations (Oyserman et al. Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 3–72, 2002). To examine this inconsistency, the current study assessed 69 Costa Rican university students’ responses on two well-known explicit attitude measures and one scenario measure of implicit tendencies (Kitayama et al. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(2), 236–255, 2009) related to group orientation. Responses were compared to those of North American participants who were of European (40) and African (60) heritage. Reliability estimates varied among the measures and in the sample groups. Costa Ricans did not differ from U.S. participants in interdependent but scored higher than both US groups in independent self-construal. Their communalism scores were lower. On the measure of implicit tendencies, however, they rated themselves more similar to communal than individualistic and competitive peers, and less similar to the individualistic peer than did either group of U.S. participants. These findings complicate the question of whether Costa Rican group orientation is at odds with other Latin American populations and raise important general questions about cross group measurement of culture.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Data Availability

The data generated and analyzed for this report are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Notes

  1. Historians document the first wave of mestizaje to have occurred 1777 and 1778, when Costa Rica’s ethnic composition was documented as 60% mestizo, between 18% black and mulatto, 12% Indian, and 10% Hispanic (Pérez 1997). The second wave of mestizaje occurred during the late 17th and 18th centuries as more Africans were brought from countries including Congo, Angola, Nigeria, Benin, and Ghana to present day Costa Rica (Marín González 2001). In 1867 there was also a large migration of Afro-Jamaicans to Costa Rica, even before the large wave of immigration of Europeans in the twentieth century (Rosabal-Coto 2008). Despite the history, at present, the Costa Rican census reports population percentages of 84% White/Mestizo, 6.7% Mulato, 2.4% Indigenous, 1.1% African descent and other ethnic groups (Central Intelligence Agency 2011).

  2. Informed consent was obtained from all participants included in this study and all procedures were approved by the IRB of the corresponding author.

  3. Power analysis using the G*power program (Faul et al. 2007) indicated that a total sample of 111 people would be needed to detect effects as small as (d = .20) with Power (1-ß err prob) = .95, using repeated measures ANOVA with alpha set at .05.

  4. All three of our sample groups achieved interdependence subscale scores that were nearly identical to the highest interdependence sub-scale means (Asian Americans) reported by the scale’s authors (Singelis, et al., 1994). However, means for our Costa Rican participants were nearly half a standard deviation higher than the highest independence sub-scale means reported in that work (Caucasian Americans). A simple ratio conversion was used to account for scaling differences across studies, however for these archival comparisons, we did not account for differences between to forced choice vs. the availability of a neutral selection option.

  5. Means were adjusted for this comparison via a simple ratio calculation to account for different scaling employed in the studies.

  6. As evidenced by achieving a place in Euro western style higher education, whose modus operandi (and admissions criteria) tends to privilege those things (Hurley and Hurley 2011).

  7. Anecdotally, the authors have observed this among some of their immigrant students, who arrive believing they are practically “American” but after some time among Americans realize how much their home culture informs their positioning on these and other dimesons of culture.

  8. Researcher are still free to quibble over whether and to what degree the described behaviors do constitute communalism, but respondents’ status relative to those behaviors at least, will be unambiguous.

References

  • Boykin, A. W., Jagers, R. J., Ellison, C. M., & Albury, A. (1997). Communalism: Conceptualization and measurement of an Afrocultural social orientation. Journal of Black Studies, 27, 409–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boykin, A. W., Tyler, K. M., & Miller, O. (2005). In search of cultural themes and their expressions in the dynamics of classroom life. Urban Education, 40(5), 521–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Central Intelligence Agency. (2011). Costa Rica. In The world factbook. Retrieved December 10, 2018, from https://www.cia.gov/library/publications/the-world-factbook/geos/br.html.

  • Christopher, M. S., Norris, P., D'Souza, J. B., & Tiernan, K. A. (2012). A test of the multidimensionality of the self-construal scale in Thailand and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(5), 758–773.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, S. E., Hardin, E. E., & Gercek-Swing, B. (2011). The what, how, why, and where of self-construal. Personality & Social Psychology Review, 15(2), 142–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Delgado-Gaitán, C. (1994). Socializing young children in Mexican-American families: An intergenerational perspective. In P. Greenfield & R. Cocking (Eds.), Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 55–86). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernández Guardia, P. (2005). Cartilla Histórica de Costa Rica [Historical Primer for Costa Rica]. San José: Editorial Universidad de Costa Rica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofstede, G. (1983). National cultures revisited. Behavior Science Research, 18(4), 285–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, S. P., & Hurley, E. A. (2011). Self-construal, culture and diversity in higher education. In D. Mashek & E. Hammer (Eds.), Empirical research in teaching and learning: Contributions from social psychology (pp. 51–72). NJ: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, E. A., Leath, S. C., Hurley, S. P., & Pauletto, E. (2018) Cultural group membership vs. (self reported) group orientation: Which predicts student performance in cooperative & competitive math learning? For whom? Urban education. Presented at the American Psychological Association Annual Convention, San Francisco, Ca (in press).

  • Keller, H., Borke, J., Yovsi, R., Lohaus, A., & Jensen, H. (2005). Cultural orientations and historical changes as predictors of parenting behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 29(3), 229–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., & Imada, T. (2010). Implicit independence and interdependence: A cultural task analysis. In B. Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, E. R. Smith, B. Mesquita, L. F. Barrett, & E. R. Smith (Eds.), The mind in context (pp. 174–200). NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K. (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(2), 236–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lobo, T., & Meléndez, M. (1997). Negros y Blancos, Todo Mezclado [black and white, all mixed]. San José: Editorial Universidad de Costa Rica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marín González, R. (2001). Redescrubriendo la historia. [Rediscovering history] Semanario Universidad, Suplemento Crisol. No. 1447, p. 3.

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marryshow, D., Hurley, E. A., Allen, B. A., Tyler, K. M., & Boykin, A. W. (2005). The impact of learning orientation on African American children’s attitudes toward high achieving peers. The American Journal of Psychology, 118(4), 603–618.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Matsumoto, D., & Juang, L. (2016). Culture and psychology., 6th ed. Belmont: Wadsworth/Thomson Learning.

  • Oishi, S. (2000). Goals as cornerstones of subjective well-being: Linking individuals with cultures. In E. F. Diener & E. M. Suh (Eds.), Subjective well-being across cultures (pp. 87–112). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128(1), 3–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pérez, B. H. (1997). Breve historia contemporánea de Costa Rica. [Brief contemporary history of Costa Rica]. México: Fondo de Cultura Económica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosabal-Coto, M. (2008). Parental belief systems, conflict resolution strategies, and cultural orientation in the mother-child interactive context: A comparative study of two Costa Rican samples. Saarbrücken: VDM Verlag Dr. Müller.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sankofa, B. M., Hurley, E. A., Allen, B. A., & Boykin, A. W. (2005). Cultural expression and black students' attitudes toward high achievers. The Journal of Psychology, 139(3), 247–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, S. J., Weisskirch, R. S., Hurley, E. A., Zamboanga, B. L., Park, I. J. K., Kim, S. Y., Umana-Taylor, A., Castillo, L. G., Brown, E., & Greene, A. D. (2010). Communalism, familism, and filial piety: Are they birds of a collectivist feather? Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16(4), 548–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwarz, N., & Oyserman, D. (2011). Asking questions about behavior: Self-reports in evaluation research. In M. M. Mark, S. I. Donaldson, B. Campbell, M. M. Mark, S. I. Donaldson, & B. Campbell (Eds.), Social psychology and evaluation (pp. 243–264). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 580–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephan, W. G., Stephan, C. W., & de Vargas, M. C. (1996). Emotional expression in Costa Rica and the United States. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 27(2), 147–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toasije, A. (2009). The africanity of Spain: Identity and problematization. Journal of Black Studies, 39(3), 348–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H. (1983). Allocentric vs. Idiocentric social behavior: A major cultural difference between Hispanics and the Mainstream. Technical Report No. ONR-16. University of Illinois - Champaign: Department of Psychology, University of Illinois. USA.

  • Triandis, H., Bontempo, R., Betancourt, H., Bond, M., Leung, K., Brenes, A., Georgas, J., Hui, H., Marín, G., Setiadi, B., Sinha, B. P., Verma, J., Spangenberg, J., Touzard, H., & Montmollin, G. (1986). The measurement of the ethic aspects of individualism and collectivism across cultures. Australian Journal of Psychology, 38(3), 257–267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Triandis, H., Bontempo, R., Villareal, M., Asai, M., & Lucca, N. (1988). Individualism and collectivism: Cross-cultural perspectives on self-in-group relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology., 54(2), 323–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vignoles, V. L., Owe, E., Becker, M., Smith, P. B., Easterbrook, M. J., Brown, R., González, R., Didier, N., Carrasco, D., Cadena, M. P., Lay, S., Schwartz, S. J., Des Rosiers, S. E., Villamar, J. A., Gavreliuc, A., Zinkeng, M., Kreuzbauer, R., Baguma, P., Martin, M., Tatarko, A., Herman, G., de Sauvage, I., Courtois, M., Garðarsdóttir, R. B., Harb, C., Schweiger Gallo, I., Prieto Gil, P., Lorente Clemares, R., Campara, G., Nizharadze, G., Macapagal, M. E. J., Jalal, B., Bourguignon, D., Zhang, J., Lv, S., Chybicka, A., Yuki, M., Zhang, X., Espinosa, A., Valk, A., Abuhamdeh, S., Amponsah, B., Özgen, E., Güner, E. Ü., Yamakoğlu, N., Chobthamkit, P., Pyszczynski, T., Kesebir, P., Vargas Trujillo, E., Balanta, P., Cendales Ayala, B., Koller, S. H., Jaafar, J. L., Gausel, N., Fischer, R., Milfont, T. L., Kusdil, E., Çağlar, S., Aldhafri, S., Ferreira, M. C., Mekonnen, K. H., Wang, Q., Fülöp, M., Torres, A., Camino, L., Lemos, F. C. S., Fritsche, I., Möller, B., Regalia, C., Manzi, C., Brambilla, M., & Bond, M. H. (2016). Beyond the ‘east–west’ dichotomy: Global variation in cultural models of selfhood. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 966–1000.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, B. C., & Constantine, M. G. (2005). Afrocentric cultural values, psychological help-seeking attitudes, and self-concealment in African American college students. Journal of Black Psychology, 31, 369–385.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Eric A. Hurley.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that there is no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hurley, E.A., Salvador, C.E. & English, K.A. Self-construal and communalism in Costa Rica: Subjective attitudes vs. implicit behavioral tendencies. Curr Psychol 40, 1711–1719 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0096-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-0096-0

Keywords

Navigation